01.05.2015 Views

o_19k6fnuqsqg41mt7rqp1c8pkueo.pdf

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

110<br />

F. R. Balteş, M. Miclea, A. C. Miu<br />

Bartlett, & Dowling, 1995). However, it is unclear whether music-induced affect is<br />

similar in musicians and non-musicians.<br />

Comparisons between musicians and non-musicians suggested that<br />

aesthetic judgments are also grounded on a common conceptual content that may be<br />

somewhat modified by musical expertise (Ystok et al., 2009). Using a verbal<br />

association task, it was shown that non-musicians generated more adjectives<br />

connected to the mood or emotional balance related to music, whereas musicians<br />

appreciated the inciting features of music, its novelty and originality (Ystok et al.,<br />

2009). The emotional categorization of short musical stimuli was equally reliable in<br />

musicians and non-musicians (Bigand et al., 2005). Using positron emission<br />

tomography, Blood and Zatorre (2001) found that the intensity of music-induced<br />

chills is associated with increased cerebral blood flow in reward areas (e.g., ventral<br />

striatum, midbrain, amygdala, orbitofrontal and ventral medial prefrontal cortex).<br />

This study included only musicians, on the assumption that this population is more<br />

likely to experience strong emotional responses to music, although the authors<br />

acknowledged that music training is not necessary to experience these responses<br />

(Blood & Zatorre, 2001). Clearly, further studies are required in order to clarify<br />

whether musical training impacts music-induced emotions.<br />

The objectives of the present study were: (1) to explore the affective space<br />

of Vivaldi’s Four Seasons; and (2) to compare the emotional arousal and valence of<br />

the Four Seasons between musicians and non-musicians. Although Vivaldi’s<br />

masterpiece is widely known, we expected differences in familiarity between<br />

musicians and non-musicians, and consequently we controlled for this variable in<br />

the comparisons of emotional responses. We hypothesized that the movements of<br />

Vivaldi’s Four Seasons would cover the entire affective space (i.e., positive and<br />

negative valence with varying degrees of emotional arousal), considering the<br />

composer’s intention to suggest the features of different seasons in his music.<br />

Another hypothesis was that, although we controlled for differences in familiarity<br />

with this particular composition, musicians would perceive the Four Seasons as less<br />

emotionally activating and pleasant than non-musicians, due to their knowledge of<br />

baroque music style.<br />

MATERIALS AND METHODS<br />

Participants<br />

N = 16 musicians and N = 14 non-musicians with ages from 22 to 40 years (mean<br />

age = 28 years), with good hearing abilities and no neurological disease records,<br />

have taken part in this experiment. The musicians group included instrument<br />

players and choir members from the National Opera House from Cluj-Napoca,<br />

Romania. The musicians benefited of an average of ten years of musical education<br />

and they were actively involved in musical activity at the time of this study. The<br />

non-musicians were recruited from the population of students in psychology from<br />

Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal<br />

16 (2012) 107-119

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!