11.07.2015 Views

Entry Testing and the Overrepresentation of Romani ... - UR Research

Entry Testing and the Overrepresentation of Romani ... - UR Research

Entry Testing and the Overrepresentation of Romani ... - UR Research

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS
  • No tags were found...

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

R26PITFALLS AND BIAS2008, 136). Ardila (2005) describes ways that testing procedures <strong>and</strong> conditions might conflict with children’shome communities. For example, tests are administered through a highly regimented script with specific instructions<strong>and</strong> formal language which might be difficult for some children who might not have had access to educationalsettings where formal instructions are usually given. For collective cultures, dem<strong>and</strong>s for individual responses orrequests to “do your best” might not be as valued as in <strong>the</strong> dominant culture. In addition, certain tasks that arepart <strong>of</strong> intelligence tests, such as figure drawing or digit repetition might seem purposeless <strong>and</strong> unimportant. Some<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> items on intelligence tests are timed or depend on a child’s processing “speed.” Time can be considereda social construct, <strong>and</strong> for some cultural groups, tests that are timed or depend on speed are inappropriate, as itmight take a slow <strong>and</strong> deliberative process to carefully work through test items <strong>and</strong> produce quality work.Individuals who administer <strong>the</strong> tests are usually strangers to those to whom <strong>the</strong>y give <strong>the</strong> test, which also mightimpact a child’s performance on <strong>the</strong> test. Moreover, for some children it might be strange for an adult to askquestions to which <strong>the</strong>y (that is, as <strong>the</strong> adults) should already know <strong>the</strong> answer. Therefore, it is essential thatassessments consider <strong>the</strong> language <strong>and</strong> cultural characteristics <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> children being tested both during testadministration <strong>and</strong> when interpreting <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> test (Center on <strong>the</strong> Developing Child 2007, 22).Dynamic testing is a strategy that might be useful to consider when administering intelligence or ability teststo children from minority communities. Whereas st<strong>and</strong>ard assessment practices focus on stasis, evaluatingpreexisting, pre-learned skills, dynamic assessment emphasizes measuring <strong>the</strong> processes involved in learning <strong>and</strong>change (Sternberg et al. 2002, 143). In a study <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> assessment <strong>of</strong> intellectual potential in rural Tanzanianschool children, Sternberg <strong>and</strong> colleagues (2002) sought to determine if short intervention teaching <strong>of</strong> cognitiveskills <strong>and</strong> strategies would have any effect on posttest scores, in essence, to assess developing abilities. All childrenwere given a pretest, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> experimental group <strong>of</strong> children received short intervention teaching sessionswhile <strong>the</strong> control group received no such interventions. All children were administered a posttest. The posttestscores <strong>of</strong> children who received short intervention teaching increased compared with children in <strong>the</strong> controlgroup. Dynamic testing by its nature contributes to a less neutral relationship between <strong>the</strong> examiner <strong>and</strong> examineethan in traditional, static testing situations. The dynamic testing procedure seems to tap into “important abilitiesthat would not have been measured were one only to have considered <strong>the</strong> pretest (static) scores” (Sternberg etal. 2002, 158). In discussing differences between static (traditional) <strong>and</strong> dynamic testing, Sternberg <strong>and</strong> colleaguesnote that <strong>the</strong> intervention teaching sessions were short, lasting less than an hour. In dynamic testing, childrenare provided feedback until <strong>the</strong>y ei<strong>the</strong>r solve a problem or give up. In this way, testing “joins with instruction, <strong>and</strong><strong>the</strong> test-taker’s ability to learn is quantified while he or she learns” (Sternberg et al. 2002, 143). Through thismethod <strong>of</strong> testing, children are able to show <strong>the</strong>ir knowledge through a brief intervention whereby children learn<strong>the</strong> nuances <strong>of</strong> test-taking, something <strong>the</strong>y might not have had exposure to in <strong>the</strong> past.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!