24.04.2013 Views

Intervention for Dyslexia - The British Dyslexia Association

Intervention for Dyslexia - The British Dyslexia Association

Intervention for Dyslexia - The British Dyslexia Association

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

dyslexia (and those studies were all very small), all the studies summarised above<br />

targeted children with serious reading difficulties, either children in the below average<br />

range (standard scores 78–85), who were not yet functionally literate, and/or children in<br />

the very low literacy range (standard scores below 78), who were complete non-readers<br />

or perhaps semi-literate. In most cases, the participants were on the SEN register at<br />

either the School Action or School Action Plus stage. It is highly likely that a substantial<br />

proportion of such children were dyslexic.<br />

<strong>The</strong> results reported from studies in UK specialist schools and teaching centres (Section<br />

3.4) did not produce such impressive results. <strong>The</strong> ratio gains <strong>for</strong> the large samples of<br />

dyslexic pupils at East Court School were only in the range 0.86–2.0, those studied by<br />

Hornsby and Miles (1980) produced ratio gains of 1.9, and in the <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Institute<br />

studies the ratio gains were around 1.0, although effect sizes were moderate. Brooks<br />

(2007) has described ratio gains of between 1.4 and 2.0 as having ‘small impact’ and<br />

being ‘of modest educational significance’; ratio gains less than 1.4 he classes as being<br />

of ‘very small impact’ and ‘of doubtful educational significance’. On this basis all the<br />

results reported from studies in UK specialist schools and teaching centres would be<br />

regarded as disappointing (or even disregarded altogether), since the largest ratio gain<br />

was only 2.0 (except at Moon Hall School – see section 3.3). However, it is well<br />

established that dyslexic pupils who do not receive intervention generally decline<br />

steadily in literacy relative to their peers. Without help, dyslexic pupils have been found<br />

to progress at around only 5 months per calendar year in reading (ratio gain 0.42) and 3<br />

months in spelling (ratio gain 0.25) (Thomson, 1990, 2001; see also Rack and Walker,<br />

1994). Arguably, the achievement of ratio gains of 1.0 or greater represents substantial<br />

progress <strong>for</strong> these individuals. <strong>The</strong>ir progress has been significantly accelerated,<br />

although in many cases further ef<strong>for</strong>ts will be necessary <strong>for</strong> them to catch up with their<br />

peers. Furthermore, the pupils being served by these interventions are older (average<br />

age around 10 years of age) and typically bear the scars of many years of cumulative<br />

frustration and failure. Such children can be notoriously ‘hard to teach’ (Rack; 2004;<br />

Thomson, 1990; Torgesen, 2005b) and almost invariably require very intensive<br />

intervention. Arguably, a greater intensity of intervention than af<strong>for</strong>ded by the two<br />

sessions per week typically provided by <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Action would produce better results.<br />

Despite the ratio gain results, real achievements should not be overlooked: at East Court<br />

School, <strong>for</strong> example, the average per<strong>for</strong>mance in reading accuracy and comprehension<br />

by pupils when leaving the school was well within the normal range. Only spelling and<br />

reading fluency – although both much improved – remained below the normal range<br />

(which is a common finding in almost all interventions with older, more severe<br />

dyslexics). But without this specialist input it is likely that the poor rate of progress that<br />

they displayed be<strong>for</strong>e the intervention would have continued so that they would<br />

otherwise have been considerably further behind in literacy.<br />

Overall, there<strong>for</strong>e, it is argued that these findings confirm the conclusions drawn at the<br />

end of Chapter 2, namely that the literacy skills of children with dyslexia can be<br />

substantially improved by use of systematic phonologically-based intervention<br />

approaches, and that this often succeeds in bringing these children’s reading accuracy<br />

and reading comprehension to within average levels, with reading fluency and spelling<br />

(although both much improved) generally remaining somewhat lower. <strong>The</strong> evidence also<br />

indicates that intervention provided in small groups can be just as effective as that<br />

delivered to children individually, a conclusion with which Fawcett (2002a) concurred in<br />

her review. However, if children also have additional problems, e.g. poor vocabulary<br />

skills, then their response to intervention is likely to be less satisfactory, and they will<br />

74 <strong>Intervention</strong> <strong>for</strong> <strong>Dyslexia</strong>

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!