24.04.2013 Views

Intervention for Dyslexia - The British Dyslexia Association

Intervention for Dyslexia - The British Dyslexia Association

Intervention for Dyslexia - The British Dyslexia Association

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Unlike traditional learning disabilities assessment that assesses students at one point in<br />

time using ability, achievement, and processing measures, the RTI approach repeatedly<br />

assesses the student’s progress using curriculum-based measurement (Fuchs & Fuchs,<br />

1997, 1998). Special education is considered only if a child’s per<strong>for</strong>mance shows a dual<br />

discrepancy in which per<strong>for</strong>mance is below the level of classroom peers and the<br />

student’s learning rate (growth) is also substantially below that of classroom peers<br />

(Gresham, 2002). <strong>The</strong> intervention itself also functions as the test (Fuchs & Fuchs,<br />

2006), although reading tests can be part of the evaluation (Fuchs, Fuchs et al., 2002).<br />

Assessment of reading-related cognitive abilities (including language-based abilities) has<br />

no place in mainstream RTI theory and practice (Fuchs, Compton, Fuchs, Bryant &<br />

Davis, 2008). However, in the face of strong research evidence (see Section 4.1.2), a<br />

few researchers and practitioners working within the RTI framework have recently<br />

begun to advocate use of screening batteries that include measures of phonological<br />

skills, orthographic knowledge, letter knowledge, vocabulary and syntactic ability (e.g.<br />

Davis, Lindo & Compton, 2007).<br />

Vellutino, Scanlon and Jaccard (2003), in a follow-up to their earlier study (Vellutino et<br />

al., 1996), confirmed their previous findings and showed that RTI can help determine<br />

whether a child’s reading difficulties are caused primarily by basic, neurodevelopmental<br />

deficits (e.g. dyslexia) or by experiential factors such as limitations in early reading<br />

experience and/or early reading instruction. <strong>The</strong> children who have the lowest RTI are<br />

most likely to be in the <strong>for</strong>mer category. Some evidence in support of the RTI<br />

approach comes from a study by Speece and Case (2001), in which students<br />

identified as non-responsive in an RTI model were found to be more deficient on<br />

measures of phonological processing, academic competence, and social skills than<br />

those students identified as learning disabled through the traditional discrepancy<br />

model diagnostic approach. Note, however, that, unlike many advocates of RTI,<br />

Vellutino, Scanlon and Jaccard (2003) do not assert that RTI should be the sole metric<br />

on which identification of a neurodevelopmental learning difficulty should be made.<br />

Rather, they conclude that the most confident basis <strong>for</strong> decisions on causality and<br />

prognosis can be made using RTI together with assessment of reading-related cognitive<br />

skills.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re remains the tricky issue of whether RTI is a legitimate basis <strong>for</strong> deciding whether<br />

or not the child has a learning disability. Does the fact that a child responded adequately<br />

to an intervention rule out the possibility that they have a disability? Does adequate RTI<br />

inevitably mean that the child’s learning difficulties were caused exclusively by poor<br />

instruction? Does the fact that the child did not respond adequately to an intervention<br />

mean that he or she has a neurodevelopmental learning difficulty? Advocates of RTI<br />

sometimes assert that if child’s learning difficulties have been remediated or normalized,<br />

then the issue of whether or not that child had a learning disability in the first place is of<br />

little consequence. However, by focusing almost exclusively on reading achievement<br />

rather than the component skills involved in reading and associated difficulties, the RTI<br />

approach appears to have confounded the category ‘reading difficulties’ with the<br />

category ‘specific learning difficulties’ (SpLD). Although children with SpLD typically do<br />

have poor reading achievement, they often have other problems as well (such as<br />

problems with arithmetic), and even when their reading skills have been normalised,<br />

many other problems are likely to remain (see Torgesen, 2005b). In the RTI approach, a<br />

low-per<strong>for</strong>ming child who shows growth rates similar to that of peers in a lowper<strong>for</strong>ming<br />

classroom would not be considered a candidate <strong>for</strong> intervention because the<br />

child is regarded as deriving similar educational benefits (although somewhat lower)<br />

from that classroom. Torgesen, Foorman and Wagner (2007) also point out that RTI has<br />

<strong>Intervention</strong> <strong>for</strong> <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 79

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!