24.04.2013 Views

Intervention for Dyslexia - The British Dyslexia Association

Intervention for Dyslexia - The British Dyslexia Association

Intervention for Dyslexia - The British Dyslexia Association

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Peer, 1998). A high overall level of accuracy was obtained in discriminant function<br />

analysis, with false negative and false positive rates below 5%. In CoPS, dyslexic profiles<br />

are typically those with relatively low scores on the subtests assessing phonological<br />

awareness, verbal sequential memory and processing speed, although very young<br />

children may also display difficulties on the Phoneme discrimination subtest. Because<br />

CoPS provides norms <strong>for</strong> time as well as accuracy, it is possible to distinguish between<br />

children whose overall rate of working is slow and those whose rate of working is slow in<br />

certain areas but not others. Strengths (e.g. in visual memory) may be uncovered,<br />

which can be used in teaching (see Singleton, 2002).<br />

Table 11. Subtests in Lucid CoPS and LASS<br />

Lucid CoPS<br />

(ages 4–8 years)<br />

LASS<br />

(ages 7–11 and 11–15 years)<br />

Visual spatial memory Visual spatial memory<br />

Symbolic memory Verbal memory<br />

Processing speed Phonological awareness<br />

Visual sequential memory Phonological decoding<br />

Associative memory Single word reading<br />

Verbal sequential memory Sentence reading<br />

Phonological awareness Spelling<br />

Phoneme discrimination Non-verbal reasoning<br />

Marks and Burden (2005) studied pupils in Devon schools who had been administered a<br />

prototype of the CoPS program at age 5 as part of trials being carried out by the <strong>British</strong><br />

<strong>Dyslexia</strong> <strong>Association</strong> in 1996. (<strong>The</strong> version of CoPS used in this study was superseded by<br />

the definitive Windows version of the program that underwent national standardisation<br />

with over 2,500 children in 1997.) Sixty-six pupils were followed up, with correlations of<br />

0.4–0.5 being found between CoPS test scores and results of National Curriculum<br />

assessments in reading, writing and spelling given at age 7, although the authors<br />

acknowledge the “questionable reliability” of National Curriculum assessment results.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se findings may be compared with correlations of around 0.6 obtained between<br />

CoPS test scores at age 5 and standardised literacy tests at age 9, in the larger study<br />

reported by Singleton, Thomas and Horne (2000). It should be pointed out that, in the<br />

Devon trials, the children’s CoPS results were available to the teachers and follow-up<br />

tuition was encouraged, with training provided <strong>for</strong> teachers in supporting dyslexic<br />

children. This would have been expected to make a difference to the outcomes <strong>for</strong> the<br />

pupils assessed and affect the predictive accuracy of the tests. In the Singleton et al.<br />

(2000) study, the children’s CoPS results were not made available to the teachers.<br />

Marks and Burden noted that the CoPS tests of visual spatial memory, symbolic memory,<br />

processing speed and phonological awareness also correlated significantly with National<br />

Curriculum assessments per<strong>for</strong>mance in maths, a finding replicated in a study by<br />

Simmons, Singleton and Horne (2008). Marks and Burden suggest that the predictive<br />

validity of CoPS tests may derive from a common factor, such as intelligence, although<br />

<strong>Intervention</strong> <strong>for</strong> <strong>Dyslexia</strong> 91

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!