05.07.2013 Views

linguistic structures - Professor Binkert's Webpage - Oakland ...

linguistic structures - Professor Binkert's Webpage - Oakland ...

linguistic structures - Professor Binkert's Webpage - Oakland ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

8<br />

The approach, therefore, seems successful. The students have memorized some formulas, the<br />

definitions in (4), applied them in exercises, and learned a tool that can be reused elsewhere. The<br />

teachers have done their job in supplying the formula for completing the assignment.<br />

The difficulty is that English also has sentences like (6).<br />

(6) The little girl feared the dogs.<br />

In (6), the dogs are still causing the fear and the little girl is still experiencing the fear so it seems,<br />

according to (4), that the dogs is still the subject phrase and the little girl is still the object phrase.<br />

If that is correct, then we cannot say that subjects precede verbs in English and objects follow, which<br />

seems to be the case in most sentences. So something is wrong. Since students do not know how<br />

or why the definitions in (4) were proposed in the first place, they generally have no idea what to<br />

do when the definitions seem to fail as they do in sentences like (6).<br />

Now notice what is going on here at an abstract level. We have some data, which happen to be<br />

sentences in English. We have a hypothesis which describes the data, namely, (4). We get some<br />

further data which the hypothesis can’t handle. Thus, we must revise the hypothesis and look for<br />

another proposal. This is exactly what happens frequently in the workplace. Consider, for example,<br />

another scenario from the auto industry.<br />

Several years ago, Ford Motor Company produced a van called the Mercury Villager. This van<br />

originally only had a sliding door on the passenger side. Marketing analysts at Ford had determined<br />

that parents would not want to have a sliding door on the driver’s side because their children might<br />

get out of the van on the side of traffic. The other major auto manufacturers had vans with sliding<br />

doors on both sides, because their marketing analysts had determined that people would find it too<br />

inconvenient going to one side of the car to deposit items like groceries and then walking to the<br />

other side of the car to get into the driver’s seat. Having sliding doors on both sides of the van was<br />

considered a plus. Ultimately, Ford modified its design and offered a new Villager with sliding<br />

doors on both sides. “A sliding door on the driver’s side of the vehicle [was] the subject of popular<br />

demand from customers” (carpartswholesale.com/cpw/mercury~villager~parts.html). At an abstract<br />

level, this problem has the same characteristics as our grammatical problem. Ford began with data,<br />

namely, its marketing analysis of what it seemed parents wanted in a van. Ford then designed a car<br />

accordingly. But further data indicated that the design did not match all the needs of its customers.<br />

So, Ford began to produce a new van which did match the additional customer information.<br />

Returning to our grammatical example, consider now an alternative approach to describing subjects<br />

and objects which begins by looking at the data, that is, good sentences like those in (5), (6), and (7),<br />

as well as bad sentences like those in (8), where the asterisk means that the sentence is<br />

ungrammatical.<br />

(7) a. They frightened her.<br />

b. She feared them.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!