31.08.2013 Views

AIAS newsletter Autumn 2012 TW.indd

AIAS newsletter Autumn 2012 TW.indd

AIAS newsletter Autumn 2012 TW.indd

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

34<br />

Interview with Maarten Berg<br />

FOCUS ON...<br />

RESEARCH<br />

A solidarity experiment in the Dapperbuurt<br />

Laboratory experiments are becoming increasingly popular among social scientists. By letting subjects play a game<br />

in an environment that is fully under control – the lab – it is possible to fi ne tune the conditions under which people<br />

interact. Thus, one can draw inferences about the causal relationship between these conditions and their behaviour.<br />

Students are the obvious group to select the participants for these experiments from. However, one may question<br />

whether the behaviour of university students really refl ects the behaviour of the population at large, especially if these<br />

students are used to participate in laboratory experiments. It is therefore of great interest to compare the behaviour<br />

of students with the behaviour of ‘ordinary’ people in the same experimental design. This was put to practice in the<br />

experiments that <strong>AIAS</strong> researcher Maarten Berg performed as part of the research programme ‘Solidarity in the 21st<br />

Century’.<br />

What kind of experiment did you perform?<br />

In the basic experiment, four participants<br />

had to answer individually ten multiple<br />

choice questions. The two participants who<br />

performed the best on this quiz were declared<br />

to be the winners. They were each<br />

rewarded with 20 euro’s that they were allowed<br />

to distribute over the four group<br />

members (including themselves). They were<br />

free to choose any possible distribution, e.g.<br />

keep all 20 euro’s to themselves, to distribute<br />

the sum equally (5-5-5-5), or to do something<br />

else. Because the participants actually<br />

earned real money with this ‘solidarity game’<br />

(and lost real money by sharing), sharing behaviour<br />

in this experiment is strong proof<br />

of the potential for solidarity. A problem<br />

of standard survey research (“How much<br />

would you be willing to share....?”) is that<br />

is very vulnerable to socially desirable statements.<br />

However, the main research focus<br />

was not the degree of solidarity, but rather<br />

the differences in solidarity between slightly<br />

different experimental conditions.<br />

Why did you choose the visitors of the<br />

Dapper market, a daily street market<br />

in Amsterdam, to participate in the experiments?<br />

The starting point of our project, ‘Solidarity<br />

in the 21st century’, was that solidarity in<br />

Dutch society might be endangered by societal<br />

developments, such as the aging population<br />

(creating a potential confl ict between<br />

generations) and increasing cultural diversity.<br />

The Dappermarket is a very diverse<br />

area of Amsterdam, and therefore very well<br />

suited to study some of our hypotheses. An<br />

additional benefi t was that Laurens Buijs, a<br />

sociologist from our team, did qualitative research<br />

in the Dappermarket area. This enabled<br />

us to compare and integrate our fi ndings,<br />

using very different methodologies.<br />

How did you convince people on the<br />

market to participate?<br />

Actually, this was very hard. We had to approach<br />

at least ten persons (and sometimes<br />

many more) to convince one person to<br />

participate. An additional complication was<br />

that we needed groups of exact four participants.<br />

This meant that occasionally we lost<br />

a potential participant, who was no longer<br />

willing to wait for the other participants to<br />

show up. With the great help of Laurens,<br />

Casper and others, we managed to do it after<br />

all.<br />

I think that a big problem was that people<br />

are so used to being approached (by people<br />

who want their money) that they did not<br />

realize that they would actually earn money<br />

with this experiment. After the experiment,<br />

participants were often grateful for receiving<br />

so much money for so little effort.<br />

What were the largest problems/obstacles<br />

that you were confronted with<br />

in performing the experiment in the<br />

Dapperbuurt?<br />

Besides having to approach the potential<br />

participants on the market, we had to create<br />

a lab in a community centre (‘buurthuis’)<br />

nearby. As the participants communicated<br />

with one another through computers,<br />

Casper had to create a network. We ran into<br />

all kinds of practical challenges, especially<br />

when participants did not fully understand<br />

the instructions or were far from fl uent in<br />

the Dutch language. Fortunately, for most<br />

participants this was not the case.<br />

Can you tell us the most remarkable<br />

differences between the outcomes of<br />

the experiment with the students and<br />

with the visitors of the Dapperbuurt?<br />

In the Dapperbuurt experiment we were especially<br />

interested in the characteristics of<br />

the participants (e.g. in terms of sex, age,<br />

cultural background, etc.) and the social<br />

distance between them. When we used students<br />

they were anonymous to one another<br />

(player 1, player 2, etc.). Our results showed<br />

that native participants share less money<br />

with Turkish or Moroccan people than with<br />

fellow natives.<br />

Another difference between the two studies<br />

was that the participants from the Dap-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!