30.12.2012 Views

The Science and Statistics Behind Spanking Suggests that

The Science and Statistics Behind Spanking Suggests that

The Science and Statistics Behind Spanking Suggests that

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

11-FULLER_FINAL_AFTERPROOF.DOC 2/17/2009 8:50 AM<br />

2009] THE SCIENCE AND STATISTICS BEHIND SPANKING 287<br />

For example, Dr. Straus often focuses on theoretical models <strong>and</strong><br />

surveys of adults <strong>that</strong> were spanked as teenagers. 221 While it is true <strong>that</strong><br />

spanking teenagers can be worse than mentally punishing them,<br />

spanking young children almost never is. 222<br />

Even on the rare occasion <strong>that</strong> Dr. Straus studied preteens, he only<br />

focused on six- to nine-year-olds <strong>that</strong> were spanked an average of 156<br />

times a year. 223 That’s up to thirteen times the normal rate. 224 Parents<br />

who physically discipline rarely spank their nine-year-old more than<br />

once a month. 225 With such an unnatural sample, it’s no wonder Dr.<br />

Straus’ was able to associate corporal punishment with antisocial<br />

behavior. 226 Many nine-year-olds <strong>that</strong> are spanked 156 times a year may<br />

simply be more prone to behavior problems, regardless of the type of<br />

punishment they get. 227<br />

When researchers ignore preexisting conditions (like a child’s<br />

preexisting misbehavior), they erroneously associate spanking with<br />

harmful effects. 228 This is known as the “intervention selection bias.” 229<br />

221. E.g., Straus, ROUNDTABLE, supra note 175, at 36-37 (mentioning his surveys of<br />

teenagers, which found links to spousal assault <strong>and</strong> abusing one’s child later in life, then<br />

generalizing such links to all spanking); STRAUS, supra note 33 (referring often to his theoretical<br />

models <strong>and</strong> surveys of teenagers).<br />

222. E.g., Larzelere, Review, supra note 17, at 827 (finding grounding less detrimental than<br />

spanking for teens, but for young children, spanking was more beneficial than nine common<br />

disciplines, including timeout <strong>and</strong> reasoning).<br />

223. Rosellini, supra note 27 (“His research indicated <strong>that</strong> frequent spanking (three or more<br />

times a week) of children 6 to 9 years old, tracked over a period of two years, increased a child’s<br />

antisocial behavior, measured in activities like cheating, bullying, or lying.”);<br />

ROBERT E. LARZELERE, CRITIQUE OF ANTI-SPANKING STUDY, available at<br />

http://ches.okstate.edu/facultystaff/Larzelere/CritiqueStraus.html [hereinafter LARZELERE,<br />

CRITIQUE] (“<strong>The</strong> only thing <strong>that</strong> Straus et al. (1997) have proven is <strong>that</strong> spanking 6- to 9-year-olds<br />

at the rate of 156 times a year has a small, but detrimental effect (accounting for 1.3% of subsequent<br />

variation in anti-social behavior). Most children spanked from 1 to 25 times annually were in their<br />

most-improved group . . . .”).<br />

224. See, e.g., Baumrind, Discipline Controversy, supra note 157, at 409 (saying <strong>that</strong>, by age<br />

nine, only one-third of the parents spanked their children as often as once a month).<br />

225. See, e.g., id.<br />

226. See, e.g., supra note 223 <strong>and</strong> accompanying text.<br />

227. See, e.g., Baumrind, Ordinary Physical Punishment, supra note 128, at 585 (“A child<br />

who is not dispositionally compliant, however, is likely also to be less malleable <strong>and</strong> therefore likely<br />

to require more forceful parental intervention . . . .”).<br />

228. See, e.g., Robert E. Larzelere, Ph.D., Univ. Neb. Med. Ctr., <strong>The</strong> Difficulty of Making<br />

Valid Causal Inferences from Passive Longitudinal Designs, Presentation at Univ. Cal., Berkeley on<br />

Inferring Causality from Longitudinal Studies (Mar. 21, 2003), available at<br />

http://ihd.berkeley.edu/larzelere.htm [hereinafter Larzelere, <strong>The</strong> Difficulty] (saying the intervention<br />

selection bias is an explanation <strong>that</strong>, if ignored, can lead to incorrect conclusions about corrective<br />

interventions. For instance, the association between mental health treatment <strong>and</strong> subsequent

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!