01.08.2013 Views

Education and Training in Autism and Developmental Disabilities

Education and Training in Autism and Developmental Disabilities

Education and Training in Autism and Developmental Disabilities

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

stood attribution when two consecutive misunderst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gs<br />

occur.<br />

Method<br />

Participant<br />

The study participant was Takao, a 12-year-old<br />

boy who was diagnosed with autism by a neuropediatrician<br />

accord<strong>in</strong>g to the DSM-IV (APA,<br />

1994). The child’s hear<strong>in</strong>g, vision, <strong>and</strong> motor<br />

development were with<strong>in</strong> the normal range.<br />

Accord<strong>in</strong>g to the Kyoto Scale of Psychological<br />

Development (Ikusawa, Matsushita, & Nakase,<br />

2002), his overall development was equivalent<br />

to 2:3. He could follow one-step spoken directions<br />

used <strong>in</strong> the rout<strong>in</strong>e. When he wanted<br />

someth<strong>in</strong>g, he exclusively relied on prel<strong>in</strong>guistic<br />

communication modes, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g vocalization,<br />

open palms, reach<strong>in</strong>g, or po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

When a written word was shown to him, he did<br />

not emit the correct correspond<strong>in</strong>g sounds.<br />

However, the vocalization synchronized with<br />

po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g to each syllable. Further, he was able<br />

to write a few words, but did not use letters <strong>in</strong><br />

functional ways.<br />

At the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the study, Takao had<br />

never been taught to use picture cards as a<br />

mode of request<strong>in</strong>g behavior. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

the repair assessment, which followed procedures<br />

proposed by Ohtake et al. (2005), <strong>in</strong><br />

free-play situations, Takao exhibited persistent<br />

requests when he faced with any types of<br />

communication breakdowns (i.e., ask<strong>in</strong>g for<br />

clarification vocally or gesturally, not attend<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>and</strong> not respond<strong>in</strong>g, misunderst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gs)<br />

<strong>in</strong> all breakdown opportunities across play activities<br />

(e.g., trampol<strong>in</strong>e, sw<strong>in</strong>g, bike rid<strong>in</strong>g).<br />

In addition, he frequently used communication<br />

forms that specified what he wanted (e.g.,<br />

po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g to, reach<strong>in</strong>g for, or lead<strong>in</strong>g listener’s<br />

h<strong>and</strong> toward the object) <strong>in</strong> response to the<br />

listener’s misunderst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gs. For example, <strong>in</strong><br />

situation where his teacher presented a shovel<br />

<strong>and</strong> said “You want to play shovel?” <strong>in</strong> response<br />

to his open palm used for request<strong>in</strong>g<br />

“push<strong>in</strong>g sw<strong>in</strong>g,” Takao threw the shovel <strong>and</strong><br />

led his teacher’s h<strong>and</strong> toward the cha<strong>in</strong> of the<br />

sw<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Sett<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> Materials<br />

Takao attended a special school affiliated with<br />

a university, where he was educated with two<br />

other students with autism <strong>in</strong> a self-conta<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

classroom led by two teachers. All sessions<br />

were conducted at a table us<strong>in</strong>g a one-to-one<br />

format dur<strong>in</strong>g a highly structured teach<strong>in</strong>g<br />

period when communication skills were targeted.<br />

The room was divided <strong>in</strong>to three spaces<br />

by partitions <strong>and</strong> lockers. Each of the spaces<br />

(2 3 m) was assigned to a different activity<br />

to help Takao predict what activity he was<br />

assigned to do. Dur<strong>in</strong>g the sessions, only<br />

Takao <strong>and</strong> his teacher were <strong>in</strong> the room.<br />

At each session, Takao <strong>and</strong> the teacher sat<br />

at a desk. A few plates of pens were placed<br />

horizontally on a table <strong>in</strong> view of the student,<br />

but out of reach (i.e., approximately 2m<strong>in</strong><br />

front of him). The distance between each<br />

plate was approximately 30 cm. Picture cards<br />

(6 7 cm) were placed with<strong>in</strong> easy reach<br />

(20–30 cm), at a distance of approximately 1<br />

cm between each. The pens used <strong>in</strong> this study<br />

consisted of five br<strong>and</strong>s (TOMBO TM , MIT-<br />

SUBISHI TM , UNI TM , ZEBRA TM , SAKURA TM ),<br />

four colors (red, black, blue, green), <strong>and</strong> two<br />

sizes (thick <strong>and</strong> th<strong>in</strong>).<br />

Before start<strong>in</strong>g the session, the teacher provided<br />

Takao an opportunity to access the table<br />

where the pens were placed to let him know<br />

what pens were available. Then the teacher<br />

<strong>in</strong>vited him to sit at a desk where an A6 size of<br />

paper (10 15 cm) <strong>and</strong> picture cards were<br />

placed <strong>and</strong> waited for his communication <strong>in</strong>itiation.<br />

Assessment for Preoccupied Attributions<br />

In a one-hour <strong>in</strong>terview with his special education<br />

teacher, Takao demonstrated a strong<br />

<strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> pa<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g the logo of a specific TV<br />

program (“Comedy Focus”). When he pa<strong>in</strong>ted<br />

the logo, he persistently po<strong>in</strong>ted to or reached<br />

for a pen of a specific br<strong>and</strong> (TOMBO or<br />

MITSUBISHI), a specific size (th<strong>in</strong>), <strong>and</strong> a<br />

specific comb<strong>in</strong>ation of color (red <strong>and</strong> black).<br />

Dur<strong>in</strong>g all trials conducted <strong>in</strong> the study, we<br />

double-checked to see if he (a) accepted when<br />

a pen with these preferred attributions was<br />

provided <strong>and</strong> (b) rejected when a pen without<br />

these preferred attributions was provided. All<br />

of Takao’s accept<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> reject<strong>in</strong>g behaviors<br />

were consistent with the <strong>in</strong>formation on preoccupied<br />

attributions collected from the <strong>in</strong>terview.<br />

Dur<strong>in</strong>g the first basel<strong>in</strong>e condition, Takao<br />

412 / <strong>Education</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2010

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!