26.12.2013 Views

THE DEVELOPMENT OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTION IN EARLY ...

THE DEVELOPMENT OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTION IN EARLY ...

THE DEVELOPMENT OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTION IN EARLY ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

did not answer in terms of the postswitch dimension only, then they were asked a specific contrast question (for<br />

example), "Is it a red card or a green card?" Some of these children (about 40%) answered the first question in<br />

terms of the postswitch dimension only, and the remaining children correctly answered the specific contrast<br />

question. All children were then asked to sort, and roughly a third of them now sorted correctly. According to<br />

these authors, most 3- to 4-year-olds fail to sort correctly during the postswitch phase because they think of the<br />

card in terms of its preswitch attributes (Towse et al., 2000, p. 361), an interpretation also proposed by Frye<br />

(1999, p. 123) and Zelazo and Frye (1997, p. 145).<br />

Kirkham et al. (in press) also found that when required to label the stimuli, 3- to 4-year-olds (who normally<br />

failed) tended to pass (78% of 3- to 4-year-olds passed), and when cards were left face up in the sorting trays (as<br />

opposed to the usual practice of putting them face down), 4-year-olds (who normally passed) tended to fail. The<br />

authors suggested that these two manipulations helped and hindered (respectively) children's efforts to inhibit<br />

attention to a previously useful aspect of the stimulus, and refocus on another, conflicting aspect of the same<br />

stimulus. Leaving the test cards face-up may have increased the salience of the preswitch dimension, whereas<br />

labeling the test cards may have helped children to refocus their attention to the currently relevant dimension.<br />

In a final study to be considered, Perner and Lang (2002; see also Brooks, Hanauer, Padowska, & Rosman,<br />

2003) found that children performed well on two new versions of the DCCS. In one version, called the Reversal<br />

Shift version, all cards (test and target) varied only in shape, and children were not, in fact, required to make a<br />

"dimensional change." Rather, children were first asked to play the "correct" animals game ("All the horses go<br />

to the horse. And all the fish go to the fish."), and then, during the postswitch phase, children were asked to play<br />

the "wrong" game ("Now, all the horses go to the fish. And all the fish go to the horse."). These authors also<br />

found that children performed well in a version of the DCCS in which (a) no target cards were used, and (b) the<br />

switch was explained in terms of a change in desires. Instead of target cards, each sorting tray was associated<br />

with a picture of a boy or a girl. During the preswitch phase, each target character was described as wanting all<br />

the cards of a particular color (e.g., "The girl wants all the green ones"). Then, during the postswitch phase,<br />

children were asked to switch from sorting by color to sorting by shape, and this switch was justified by a<br />

change in the children's desires (e.g., "The girl now wants all the fish."). To account for their findings, Perner<br />

and Lang (2002) suggested that their new versions of the DCCS provide more explicit clues than the standard<br />

version to the fact that an alternative description is required.<br />

Correlational Studies<br />

A growing number of studies have found that performance on the DCCS is correlated with individual and group<br />

differences. For example, several studies, starting with Frye et al. (1995), have found that performance on the<br />

DCCS is correlated with performance on tasks assessing theory of mind (e.g., understanding false beliefs), even<br />

when controlling for age and vocabulary (e.g., Carlson & Moses, 2001; Perner et al., 2002). These findings<br />

support the suggestion that the DCCS provides a marker of relatively domain-general aspects of executive<br />

function.<br />

Similar results have also been found in studies with clinical samples known to have impairments in theory of<br />

mind and executive function. Zelazo, Jacques, Burack, and Frye (2002) found relations between executive<br />

function (including DCCS performance) and autism in a small group of moderately high functioning individuals<br />

with autism-spectrum disorders. Colvert, Custance, and Swettenham (2002) replicated this finding in a larger<br />

sample, and also found that this group exhibited impairments on the DCCS (and theory of mind) relative to two<br />

mental-age matched comparison groups. Zelazo, Burack, Benedetto, and Frye (1996) reported similar findings<br />

for a group of individuals with Down syndrome, suggesting that correlations between theory of mind and<br />

performance on the DCCS are robust across a range of individual and group differences, and that several<br />

different types of developmental disorder may result in impaired executive function as measured by the DCCS.<br />

Finally, a study by Bialystok (1999) suggests that performance on the DCCS tasks may be related to<br />

bilingualism. In this study, a group of Chinese-English bilingual children (tested in Toronto) were better at the<br />

DCCS than were non-Chinese monolingual children, although the basis for this advantage remains to be

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!