26.12.2013 Views

THE DEVELOPMENT OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTION IN EARLY ...

THE DEVELOPMENT OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTION IN EARLY ...

THE DEVELOPMENT OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTION IN EARLY ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Negative Priming version are not. By contrast, the findings from the Negative Priming version are consistent<br />

with the negative priming hypothesis, but the findings from the Partial Change are not. In order to capture the<br />

full pattern of empirical findings, an alternative account is needed that includes both processes of activation and<br />

processes of inhibition. This alternative is described in Chapter VI.<br />

VI. <strong>THE</strong> <strong>DEVELOPMENT</strong> <strong>OF</strong> <strong>EXECUTIVE</strong> <strong>FUNCTION</strong>: COGNITIVE COMPLEXITYAND<br />

CONTROL-REVISED<br />

SUMMARY <strong>OF</strong> RESULTS ACROSS STUDIES<br />

The current Monograph consisted of four studies (9 experiments) designed to explore the development of<br />

children's executive function, as revealed by their performance on the DCCS. The DCCS is a widely used<br />

measure of rule use, and, as such, it provides a relatively well-defined window on the development of<br />

executive function during the preschool years. Executive function in general, and as assessed by the DCCS in<br />

particular, has been implicated in children's behavior in a variety of domains and in a variety of subject groups,<br />

including bilingual children, children with autism, and children with Down's syndrome.<br />

By experimentally manipulating features of the DCCS paradigm, we tested hypotheses about the cognitive<br />

mechanisms underlying executive function and its development. Results of this research have the potential to<br />

shed light not only on children's development in a variety of domains, but also on dorsolateral prefrontal<br />

cortical function especially during the preschool years. Performance on the DCCS almost certainly relies on<br />

neural systems involving prefrontal cortex. In particular, dorsolateral regions of prefrontal cortex have been<br />

implicated in a number of tasks quite similar to the DCCS (see Nagahama et al., 2001; Rogers, Andrews,<br />

Grasby, Brooks, & Robbins, 2000, for research using functional magnetic resonance imaging [fMRI] and<br />

positron-emission tomography [PET], respectively).<br />

Study 1 examined whether 3- to 4-year-olds' errors on the DCCS can be attributed to capacity limitations on<br />

memory. Three experiments showed that 3- to 4-year-olds performed well on four versions of the DCCS in<br />

which children were required to use four nonoverlapping rules (i.e., children were not required to treat the<br />

same test card differently depending on the relevant dimension). In Experiment 1, the two new versions were<br />

unidimensional. In Experiment 2, children were given the 2+2-Rules (bidimensional, no overlap) version, in<br />

which they were shown four target cards and asked to sort test cards first by two rules based on one dimension<br />

(e.g., color) and then by two rules based on the other dimension (e.g., shape). Although children needed to<br />

remember which cards to sort by color and which cards to sort by shape, there was no conflict among rules<br />

because each test card was uniquely associated with a single rule. Children performed well on this version,<br />

showing that bidimensionality per se does not cause children's difficulty on the DCCS.<br />

In Experiment 3, children performed well on the 4-Rules (super-ordinate) version, which likely posed even<br />

greater memory demands than the 4-Rules versions used in Experiments 1 and 2 because it involved<br />

superordinate rules and a heterogeneous series of test cards. Not only did children need to keep four<br />

superordinate distinctions in mind, but they also need to determine which rule applied to each test card (i.e.,<br />

there were storage plus processing demands). Nonetheless, children performed significantly better on the 4-<br />

Rules (superordinate) version than on the standard version of the DCCS. Taken together, findings from the three<br />

experiments provide strong evidence against most memory capacity accounts of performance on the DCCS, and<br />

they point to the importance of conflict among rules.<br />

Study 2 was designed to explore further the role of conflict among rules by determining more precisely the<br />

circumstances in which children can and cannot treat a single test card in two different ways. Three experiments<br />

assessed the effects of rule dimensionality (bidimensional vs. unidimensional rules) and two types of stimulus<br />

characteristic (1 vs. 2 test cards; integrated vs. separable stimuli). Here, unlike Study 1, bidimensional rule sets<br />

always involved conflict among rules whereas unidimensional rule sets did not. Results revealed effects of rule<br />

dimensionality but no effects of stimulus characteristics. Sorting by even a single pair of rules was difficult for<br />

3- to 4-year-olds when the two rules spanned major branches of the hierarchical tree structure shown in Figure

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!