26.12.2013 Views

THE DEVELOPMENT OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTION IN EARLY ...

THE DEVELOPMENT OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTION IN EARLY ...

THE DEVELOPMENT OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTION IN EARLY ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Figure 3. This experiment also assessed the effect of a potentially important difference in the stimuli used in<br />

Experiment 4 versus those used in Experiment 5—whether test cards were integrated versus separated. In this<br />

manner, Experiment 6 examined the possibility, raised in defense of the redescription theory, that when<br />

presented with separated stimuli, children serially alternate between focusing on different aspects of the<br />

stimulus, without coordinating two alternative perspectives. If children simply serially alternated between<br />

different perspectives in Experiment 5, then they should do well on a bidimensional version of the DCCS that<br />

involved separated stimuli.<br />

Shepp and colleagues (Barrett & Shepp, 1988; Shepp & Barrett, 1991; Shepp, Barrett, & Kolbet, 1987; but see<br />

Ridderinkhof, van der Molen, Band, & Bashore, 1997) have suggested that there are age-related changes in the<br />

way in which young children and adults perceive objects (e.g., colored rabbits). Whereas younger children<br />

perceive these stimuli holistically as integrated wholes, older children and adults are better at analyzing these<br />

stimuli into separate dimensions or features. Support for this suggestion comes from findings that in matching<br />

tasks, the performance of younger children (e.g., 5- to-6-year olds) is more closely related to whether the stimuli<br />

are spatially integrated or separated than is the performance of older children and adults.<br />

According to this separability hypothesis, children's performance on the DCCS should be related to whether the<br />

dimensions of the target and test cards are presented in an integrated or separated fashion. Indeed, children's<br />

difficulty with the DCCS may be due to difficulty analyzing the stimuli into separate attributes, located on<br />

different dimensions.<br />

In this experiment, four versions of the DCCS were created by crossing two variables, rule dimensionality<br />

(bidimensional vs. unidimensional) and whether the test cards were integrated or separated (see Figure 12).<br />

According to CCC theory, children were expected to perform well on both unidimensional versions because,<br />

although these versions involve conflicting rules, they do not require the use of rules nested under different<br />

major branches of the tree structure in Figure 3. In contrast, children were expected to perform poorly in the<br />

bidimensional versions because these versions do require the use of rules nested under different major branches.<br />

The separability hypothesis was tested by comparing children's performance on the two bidimensional versions.<br />

Method<br />

Participants and design. Forty-eight 3- to 4-year-olds (M = 42.8 months; range: 37 to 49 months; 23 girls<br />

and 25 boys) were recruited in the same fashion as in Experiment 1, except that 13 of the children tested in<br />

this experiment also participated in Experiment 5. These 13 children were evenly distributed across the four<br />

experimental versions (4 children in one version, and 3 each in the other three versions), and removal of these<br />

children from the analyses did not alter the results. An additional 2 children were tested but excluded from the<br />

final sample because they refused to complete the experiment.<br />

Twelve children were assigned randomly to each of the four conditions: unidimensional separated (US),<br />

unidimensional integrated (UI), bidimensional separated (BS), and bidimensional integrated (BI). In each<br />

condition, children received two tasks (i.e., a replication of the version used in that condition), one involving<br />

color and/or shape, and the other involving pattern and/or size. The order in which rule pairs were presented<br />

was counterbalanced across participants.<br />

Materials<br />

Two sets of cards were used. The first set varied in color (red or blue) and shape (cars or flowers). The second<br />

set varied in size (large or small) and pattern (dots or stripes). In the unidimensional versions, separated test<br />

cards depicted the two possible response options (e.g., red and blue) adjacently on a single card, whereas the<br />

integrated test cards depicted the two aspects of a dimension in a single form, such as a red and blue rabbit. The<br />

target cards for such a version would be a red rabbit and a blue rabbit (see Figure 12). For the separated test<br />

cards, one level of the relevant dimension was presented on the left side of the card for half of the trials, and on<br />

the right side for the remaining trials.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!