26.12.2013 Views

THE DEVELOPMENT OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTION IN EARLY ...

THE DEVELOPMENT OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTION IN EARLY ...

THE DEVELOPMENT OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTION IN EARLY ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

(2000) pointed out, even in the DCCS children need to understand the experimenter's intention to signal that the<br />

rules have changed.<br />

To date, theories of executive function have focused almost exclusively on the processing of information<br />

defined objectively from a third-person perspective. Eventually, however, if we are to take seriously the<br />

suggestion that executive function corresponds to consciously controlled behavior, we must attempt to reconcile<br />

our third-person descriptions of cognition and behavior with first-person, intentional characterizations of<br />

meaningful thought. Although this objective has largely been lost since the days of Baldwin (1897) and Piaget<br />

(1963), it is increasingly being rediscovered, both in developmental psychology (e.g., Barresi & Moore, 1996;<br />

Bloom & Tinker, 2001; Deak, 2000; Overton, 2003) and in other areas of psychology (e.g., Thompson &<br />

Varela, 2001; Varela, 1996).<br />

REVISED CCC <strong>THE</strong>ORY<br />

The findings from this Monograph support a revision of the CCC theory in which the key claims of CCC theory<br />

are retained. These claims include the following: (a) Executive function is best viewed functionally, as an<br />

outcome, not an explanatory construct. (b) Children's plans are assumed to correspond literally to rules,<br />

formulated in potentially silent self-directed speech. (c) In response to particular problems, children formulate<br />

rule systems in an ad hoc fashion and use these rules systems to regulate their inferences and action, and select<br />

particular pieces of information for maintainence in working memory. (d) There are several age-related<br />

increases in the highest possible complexity of children's rule systems, and these increases can be observed in<br />

many domains of behavior. (e) Complexity is measured by the number of levels of embedding in children's rule<br />

systems. (f) Age-related changes in complexity are made possible by age-related changes in reflection that in<br />

turn might be dependent on the experience-dependent maturation of prefrontal cortex. Based on evidence from<br />

animal studies (e.g., see Huttenlocher, 2002), it is very likely that the maturation of prefrontal cortex is<br />

influenced by environmental stimulation. Moreover, there is evidence that parents' verbal scaffolding of<br />

children's play is related to their executive function skills (Landry, Miller-Loncar, Smith, & Swank, 2002), and<br />

training studies designed to improve children's executive function have been successful (e.g., Dowsett &<br />

Livesey, 2000; Kloo & Perner, in press). Clearly, the role of the environment in general, and parent-child<br />

interaction in particular, on the development of executive function needs to be examined in more detail.<br />

The revised CCC theory (the CCC-r) adds to the original theory in several ways. It specifies more clearly the<br />

circumstances in which children will have difficulty using rules at various levels of complexity, and it provides<br />

a more detailed account of how to determine the complexity of rules required in a task. It takes account not only<br />

of the activation of rules as a function of experience but also the inhibition of rules. Finally, insofar as the CCCr<br />

theory takes intentionality seriously, it represents an integration of the CCC theory with the Levels of<br />

Consciousness (LOG) Model (e.g., Zelazo, 1999, 2000; P. R. Zelazo & P. D. Zelazo, 1998). CCC theory shows<br />

how age-related changes in executive function—considered as a functional construct are due to age-related<br />

changes in the maximum complexity of the rules that children can formulate and use when solving problems.<br />

The LOC model shows how these age-related changes in maximum rule complexity are, in turn, made possible<br />

by age-related increases in the degree to which children can consciously reflect on the rules they represent (i.e.,<br />

age-related increases in children's highest level of consciousness; Zelazo, 1999, 2000, in press). Together, the<br />

CCC theory and the LOC model provide a framework for an intentionalistic, but still scientifically tractable,<br />

account of executive function (Frye & Zelazo, 2003).<br />

The LOG model is an information-processing model designed to capture the development of consciousness and<br />

explain its role in executive function. As an information processing model, it traces the flow of information<br />

through a functional system—in this case, illustrating the way in which intentional objects (i .e.,<br />

representations) are operated on as they come to figure in the conscious control of behavior. As a developmental<br />

model, the LOC model depicts the way in which this functional system changes in the course of ontogeny, and<br />

shows the consequences of these changes for executive function. According to this model, higher levels of

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!