29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society — Volume 14 — November ...

Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society — Volume 14 — November ...

Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society — Volume 14 — November ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Saturday Morning Papers 182–189<br />

11:40–11:55 (182)<br />

The Role <strong>of</strong> Physical Similarity Versus Numerical Distance in<br />

Numerical Judgment Tasks. DALE J. COHEN, University <strong>of</strong> North<br />

Carolina, Wilmington<strong>—</strong>For decades, researchers have been studying<br />

how people perceive numbers. Researchers have generally come to <strong>the</strong><br />

conclusion that, when people are presented a number (such as 2, 3, etc.),<br />

<strong>the</strong>y will automatically perceive <strong>the</strong> quantity symbolized by that number.<br />

I present and test an alternative hypo<strong>the</strong>sis.<br />

Implicit Memory<br />

Independence Ballroom, Saturday Morning, 10:20–11:55<br />

Chaired by Eric-Jan Wagenmakers, University <strong>of</strong> Amsterdam<br />

10:20–10:35 (183)<br />

Are Researchers Egoistic When They Write an Abstract? A Bayesian<br />

Hierarchical Test <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Name Letter Effect. ERIC-JAN WAGEN-<br />

MAKERS, University <strong>of</strong> Amsterdam, OLIVER DYJAS, University <strong>of</strong><br />

Tübingen, & RAOUL GRASMAN & RUUD WETZELS, University <strong>of</strong><br />

Amsterdam<strong>—</strong>People prefer <strong>the</strong>ir initials to <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r letters <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> alphabet,<br />

a phenomenon known as <strong>the</strong> name letter effect. This effect, researchers<br />

have argued, makes Angela move to Los Angeles, makes Phil buy a Philips<br />

TV, and influences Dennis to become a dentist. In order to establish such<br />

associations between people’s initials and <strong>the</strong>ir behavior, researchers typically<br />

carry out statistical analyses <strong>of</strong> large databases. Unfortunately, <strong>the</strong><br />

standard p-value procedures ignore <strong>the</strong> hierarchical structure <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> data<br />

and do not allow one to confirm <strong>the</strong> null hypo<strong>the</strong>sis. Here, we propose a<br />

Bayesian hierarchical hypo<strong>the</strong>sis test that avoids <strong>the</strong>se limitations. We illustrate<br />

<strong>the</strong> method with examples that involve <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> author initials in<br />

scientific abstracts. The conclusions from our Bayesian hypo<strong>the</strong>sis test are<br />

sometimes in radical opposition to those based on p-value procedures.<br />

10:40–10:55 (184)<br />

Sequential Effects Reflect Learning <strong>of</strong> Temporal Structure. MATT<br />

JONES, TIM CURRAN, MICHAEL C. MOZER, & MATTHEW H.<br />

WILDER, University <strong>of</strong> Colorado, Boulder<strong>—</strong>Binary choice tasks such<br />

as two-alternative forced choice tasks show a remarkably consistent pattern<br />

<strong>of</strong> sequential effects, whereby choices and response times depend<br />

on <strong>the</strong> detailed pattern <strong>of</strong> prior stimuli going back at least five trials. We<br />

show that existing data are well explained by a combination <strong>of</strong> two priming<br />

mechanisms, reflecting incremental learning <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> base rate and<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> repetition rate in <strong>the</strong> sequence. EEG and o<strong>the</strong>r types <strong>of</strong> evidence<br />

indicate that <strong>the</strong> base rate mechanism resides in response preparation<br />

(i.e., response priming) and that <strong>the</strong> repetition mechanism resides in<br />

stimulus identification (i.e., learning <strong>the</strong> rate <strong>of</strong> stimulus repetitions vs.<br />

alternations). However, results from a new experiment that manipulated<br />

<strong>the</strong> repetition rate between subjects show that <strong>the</strong> learning mechanism<br />

is more sophisticated than simple priming. The findings highlight a tension<br />

between <strong>the</strong> two broad and well-established classes <strong>of</strong> trace-based<br />

memory models and error-driven learning models. Attempts at reconciling<br />

<strong>the</strong>se approaches will be discussed.<br />

11:00–11:15 (185)<br />

Knowledge Representation in a New Perceptual–Motor Sequence<br />

Learning Task. PAUL J. REBER, DANIEL J. SANCHEZ, & ERIC W.<br />

GOBEL, Northwestern University<strong>—</strong>A new experimental paradigm is<br />

described for studying perceptual–motor sequence learning that combines<br />

<strong>the</strong> well-studied serial reaction time (SRT) task with a novel interface<br />

derived from several popular video games. The new task naturally<br />

incorporates timing information into motor sequence learning. In Experiment<br />

1, timing is shown to be integrated with sequence knowledge<br />

so that minimal transfer is observed when participants attempt to execute<br />

a familiar sequence <strong>of</strong> actions with unfamiliar interitem timing demands.<br />

The level <strong>of</strong> cognitive challenge can be easily manipulated to increase<br />

<strong>the</strong> difficulty <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> task. In Experiment 2, we show that making <strong>the</strong> task<br />

challenging produces an unusually strong dissociation between performance<br />

and awareness in healthy participants. Following learning a randomly<br />

selected sequence, participants’ error rates can be used to identify<br />

which sequence was learned even though <strong>the</strong>ir ability to recognize <strong>the</strong><br />

trained sequence is approximately at chance.<br />

28<br />

11:20–11:35 (186)<br />

Different Musical Features Predict Implicit and Explicit Memory<br />

for Melodies. ANDREA R. HALPERN, Bucknell University, & DAN-<br />

IEL MÜLLENSIEFEN & GERAINT WIGGINS, Goldsmiths College<strong>—</strong><br />

Evidence for <strong>the</strong> separation <strong>of</strong> implicit and explicit memory systems<br />

has come from experimental dissociations as well as patient and activation<br />

studies. A completely different kind <strong>of</strong> evidence may be gleaned<br />

from discovery-driven approaches. We presented a set <strong>of</strong> melodies from<br />

40 unfamiliar pop tunes, followed by 40 old and 40 new melodies for<br />

explicit recognition, and for rating on pleasantness. A set <strong>of</strong> musical<br />

features was selected to represent first-order characteristics relating to<br />

pitches, durations, and so on, <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> melodies, as well as second-order<br />

information about <strong>the</strong> frequencies <strong>of</strong> melodic patterns relative to both<br />

<strong>the</strong> 80-item test set, and <strong>the</strong> parent corpus <strong>of</strong> <strong>14</strong>,000 tunes. Regression<br />

models were quite successful in predicting which tunes elicited better<br />

explicit and implicit performance. However, <strong>the</strong> actual features retained<br />

in <strong>the</strong> models differed considerably. This dissociation between implicit<br />

and explicit memory measures was thus based entirely on sensitivity to<br />

different stimulus features.<br />

11:40–11:55 (187)<br />

Breaking Miller’s 7�2 Limit: Practice Effects in Absolute Identification<br />

(“Dead Reckoning”). PENNIE DODDS, CHRIS DONKIN,<br />

SCOTT D. BROWN, & ANDREW HEATHCOTE, University <strong>of</strong> Newcastle,<br />

Australia (read by Scott D. Brown)<strong>—</strong>Miller’s (1956) review <strong>of</strong> a<br />

series <strong>of</strong> absolute identification experiments suggested a fundamental<br />

limit to human processing capacity. This limit has been confirmed by<br />

50 years <strong>of</strong> subsequent research, and is thought to be highly resistant to<br />

practice. Following work from Rouder and colleagues, we outline an extensive<br />

series <strong>of</strong> experiments that show that people can learn to increase<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir information capacity. We discuss effects <strong>of</strong> stimulus manipulations<br />

(including modality and set size) as well as individual differences between<br />

participants. We also provide a framework in which our results<br />

(and Rouder et al.’s) can be reconciled with 50 years <strong>of</strong> prior research<strong>—</strong><br />

which appears incongruent.<br />

Human Learning and Instruction I<br />

Back Bay Ballroom C, Saturday Morning, 10:20–11:55<br />

Chaired by John B. Black, Teachers College, Columbia University<br />

10:20–10:35 (188)<br />

Haptic and Immediate Visual Feedback Increases Learning <strong>of</strong><br />

Mental Models. INSOOK HAN, JOHN B. BLACK, IRINA PALEY,<br />

& GREG HALLMAN, Teachers College, Columbia University (read<br />

by John B. Black)<strong>—</strong>In two studies, students learned mental models <strong>of</strong><br />

simple systems such as gear and pulley configurations through interactive<br />

visual simulations <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> systems. These were direct manipulation<br />

animations in which <strong>the</strong> students directly manipulated one variable and<br />

got immediate feedback on <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r variables. Providing haptic feedback<br />

using a force-feedback joystick, so that <strong>the</strong> students could feel how<br />

hard it was to move <strong>the</strong> gears or pulleys, increased learning over just<br />

providing visual feedback. A narrative voice-over also provided better<br />

learning than an expository voice-over. These results are consistent with<br />

a perceptually grounded approach to mental models <strong>of</strong> systems.<br />

10:40–10:55 (189)<br />

Learning at Study Versus Learning at Test: Does Familiarity Matter?<br />

KERRY A. CHALMERS & BEATRICE BORA, University <strong>of</strong> Newcastle,<br />

Australia<strong>—</strong>Learning during study and test was examined in a<br />

three-phase (i.e., study, Test 1, Test 2) recognition memory paradigm. In<br />

Experiment 1, 32 undergraduate psychology students studied a list consisting<br />

<strong>of</strong> familiar (high- and low-frequency) and unfamiliar (very lowfrequency)<br />

words prior to completing two yes/no recognition memory<br />

tests. Items from <strong>the</strong> study list served as targets for Test 1. Test List 1<br />

served as <strong>the</strong> target list for Test 2. Significant interactions between familiarity<br />

and testing were observed. High-frequency words were recognized<br />

more accurately at Test 2 than at Test 1, whereas very low-frequency<br />

words were recognized more accurately at Test 1 than at Test 2. In Experiment<br />

2, using <strong>the</strong> same paradigm but different stimuli (words and

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!