29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society — Volume 14 — November ...

Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society — Volume 14 — November ...

Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society — Volume 14 — November ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Sunday Morning Papers 300–307<br />

11:00–11:15 (300)<br />

Spatiotemporal Occlusion <strong>of</strong> Biological Motion Reveals Anticipation<br />

Processes <strong>of</strong> Soccer Players. SHUJI MORI, Kyushu University, &<br />

FUMIAKI YOSHITOMI, Japan Airlines Co.<strong>—</strong>Sport perception research<br />

has demonstrated that expert sport players anticipate forthcoming plays<br />

better than do <strong>the</strong>ir novice counterparts. What remains to be specified<br />

is <strong>the</strong> perceptual processes underlying <strong>the</strong>ir superior anticipation. To address<br />

this question about soccer goalkeepers who attempt to predict <strong>the</strong><br />

direction <strong>of</strong> penalty kicks, we conducted a series <strong>of</strong> experiments using<br />

biological motion <strong>of</strong> a penalty kicker in temporal and spatial occlusion<br />

tasks. The results show quite different patterns <strong>of</strong> anticipatory performance<br />

between experienced players and novices. The players’ anticipation<br />

accuracy increased suddenly from 250 msec to 150 msec before<br />

<strong>the</strong> kicker kicked <strong>the</strong> ball, and <strong>the</strong>ir anticipation relied on <strong>the</strong> kinematic<br />

information <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> kicker’s waist and support leg at that moment. On <strong>the</strong><br />

o<strong>the</strong>r hand, <strong>the</strong> novices’ accuracy increased gradually with increasing<br />

spatiotemporal information in <strong>the</strong> kicker’s motion. From those results,<br />

we describe two separate time courses <strong>of</strong> anticipation for experienced<br />

players and novices.<br />

11:20–11:35 (301)<br />

Forms <strong>of</strong> Underhand Throwing Fit Environmental Conditions and<br />

Personal Situations. JOHN RIESER & GAYATHRI NARASIMHAM,<br />

Vanderbilt University<strong>—</strong>People vary <strong>the</strong>ir forms <strong>of</strong> action so <strong>the</strong>re is<br />

enough force to get a job done while minimizing unnecessary movement<br />

and satisfying constraints posed by <strong>the</strong> actor and context. In this study,<br />

adults and young children tried to throw a ball with an underhanded toss<br />

into a wastebasket that was 1–30 m away. They could not see or hear <strong>the</strong><br />

ball’s trajectory. Every subject varied <strong>the</strong> form <strong>of</strong> throw by increasing <strong>the</strong><br />

number <strong>of</strong> degrees <strong>of</strong> freedom for longer distances<strong>—</strong>swinging from <strong>the</strong><br />

elbow alone for <strong>the</strong> 1 m target; from elbow and shoulder for <strong>the</strong> intermediate<br />

targets; elbow, shoulder, and waist for <strong>the</strong> far targets; and adding<br />

a run-up for <strong>the</strong> far<strong>the</strong>st target. We go on to ask whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> transition<br />

points optimize efficiency, are predictable from individual measures <strong>of</strong><br />

strength and limb lengths, and vary with <strong>the</strong> ball’s shape or weight, as<br />

well as target distance.<br />

11:40–11:55 (302)<br />

The Role <strong>of</strong> Concepts About Agency in Perceiving Actions. DAN-<br />

IEL T. LEVIN, MEGAN M. SAYLOR, & JULIE A. ADAMS, Vanderbilt<br />

University<strong>—</strong>In previous research, adults predicted that people would engage<br />

in more goal-directed actions that are guided by objects’ categories<br />

(as opposed to <strong>the</strong>ir surface features) than would computers or robots. In<br />

<strong>the</strong> present experiment, we explored how <strong>the</strong>se concepts affect people’s<br />

memory for <strong>the</strong> observed actions <strong>of</strong> a robot that were described ei<strong>the</strong>r<br />

in basic mechanical terms or in more intentional terms. After hearing<br />

<strong>the</strong> mechanical or intentional context, participants watched as <strong>the</strong> robot<br />

traveled among a set <strong>of</strong> objects, visiting (and “looking at”) four <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>m.<br />

Participants were better at naming <strong>the</strong> object categories in <strong>the</strong> scene for<br />

<strong>the</strong> intentional robot but were worse at remembering small variations in<br />

<strong>the</strong> intentional robot’s actions. This demonstrates that concepts about<br />

agency form an interpretive framework that guides attention in predictable<br />

ways during action perception.<br />

Vision<br />

Constitution Ballroom, Sunday Morning, 9:40–11:55<br />

Chaired by Thomas A. Busey, Indiana University, Bloomington<br />

9:40–9:55 (303)<br />

Temporal Processing Across <strong>the</strong> Senses: Individual Differences in<br />

Elders. THOMAS A. BUSEY, LARRY E. HUMES, JAMES C. CRAIG,<br />

& DIANE KEWLEY-PORT, Indiana University, Bloomington<strong>—</strong>Temporal<br />

slowing associated with aging affects <strong>the</strong> auditory, visual, and tactile<br />

senses. Such slowing may lead to deficits in processing rapid speech and<br />

changing scenes, as well as in haptic object identification. Participants<br />

made four different types <strong>of</strong> temporal order judgments with visual, auditory,<br />

and tactile stimuli. These tasks involved reporting <strong>the</strong> order <strong>of</strong> two<br />

or four patterns presented sequentially to <strong>the</strong> same location or ordering<br />

two items presented to different locations. Elder (n 5 150), middle-aged<br />

46<br />

(n 5 30), and younger (n 5 50) participants were tested. We varied <strong>the</strong><br />

stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) between <strong>the</strong> patterns to find a critical<br />

SOA. We found group differences in virtually all <strong>of</strong> our measures,<br />

suggesting a strong age-related component in each modality. Younger<br />

participants showed strong within- and across-modality correlations.<br />

Elders showed weaker correlations, especially across modalities. Factor<br />

analyses revealed that elders showed more modality independence,<br />

which contradicts common cause accounts <strong>of</strong> perceptual decline.<br />

10:00–10:15 (304)<br />

The Psychological Four-Color Mapping Problem. GREGORY<br />

FRANCIS, Purdue University<strong>—</strong>Ma<strong>the</strong>maticians have proven that four<br />

colors are sufficient to color 2-D maps so that no neighboring regions<br />

share <strong>the</strong> same color. Here, we consider <strong>the</strong> psychological four-color<br />

problem: identifying which four colors should be used to make a map<br />

easy to use. We build a model <strong>of</strong> visual search for this design task and<br />

demonstrate how to apply it to <strong>the</strong> task <strong>of</strong> identifying <strong>the</strong> optimal colors<br />

for a map. Application and testing <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> model shows that an optimally<br />

colored version <strong>of</strong> a map <strong>of</strong> English counties is searched 15% faster than<br />

<strong>the</strong> corresponding worst colored map.<br />

10:20–10:35 (305)<br />

Human Color Preference: WAVEs <strong>of</strong> Color, Culture, and Emotion.<br />

STEPHEN E. PALMER & KAREN B. SCHLOSS, University <strong>of</strong> California,<br />

Berkeley<strong>—</strong>Color preference is an important aspect <strong>of</strong> human<br />

behavior, but little is known about why people like certain colors more<br />

than o<strong>the</strong>r colors, or even why <strong>the</strong>y like colors at all. We report detailed<br />

measurements <strong>of</strong> preferences among 37 colors<strong>—</strong>people like blues and<br />

cool colors most and browns and olives least<strong>—</strong>and o<strong>the</strong>r relevant aspects<br />

<strong>of</strong> color perception. Models based on cone–opponent contrasts, colorimetric<br />

ratings, and color–emotion associations fit <strong>the</strong> results less well<br />

than do average ecological valences <strong>of</strong> color-associated objects, which<br />

account for more than 70% <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> group variance: People like colors<br />

strongly associated with objects <strong>the</strong>y like (e.g., blues with clear skies<br />

and clean water) and dislike colors strongly associated with objects <strong>the</strong>y<br />

dislike (e.g., browns with feces and rotten fruit). Fur<strong>the</strong>r results show<br />

that similar effects arise from social/cultural associations: People like<br />

colors strongly associated with <strong>the</strong>ir universities, favorite sports teams,<br />

and religions.<br />

10:40–10:55 (306)<br />

Visual Adaptation: Noise or Gain? ADAM J. REEVES & REBECCA<br />

GRAYHEM, Nor<strong>the</strong>astern University<strong>—</strong>We <strong>of</strong>fer support for both <strong>the</strong><br />

Weber law and <strong>the</strong> Rose–DeVries square-root law by using visual detection<br />

thresholds for brief large (Weberian) or tiny (square-root) incremental<br />

spots <strong>of</strong> light presented on steady light-adapting fields in Maxwellian<br />

view. Turning <strong>of</strong>f <strong>the</strong> field permits thresholds to drop almost instantaneously<br />

to a level predicted by <strong>the</strong> removal <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> photon-driven noise<br />

generated by <strong>the</strong> field. We conclude that photon noise controls visual<br />

threshold not just at absolute threshold (Hecht, Shlaer, & Pirenne, 1942,<br />

J. Gen. Physiol., 25, 819), but also in <strong>the</strong> light; <strong>the</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> noise accounts<br />

for <strong>the</strong> entire square-root law and half <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Weber law.<br />

11:00–11:15 (307)<br />

A.H.’s Persisting Visual Mislocalizations: Implications for Visual Updating<br />

and Visual Awareness. MICHAEL McCLOSKEY & EMMA<br />

GREGORY, Johns Hopkins University<strong>—</strong>A.H., a young woman with a<br />

developmental deficit in perceiving <strong>the</strong> location <strong>of</strong> visual stimuli, makes<br />

left–right and up–down reflection errors in a variety <strong>of</strong> tasks. For example,<br />

when an object is located on her right, A.H. <strong>of</strong>ten perceives it at<br />

<strong>the</strong> corresponding position on her left, or vice versa. Remarkably, A.H.’s<br />

misperceptions <strong>of</strong> location may persist when she moves her eyes. If she<br />

erroneously perceives an object to be on her left while looking straight<br />

ahead and <strong>the</strong>n makes a leftward saccade in an effort to fixate <strong>the</strong> object,<br />

she may report that she is looking at <strong>the</strong> object, even though <strong>the</strong> eye<br />

movement actually shifted <strong>the</strong> target to a more peripheral position in her<br />

visual field. We argue that A.H.’s persisting visual mislocalization errors<br />

have implications for understanding how visual representations are<br />

updated over time and what levels <strong>of</strong> representation give rise to visual<br />

awareness.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!