29.01.2013 Views

Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society — Volume 14 — November ...

Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society — Volume 14 — November ...

Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society — Volume 14 — November ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Friday Morning Papers 22–27<br />

We present research that focuses on language learning and inhibition<br />

in English monolinguals and English–Spanish bilinguals. First, monolinguals<br />

and bilinguals were taught artificially constructed words that<br />

overlapped with English orthographically but diverged phonologically.<br />

Native-language orthographic information presented during learning<br />

interfered with encoding <strong>of</strong> novel words in monolinguals but not in<br />

bilinguals. Next, monolinguals and bilinguals learned artificially constructed<br />

words that shared English phonology. While using <strong>the</strong> newly<br />

learned vocabulary, coactivation and inhibition <strong>of</strong> English were tested<br />

via eyetracking and mousetracking. The results suggest that bilinguals<br />

outperform monolinguals on novel word learning and recruit inhibitory<br />

control differently while processing language. These findings extend<br />

research on coactivation and inhibition in fluent bilinguals to new language<br />

learners and suggest that bilingualism changes how <strong>the</strong> cognitive<br />

system responds to linguistic input.<br />

Discourse Processing<br />

Back Bay Ballroom C, Friday Morning, 8:00–9:15<br />

Chaired by Gabriel A. Radvansky, University <strong>of</strong> Notre Dame<br />

8:00–8:15 (22)<br />

Aging and Mind Wandering. GABRIEL A. RADVANSKY, SABINE A.<br />

KRAWIETZ, & ANDREA K. TAMPLIN, University <strong>of</strong> Notre Dame<strong>—</strong><br />

Younger and older adults were asked to read <strong>the</strong> first five chapters <strong>of</strong><br />

Leo Tolstoy’s War and Peace. At various intervals (randomly sampled<br />

every 2–4 min), <strong>the</strong> participants were probed to assess whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y were<br />

mind wandering and to assess whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y could answer recognition<br />

questions. The results <strong>of</strong> this paradigm revealed that older adults mind<br />

wandered less <strong>of</strong>ten than <strong>the</strong> younger adults and were more accurate at<br />

answering <strong>the</strong> questions. These results are interpreted in light <strong>of</strong> current<br />

<strong>the</strong>ories <strong>of</strong> mind wandering and situation model processing and aging.<br />

Specifically, older adults have less working memory capacity and, <strong>the</strong>refore,<br />

have fewer resources to allow mind wandering to occur. Also, superior<br />

processing at <strong>the</strong> situation model level allows older adults to better<br />

engage in what <strong>the</strong>y are reading, leading to less mind wandering.<br />

8:20–8:35 (23)<br />

Establishing Global and Local Coherence in Discourse Processing:<br />

Evidence From fMRI. PAUL va n den BROEK, Leiden University, BEN<br />

SEIPEL & VIRGINIA CLINTON, University <strong>of</strong> Minnesota, EDWARD J.<br />

O’BRIEN, University <strong>of</strong> New Hamsphire, PHILIP BURTON, University<br />

<strong>of</strong> Minnesota, & NICOLE LANDI, Yale University and Haskins Laboratories<strong>—</strong>Maintaining<br />

both global and local text coherence is critical for<br />

successful reading comprehension. The present study used event-related<br />

fMRI to compare patterns <strong>of</strong> brain activation associated with reading<br />

narrative texts that were consistent relative to those that were inconsistent<br />

at ei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> global or <strong>the</strong> local level. Global inconsistencies evoked<br />

increased activation in <strong>the</strong> prefrontal cortex along <strong>the</strong> midline, an area<br />

associated in prior research with monitoring character motivation and<br />

action, as well as with conflict resolution. Local inconsistencies evoked<br />

increased activation in <strong>the</strong> precuneus. These results indicate that reading<br />

inconsistent (as compared with consistent) texts elicits unique neural patterns<br />

and, moreover, that <strong>the</strong>se neural activations are distinct for global<br />

versus local inconsistencies. The results also suggest that readers engage<br />

in different cognitive processes for building a mental representation at <strong>the</strong><br />

local and global text levels, respectively.<br />

8:40–8:55 (24)<br />

Regulatory Focus and <strong>the</strong> Accessibility <strong>of</strong> Goals During Reading.<br />

DAVID B. MIELE, Columbia University, & DAVID N. RAPP, Northwestern<br />

University (read by David N. Rapp)<strong>—</strong>A large body <strong>of</strong> work<br />

has demonstrated that information about a protagonist’s goals is highly<br />

accessible to readers while those goals remain unsatisfied; in contrast,<br />

this information becomes less accessible once <strong>the</strong> goals have been satisfied<br />

(perhaps as a function <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> decreased importance or relevance <strong>of</strong><br />

satisfied goals). Notably, <strong>the</strong> accessibility <strong>of</strong> everyday goal information<br />

appears to depend on whe<strong>the</strong>r an individual is predominantly focused on<br />

promotion or prevention concerns (i.e., seeking opportunities for gain vs.<br />

4<br />

protecting against losses). The present project examined whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>se<br />

regulatory concerns impact <strong>the</strong> accessibility <strong>of</strong> goals during text experiences.<br />

Participants read stories in which protagonists satisfied goals<br />

or postponed <strong>the</strong>ir completion. After each story, participants indicated<br />

whe<strong>the</strong>r a test statement matched <strong>the</strong> protagonist’s goal. Promotionfocused<br />

participants exhibited <strong>the</strong> standard pattern, with slower judgments<br />

to goal statements when <strong>the</strong> goal was satisfied than when it was<br />

unsatisfied; however, prevention-focused participants did not exhibit<br />

similar declines in judgment latencies for satisfied goals.<br />

9:00–9:15 (25)<br />

Does Rapid Talk Impose a Cost or a Benefit in <strong>the</strong> Referential Communication<br />

Task? CLAUDE G. ČECH & KYLE E. ALBARADO,<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Louisiana, Lafayette<strong>—</strong>In one version <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> referential<br />

communication task, directors describe tangram figures with <strong>the</strong> goal <strong>of</strong><br />

enabling matchers to select and arrange <strong>the</strong>ir figures in <strong>the</strong> same order.<br />

Fox Tree (1999) found that overhearers listening to tapes <strong>of</strong> interactive<br />

directors and matchers placed more figures correctly, despite <strong>the</strong><br />

fact that rate <strong>of</strong> speech was slower in <strong>the</strong> monologue condition, <strong>the</strong>reby<br />

presumably facilitating comprehension. She thus concluded that better<br />

performance was moderated by factors (e.g., multiple perspectives,<br />

discourse markers) o<strong>the</strong>r than speech rate. We explored an alternative<br />

claim that faster (but still normal) rates <strong>of</strong> speech may engage enhanced<br />

attentional processing, <strong>the</strong>reby partly accounting for <strong>the</strong> dialogue superiority<br />

effect. To test this claim, we looked at <strong>the</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> speech rate on<br />

accuracy within monologue and dialogue conditions.<br />

Associative Learning<br />

Back Bay Ballroom D, Friday Morning, 8:00–9:35<br />

Chaired by Russell M. Church, Brown University<br />

8:00–8:15 (26)<br />

Memorization or Rapid Relearning in a Multiple-Cued-Interval<br />

Task. PAULO GUILHARDI, Brown University and New England Center<br />

for Children, MARCELO S. CAETANO, Brown University and<br />

The John B. Pierce Laboratory, & RUSSELL M. CHURCH, Brown<br />

University (read by Russell M. Church)<strong>—</strong>The problem was to identify<br />

training conditions under which temporal intervals, signaled by different<br />

stimuli, are memorized (i.e., under stimulus control) or rapidly<br />

relearned. The task was a simple computer game in which a participant<br />

shot at a moving target, with feedback regarding hits and misses; this<br />

was a relatively engaging multiple-cued-interval task. The conditions<br />

were variations in <strong>the</strong> speed <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> target, <strong>the</strong> discriminability <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

stimuli, whe<strong>the</strong>r or not <strong>the</strong> target was visible, and <strong>the</strong> order <strong>of</strong> presentation<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> types <strong>of</strong> trials. The participants readily learned <strong>the</strong><br />

multiple-cued-interval task. The results were generally consistent with<br />

<strong>the</strong> principles <strong>of</strong> scalar timing and with a modular process model <strong>of</strong><br />

timing. Although responding was similar across conditions at <strong>the</strong> end<br />

<strong>of</strong> training, transfer tests demonstrated that <strong>the</strong> resulting intervening<br />

processes differed. The results with human participants were similar to<br />

those observed with rats.<br />

8:20–8:35 (27)<br />

Putting Names to Unfamiliar Faces: Similarity in Face–Name Associative<br />

Learning. ROBERT SEKULER & JIE HUANG, Brandeis<br />

University, & MICHAEL J. KAHANA, University <strong>of</strong> Pennsylvania<strong>—</strong><br />

How does perceptual similarity influence <strong>the</strong> learning <strong>of</strong> face–name<br />

associations? For answers, we used realistic syn<strong>the</strong>tic faces and monosyllabic<br />

Chinese names. The stimuli’s perceptual similarity spaces were<br />

defined by multidimensional scaling. Then associative recognition was<br />

measured with stimulus sets whose similarities were manipulated parametrically.<br />

In alternating study and test blocks, subjects studied a fixed<br />

set <strong>of</strong> face–name pairs and were tested with preserved and rearranged<br />

pairs. Over successive trial blocks, correct recognitions <strong>of</strong> preserved<br />

face–name pairs increased, whereas false recognitions <strong>of</strong> rearranged<br />

pairs decreased. Face and name similarity each strongly influenced associative<br />

recognition. These similarity effects were accommodated within<br />

a novel, hybrid model in which an interactive activation and competition

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!