07.04.2013 Views

Slips of the Tongue and Pen in Chinese - Sino-Platonic Papers

Slips of the Tongue and Pen in Chinese - Sino-Platonic Papers

Slips of the Tongue and Pen in Chinese - Sino-Platonic Papers

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1)<br />

complements <strong>in</strong> directional verbs such as & $, $, 3, etc., or<br />

resultative verbs such as j& (as <strong>in</strong> 6 @) <strong>and</strong> 7 (as <strong>in</strong> @ 5 T), etc.<br />

4.3. Ch<strong>in</strong>ese measure words, or classifiers, might be ano<strong>the</strong>r place to<br />

look for <strong>in</strong>structive errors. One <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ongo<strong>in</strong>g questions asked about<br />

classifiers is: To what extent are <strong>the</strong>y applied mechanically (like gender <strong>in</strong><br />

Romance languages) <strong>and</strong> to what extent do <strong>the</strong>y reflect deep cognitive<br />

aspects <strong>of</strong> human category boundaries? There is general agreement among<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guists that classifiers <strong>in</strong> most cases have some semantic function, <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong>re are ongo<strong>in</strong>g research projects <strong>in</strong> this area. For example, Tai & Wang<br />

(1990) have studied similarities <strong>and</strong> differences <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> classifiers<br />

across various Ch<strong>in</strong>ese dialects.<br />

Children's errors <strong>in</strong> acquir<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> classifier system <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese have<br />

already been used to support <strong>the</strong>ories about <strong>the</strong> relevance <strong>of</strong> classifiers to<br />

human conceptual mechanisms. Loke & Harrison (1986) <strong>and</strong> Clark (1977)<br />

have studied overextensions <strong>and</strong> misapplications <strong>of</strong> shape classifiers <strong>in</strong><br />

young children learn<strong>in</strong>g Cantonese or M<strong>and</strong>ar<strong>in</strong>, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>y have found that<br />

<strong>the</strong> stages Ch<strong>in</strong>ese children go through <strong>in</strong> master<strong>in</strong>g classifiers corresponds<br />

to <strong>the</strong> acquisition stages <strong>of</strong> shape concepts. One <strong>of</strong> Clark's f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs was that<br />

nearly all <strong>the</strong> criteria used by <strong>the</strong> young children <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>correct<br />

over-extensions <strong>of</strong> shape classifiers have correspond<strong>in</strong>g classifiers <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

languages with classifier systems. This would seem to imply that both<br />

classifier systems <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>g semantic system <strong>of</strong> children seem to<br />

have been cognitively based on <strong>the</strong> same universal pr<strong>in</strong>ciples govern<strong>in</strong>g<br />

categorization. That is, children's overextensions provide <strong>in</strong>formation about<br />

<strong>the</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong> organizational pr<strong>in</strong>ciples humans use <strong>in</strong> try<strong>in</strong>g to make sense<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> world. Mistakes <strong>in</strong> both children <strong>and</strong> adults <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g classifiers<br />

could be used to extend exist<strong>in</strong>g work on categories by researchers such as<br />

Rosch (1 973) <strong>and</strong> Lak<strong>of</strong>f (1 987).<br />

Allen (1977) br<strong>in</strong>gs up three ways <strong>of</strong> decid<strong>in</strong>g whe<strong>the</strong>r or not<br />

classifiers have a semantic function ( e . denote certa<strong>in</strong> perceived<br />

characteristics <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> th<strong>in</strong>g to which <strong>the</strong> associated noun refers). One is to<br />

use <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tuition <strong>of</strong> native <strong>in</strong>formants, suspect though it may be. A second<br />

is to use a foreign observer's <strong>in</strong>tuition about <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> noun classes<br />

revealed by classifiers - suspect though that may be. A third, more<br />

performance-oriented method, is to <strong>in</strong>troduce new words <strong>and</strong> objects to a<br />

number <strong>of</strong> native speakers <strong>and</strong> see what classifiers <strong>the</strong>y use with <strong>the</strong>m. To<br />

<strong>the</strong>se three I would add a fourth: To observe speech errors <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g<br />

classifiers <strong>and</strong> see to what extent <strong>the</strong> slips reveal systematic<br />

correspondences with category boundaries. For example:

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!