07.04.2013 Views

Slips of the Tongue and Pen in Chinese - Sino-Platonic Papers

Slips of the Tongue and Pen in Chinese - Sino-Platonic Papers

Slips of the Tongue and Pen in Chinese - Sino-Platonic Papers

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

SINO-PLATONIC PAPERS<br />

Number 22 March, 1991<br />

<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

by<br />

David Moser<br />

Victor H. Mair, Editor<br />

S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong><br />

Department <strong>of</strong> East Asian Languages <strong>and</strong> Civilizations<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

Philadelphia, PA 19104-6305 USA<br />

vmair@sas.upenn.edu<br />

www.s<strong>in</strong>o-platonic.org


SINO-PLATONIC PAPERS is an occasional series edited by Victor H. Mair.<br />

The purpose <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> series is to make available to specialists <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>terested<br />

public <strong>the</strong> results <strong>of</strong> research that, because <strong>of</strong> its unconventional or controversial<br />

nature, might o<strong>the</strong>rwise go unpublished. The editor actively encourages younger,<br />

not yet well established, scholars <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dependent authors to submit manuscripts<br />

for consideration. Contributions <strong>in</strong> any <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> major scholarly languages <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

world, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g Romanized Modern St<strong>and</strong>ard M<strong>and</strong>ar<strong>in</strong> (MSM) <strong>and</strong> Japanese, are<br />

acceptable. In special circumstances, papers written <strong>in</strong> one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> S<strong>in</strong>itic topolects<br />

(fangyan) may be considered for publication.<br />

Although <strong>the</strong> chief focus <strong>of</strong> S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong> is on <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tercultural relations <strong>of</strong><br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>a with o<strong>the</strong>r peoples, challeng<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> creative studies on a wide variety <strong>of</strong><br />

philological subjects will be enterta<strong>in</strong>ed. This series is not <strong>the</strong> place for safe,<br />

sober, <strong>and</strong> stodgy presentations. S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong> prefers lively work that,<br />

while tak<strong>in</strong>g reasonable risks to advance <strong>the</strong> field, capitalizes on brilliant new<br />

<strong>in</strong>sights <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong> development <strong>of</strong> civilization.<br />

The only style-sheet we honor is that <strong>of</strong> consistency. Where possible, we prefer<br />

<strong>the</strong> usages <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Journal <strong>of</strong> Asian Studies. S<strong>in</strong>ographs (hanzi, also called<br />

tetragraphs [fangkuaizi]) <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r unusual symbols should be kept to an absolute<br />

m<strong>in</strong>imum. S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong> emphasizes substance over form.<br />

Submissions are regularly sent out to be refereed <strong>and</strong> extensive editorial<br />

suggestions for revision may be <strong>of</strong>fered. Manuscripts should be double-spaced<br />

with wide marg<strong>in</strong>s <strong>and</strong> submitted <strong>in</strong> duplicate. A set <strong>of</strong> "Instructions for Authors"<br />

may be obta<strong>in</strong>ed by contact<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> editor.<br />

Ideally, <strong>the</strong> f<strong>in</strong>al draft should be a neat, clear camera-ready copy with high black<strong>and</strong>-white<br />

contrast. Contributors who prepare acceptable camera-ready copy will<br />

be provided with 25 free copies <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>ted work. All o<strong>the</strong>rs will receive 5<br />

copies.<br />

S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong> is licensed under <strong>the</strong> Creative Commons Attribution-<br />

NonCommercial-NoDerivs 2.5 License. To view a copy <strong>of</strong> this license, visit<br />

http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/2.5/ or send a letter to Creative<br />

Commons, 543 Howard Street, 5th Floor, San Francisco, California, 94105, USA.<br />

Please note: When <strong>the</strong> editor goes on an expedition or research trip, all operations<br />

(<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g fill<strong>in</strong>g orders) may temporarily cease for up to two or three months at a<br />

time. In such circumstances, those who wish to purchase various issues <strong>of</strong> SPP<br />

are requested to wait patiently until he returns. If issues are urgently needed while<br />

<strong>the</strong> editor is away, <strong>the</strong>y may be requested through Interlibrary Loan.<br />

N.B.: Beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with issue no. 171, S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong> will be published<br />

electronically on <strong>the</strong> Web. Issues from no. 1 to no. 170, however, will cont<strong>in</strong>ue to<br />

be sold as paper copies until our stock runs out, after which <strong>the</strong>y too will be made<br />

available on <strong>the</strong> Web.<br />

_______________________________________________


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 1991)<br />

1. Introduction<br />

Sli~s <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Ton~ue <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

David Moser<br />

Department <strong>of</strong> Asian Languages <strong>and</strong> Cultures<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Michigan<br />

Ann Arbor, MI 48 109<br />

In <strong>the</strong> West <strong>the</strong>re is a ra<strong>the</strong>r extensive literature on <strong>the</strong><br />

psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic implications <strong>of</strong> errors <strong>in</strong> speech <strong>and</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g. Psychologists<br />

<strong>and</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guists such as Victoria Fromk<strong>in</strong>, Charles Hockett, Gary Dell, David<br />

Fay, Anne Cutler, Donald MacKay, <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rs have for many years been<br />

classify<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> analyz<strong>in</strong>g various l<strong>in</strong>guistic errors <strong>in</strong> English <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

Western languages, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> study <strong>of</strong> errors now constitutes a ra<strong>the</strong>r lively<br />

doma<strong>in</strong> which is shedd<strong>in</strong>g new light on <strong>the</strong> various cognitive processes<br />

<strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> speech <strong>and</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Psychologists <strong>and</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guists have known for some time that slips <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

tongue can provide enlighten<strong>in</strong>g glimpses <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong> mechanisms <strong>of</strong> language.<br />

By study<strong>in</strong>g such errors, much can be learned about <strong>the</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d without<br />

undertak<strong>in</strong>g any formal experiments. And one advantage <strong>of</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g mistakes<br />

as one's w<strong>in</strong>dow onto <strong>the</strong> mechanisms <strong>of</strong> language <strong>and</strong> thought is that <strong>the</strong>re<br />

is an <strong>in</strong>exhaustible supply <strong>of</strong> fresh new data be<strong>in</strong>g produced every day.<br />

Speech errors <strong>of</strong> all k<strong>in</strong>ds swarm <strong>in</strong> our l<strong>in</strong>guistic environment like hordes<br />

<strong>of</strong> variegated <strong>in</strong>sects wait<strong>in</strong>g to be caught, labeled, <strong>and</strong> categorized.<br />

It is a pity, <strong>the</strong>n, that so far <strong>the</strong>re have been so few dedicated Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

"entomologists" who have undertaken to collect <strong>and</strong> analyze <strong>the</strong>se l<strong>in</strong>guistic<br />

"<strong>in</strong>sects". The l<strong>in</strong>guist Zhao Yuanren was someone who recognized <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>structive value <strong>of</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistic errors, <strong>and</strong> his writ<strong>in</strong>gs are filled with<br />

fasc<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g examples <strong>of</strong> grammatical anomalies <strong>and</strong> revelatory slips <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

tongue. But to my knowledge, virtually no systematic psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic<br />

research on errors has been done <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese.<br />

Speech errors occur <strong>in</strong> all languages, <strong>of</strong> course, but until now by far<br />

<strong>the</strong> bulk <strong>of</strong> speech-error research has been done <strong>in</strong> English. To my<br />

knowledge, only two studies have been done on speech errors <strong>in</strong><br />

non-Indo-european languages (namely, Arabic <strong>and</strong> Thai; see Abd-el-Jawad<br />

& Abu-Salim, 1987 <strong>and</strong> G<strong>and</strong>our, 1977). I have made a very modest start<br />

at collect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> analyz<strong>in</strong>g errors <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese that have fairly clear or even


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese"<br />

S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1)<br />

exact counterpart categories <strong>in</strong> English, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> first part <strong>of</strong> this paper deals<br />

with <strong>the</strong>se examples. It would be perhaps even more <strong>in</strong>structive to<br />

discover <strong>and</strong> classify errors that are specific to Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>and</strong> that have no<br />

clear counterparts <strong>in</strong> English or o<strong>the</strong>r Western languages. What is needed<br />

is a large corpus <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese l<strong>in</strong>guistic errors for more systematic study.<br />

Hopefully, careful analysis <strong>of</strong> such a corpus would raise some new issues as<br />

well as fur<strong>the</strong>r validate some <strong>the</strong>ories <strong>of</strong> error-mak<strong>in</strong>g that have become<br />

ma<strong>in</strong>stream over <strong>the</strong> past couple <strong>of</strong> decades.<br />

1.1. What <strong>the</strong> studv <strong>of</strong> errors can tell us<br />

A full discussion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistic <strong>and</strong> cognitive-science implications <strong>of</strong><br />

error-mak<strong>in</strong>g is beyond <strong>the</strong> scope <strong>of</strong> this paper, but <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g are just a<br />

few <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong> questions <strong>the</strong> study <strong>of</strong> errors can shed light on:<br />

Speech errors have been used to explore <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mental<br />

lexicon. How are words <strong>and</strong> phrases stored <strong>and</strong> retrieved <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d? To<br />

what extent are <strong>the</strong>y l<strong>in</strong>ked by semantic associations <strong>and</strong> to what extent by<br />

phonetic or grammatical aspects? Error researchers rout<strong>in</strong>ely use evidence<br />

from l<strong>in</strong>guistic phenomena such as malapropisms (e.g., Fay & Cutler, 1977)<br />

to deduce facts about <strong>the</strong> mental lexicon. One <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> most important<br />

current models <strong>of</strong> lexical storage <strong>and</strong> retrieval <strong>in</strong>volves <strong>the</strong> notion <strong>of</strong><br />

"spread<strong>in</strong>g activation", <strong>and</strong> several researchers study<strong>in</strong>g speech errors have<br />

begun to develop <strong>the</strong>ories based on this mechanism (e.g., Dell, 1986). (For a<br />

brief discussion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> notion <strong>of</strong> spread<strong>in</strong>g activation <strong>and</strong> its implications<br />

for speech errors, see H<strong>of</strong>stadter & Moser, 1989.)<br />

Speech errors have been useful <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> construction <strong>of</strong> performance<br />

models. How do <strong>the</strong> various low-level <strong>and</strong> high-level language processes<br />

<strong>in</strong>teract <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> flow <strong>of</strong> speech? To what extent are <strong>the</strong> higher-level<br />

semantic <strong>and</strong> syntactic processes <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> lower-level phonetic <strong>and</strong><br />

articulatory processes <strong>in</strong>dependent? Fromk<strong>in</strong> (1973, 1980), Dell & Reich<br />

(1980, 1981), Garrett (1980) <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rs have used evidence from speech<br />

errors to construct models <strong>of</strong> speech production. Nooteboom (1 980), Motley<br />

& Baars (1975), <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r error researchers have used phenomena<br />

observed <strong>in</strong> errors to argue for <strong>the</strong> existence <strong>of</strong> phonological, syntactic, <strong>and</strong><br />

semantic edit<strong>in</strong>g mechanisms <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> speech production.<br />

Written errors can provide w<strong>in</strong>dows onto <strong>the</strong> plann<strong>in</strong>g processes<br />

<strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> can also provide clues to <strong>the</strong> mechanisms <strong>in</strong>volved<br />

<strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g. How important a role does phonetic recod<strong>in</strong>g play <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1)<br />

process <strong>of</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g? To what degree is Ch<strong>in</strong>ese different from<br />

alphabetic languages such as English <strong>in</strong> this respect? What implications do<br />

<strong>the</strong> various differences between alphabetic writ<strong>in</strong>g systems <strong>and</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

script have for written language acquisition, read<strong>in</strong>g ability, <strong>and</strong> semantic<br />

process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> text? These issues have been dealt with at some length by<br />

DeFrancis (1984, 1989), Tzeng & Hung (1981) <strong>and</strong> Tzeng, Hung & Wang<br />

(1977). Chu-Chang & Loritz (1977) used errors on a short-term memory<br />

lexical-recall task to <strong>in</strong>fer that native Ch<strong>in</strong>ese readers process characters<br />

phonetically. At least one study <strong>of</strong> experimentally <strong>in</strong>duced slips <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pen<br />

<strong>in</strong> Japanese (<strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g Kanji characters) addresses some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se issues, as<br />

well (Nihei, 1986).<br />

* Cognitive scientists are also <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> error-mak<strong>in</strong>g because<br />

errors can reveal much about how people form concepts <strong>and</strong> categories,<br />

how <strong>the</strong>y make analogies <strong>and</strong> judgments, <strong>and</strong> how <strong>the</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong>terprets <strong>and</strong><br />

makes sense <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> world. Such error research is by no means limited to<br />

speech errors, <strong>of</strong> course. Psychologist Donald Norman (Norman, 1981,<br />

1988) has studied various k<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong> high-level "action slips" <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

implications for perception <strong>and</strong> cognition. Speech errors are <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

researchers <strong>in</strong> language <strong>and</strong> cognition because <strong>in</strong> error-free speech <strong>and</strong><br />

writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> various processes that go <strong>in</strong>to language production are <strong>of</strong>ten<br />

hidden; it is only when a tell-tale error crops up that all <strong>the</strong> compet<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>teract<strong>in</strong>g forces reveal <strong>the</strong>mselves, just as when a magic trick goes wrong<br />

<strong>the</strong> techniques <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> magician are revealed to <strong>the</strong> audience.<br />

2. S~eech errors (a is)<br />

A basic dist<strong>in</strong>ction between types <strong>of</strong> errors <strong>in</strong> English is one that is<br />

made <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, too: that is, <strong>the</strong> basic difference between a written error<br />

($$ iz) <strong>and</strong> a spoken error (a ) I will here deal with <strong>the</strong>se two<br />

classifications separately, although, as will be shown, many <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> same<br />

k<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong> errors appear <strong>in</strong> both modalities.<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g categories represent merely a sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

st<strong>and</strong>ard k<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong> errors that l<strong>in</strong>guists have noticed <strong>and</strong> studied for many<br />

decades, along with several equivalent or related examples from my small<br />

corpus <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese errors. The Ch<strong>in</strong>ese examples were collected by me over<br />

<strong>the</strong> course <strong>of</strong> a year or so, <strong>and</strong> I have had to rely on examples collected by<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese friends as well as those I collected myself. My corpus is still far too<br />

small (around 150) to be used for any systematic analysis, <strong>and</strong> thus this<br />

paper is as much a call for additional examples as it is a presentation <strong>of</strong> my<br />

own examples. I have provided tentative Ch<strong>in</strong>ese translations for <strong>the</strong>


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese"<br />

S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1)<br />

various error categories, as I am unaware <strong>of</strong> any already-exist<strong>in</strong>g<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ardized l<strong>in</strong>guistic error term<strong>in</strong>ology <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese.<br />

2.1. Anticipation errors (3 is)<br />

An anticipation error happens when a particularly salient segment<br />

about to be uttered masks (<strong>in</strong>terferes with or replaces) <strong>the</strong> segment <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

process <strong>of</strong> be<strong>in</strong>g uttered. For example:<br />

EX. 1. "Tomorrow we can expect partly *skoudy skies ... that is, partly<br />

cloudy skies."<br />

Here <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial /sk/ consonant cluster at <strong>the</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> "skies" masked<br />

<strong>the</strong> correspond<strong>in</strong>g segment <strong>of</strong> "cloudy" to produce "skoudy". The mask<strong>in</strong>g<br />

usually <strong>in</strong>volves <strong>in</strong>itial consonant sounds, but can <strong>in</strong>volve nuclear segments<br />

<strong>and</strong> f<strong>in</strong>al segments as well:<br />

9 9<br />

Ex. 2. "The *concleesions... <strong>the</strong> conclusions reached by <strong>the</strong> committee ...<br />

Ex. 3. "We can make certa<strong>in</strong> *assunctions... assumptions about this<br />

function."<br />

Ex. 4. "The two defendants were *<strong>in</strong>depentence... <strong>in</strong>dependently<br />

sentenced."<br />

Ex. 5. "...<strong>and</strong> for those who really believe we have become a *k<strong>in</strong>dler,<br />

gentler nation ... 77<br />

Anticipation errors are, <strong>of</strong> course, very common <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese as well.<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g three examples are ra<strong>the</strong>r straightforward ones <strong>in</strong> which <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>itial segment <strong>of</strong> a syllable is masked by <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial segment <strong>of</strong> a syllable<br />

about to be uttered. (The English translations I've provided usually do not<br />

attempt to translate <strong>the</strong> error.):<br />

EX. 6. "~h'ui ... j7$, 3 7 fi."<br />

"*~h'ui ... dbi, zh'ei lisng ge b'u x<strong>in</strong>g." ("Right, <strong>the</strong>se two are no good.")<br />

The <strong>in</strong>titial segment /zh/ <strong>of</strong> "zh\eiN (2)<br />

masked <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial segment /dl <strong>of</strong><br />

"d'uiY ($$).


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 1991)<br />

"~gige xi50 zh'angpeng li kgyi *s'ui...sh'ui s3nge re'n." ("Each small tent can<br />

sleep three people.")<br />

Here it is likely that <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial /s/ <strong>of</strong> "sZ<strong>in</strong>' (3) <strong>in</strong>terfered with <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

/sh/ <strong>of</strong> "sh'ui' (@).<br />

EX. 8. uR a zh6ng ... 2 M + a $ m."<br />

" ~ sh'i 6 *zh6ng...wZ sh'i c6ng ~h5nggu6 l6i de.' ("I come from Ch<strong>in</strong>a.")<br />

The <strong>in</strong>itial segment /zh/ <strong>of</strong> "zh6ngN (PI was anticipated <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> utter<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong><br />

"c6ngU (A). Dell (1984) has noted that repeated sounds <strong>in</strong> a speech plan<br />

are contributory causes <strong>of</strong> phoneme exchanges, anticipations, <strong>and</strong><br />

perseverations. (For example, a pair <strong>of</strong> words with similar vowel sounds<br />

such as "mad" <strong>and</strong> "plan" are more likely to give rise to errors than a pair<br />

like "mode" <strong>and</strong> "plane".) Given that <strong>the</strong> f<strong>in</strong>al <strong>in</strong> both "zh5ngN <strong>and</strong> "co'ng" is<br />

<strong>the</strong> same, this example may be an <strong>in</strong>stance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> phenomenon Dell is<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g about. (See also <strong>the</strong> English examples 3 <strong>and</strong> 4 above.) On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

h<strong>and</strong>, <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, <strong>the</strong> suprasegmental aspect <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> syllable (i.e., its tone)<br />

must be taken <strong>in</strong>to account <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> assessment <strong>of</strong> similarity - mean<strong>in</strong>g, for<br />

example, that two syllables with homophonic f<strong>in</strong>als but differ<strong>in</strong>g tones like<br />

"z h6ng" <strong>and</strong> "c6ng" may not be perceived by a native Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> same<br />

way a native English speaker perceives two English words like "wrong" <strong>and</strong><br />

"song". This would suggest a relatively higher frequency <strong>of</strong> errors such as<br />

<strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> which <strong>the</strong> mask<strong>in</strong>g syllable <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> target syllable share<br />

<strong>the</strong> same tone as well as <strong>the</strong> same f<strong>in</strong>al:<br />

% H A<br />

EX. 9. uR $11 7 X<strong>in</strong>g +... $ A I #<br />

"wZ dBole *~<strong>in</strong>ghu6... ~<strong>in</strong>ghuA ~'axu6 shlge x<strong>in</strong>gqi si.- ("I amved at Q<strong>in</strong>ghua<br />

University on a Thursday.")<br />

It seems possible that <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial <strong>of</strong> "x<strong>in</strong>g' ( masked <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tial <strong>of</strong> "q<strong>in</strong>g"<br />

, though an alternate explanation would be that <strong>the</strong> entire syllable,<br />

f<strong>in</strong>al <strong>and</strong> all, was anticipated. (It is perhaps impossible <strong>in</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>ciple to tell<br />

<strong>the</strong> difference.) I have run across a number <strong>of</strong> such errors, but my<br />

collection is still too small to draw any conclusions about <strong>the</strong> relative<br />

frequency <strong>of</strong> such error types.<br />

In <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g example it is not <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial consonant but <strong>the</strong> vowel<br />

that is masked by an anticipated phoneme:


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese"<br />

fi BBB $A<br />

T..."<br />

S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1)<br />

" ~ 6<br />

ycqi ..." ("It seems to me that <strong>the</strong>se three bits <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation are conta<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> English subjunctive...")<br />

jdde zhii s3nge &nxi shi t6ngshi baoh<strong>in</strong> z'ai Y<strong>in</strong>gyc de yige *fi... xT<strong>in</strong>i<br />

It seems likely here that <strong>the</strong> vowel <strong>of</strong> "ni" ($"A) masked <strong>the</strong> vowel <strong>of</strong> "xD"<br />

(B), but note that <strong>the</strong> first tone <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> target syllable XU is preserved. The<br />

vowel sound <strong>of</strong> "$" (7) may have contributed as well.<br />

In <strong>the</strong> preced<strong>in</strong>g examples, phonemes are masked by anticipated<br />

phonemes. In <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g example, an entire syllable is clearly replaced<br />

by an anticipated syllable:<br />

Ex. 11. "A 3 2 2 /L 3 & ...~)~. & @ 1 .<br />

"RQ~ jih shi zh\eme qi4g:i ... qigu'ai. G% r? z'ai t6n ba." ("People are funny that<br />

way. Let's talk about it later.")<br />

'A [g 7,<br />

The salient word "g'ii' (&) on <strong>the</strong> process<strong>in</strong>g stack masked <strong>the</strong> syllable<br />

"gu'ai"(~F), no doubt helped by <strong>the</strong> fact that both syllables start with a /g/<br />

sound. Note here that <strong>the</strong> mask<strong>in</strong>g syllable reta<strong>in</strong>s its proper tone ra<strong>the</strong>r<br />

than <strong>the</strong> tone <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> target syllable (which would have resulted <strong>in</strong> "qigki").<br />

If this were always <strong>the</strong> case, it would be evidence that <strong>the</strong> assignment <strong>of</strong><br />

tone takes place dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> same process<strong>in</strong>g stage as organization <strong>of</strong><br />

segmentals. But as will be seen <strong>in</strong> later examples, <strong>the</strong> tone <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

anticipated syllable is not always preserved.<br />

Indeed, one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> first questions that one might ask about such<br />

examples is, are <strong>the</strong> tones <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese subject to <strong>the</strong> same k<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong><br />

process<strong>in</strong>g errors as phonemes <strong>and</strong> segments? One might reasonably<br />

assume so, consider<strong>in</strong>g that tones play a dist<strong>in</strong>ctive phonological role <strong>in</strong><br />

tonal languages just as do vowels <strong>and</strong> consonants <strong>in</strong> all languages. Fromk<strong>in</strong><br />

(1980) notes that "One <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g aspect <strong>of</strong> speech error data is that no<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guistic unit, structure, or grammatical component seems to be immune[.]"<br />

She goes on to cite G<strong>and</strong>our (1977), who showed that tones <strong>in</strong> Thai are<br />

anticipated, perseverated, <strong>and</strong> reversed, just as segments are <strong>in</strong> English <strong>and</strong><br />

o<strong>the</strong>r languages. The follow<strong>in</strong>g anticipation errors provide some evidence<br />

that <strong>the</strong> same is true <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese:<br />

EX. 12. u~ 7 3 $$ $$yu'an ... fl a am..,,<br />

"TZ kgyi bGy'ao hi ti *y u'an ... r'enhe yuby<strong>in</strong> .' ("He doesn't have to give any<br />

reason whatsoever.")


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 1991)<br />

i6<br />

Here <strong>the</strong> segment yuan" is anticipated totally <strong>in</strong>tact, but carry<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong><br />

fourth tone <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> target syllable "rkn" ( ) (Note that, as this example<br />

shows, anticipations do not always occur on contiguous syllables. This<br />

suggests, as most process<strong>in</strong>g models hypo<strong>the</strong>size, that speech production is<br />

not based upon a simple left-to-right process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> successive elements, but<br />

ra<strong>the</strong>r <strong>in</strong>volves <strong>the</strong> process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> syntactic <strong>and</strong> phonological organizational<br />

units <strong>in</strong> several stages prior to <strong>the</strong> issu<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> low-level motor comm<strong>and</strong>s.)<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g is ano<strong>the</strong>r example <strong>of</strong> tonal anticipation:<br />

P+ /<br />

Ex. 13. "M 3 ~ fi Chang ... # #? ..."<br />

~ 6 n g zh'e ge &fang *ChSng... chgng'F<strong>in</strong>ji5 ...' ("From here, <strong>the</strong> Avenue <strong>of</strong><br />

Heavenly Peace.. .")<br />

It seems probable here that <strong>the</strong> first tene <strong>of</strong> "F<strong>in</strong>" (e) was anticipated <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> utter<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> "~hgng' (#).<br />

In <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g tonal error, <strong>the</strong> mask<strong>in</strong>g tone comes from a syllable <strong>in</strong><br />

a word that was active dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> lexical decision-mak<strong>in</strong>g stage, but not<br />

present <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> f<strong>in</strong>al sentence itself:<br />

Ex. 14. " fu % shgng ... a@."<br />

" ~ 8men xian *shgng... sh~ngli'ang ba." ("Let's first discuss it.")<br />

Accord<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> speaker's <strong>in</strong>trospective account (to which one must attach<br />

<strong>the</strong> usual caveats), <strong>the</strong> word "t~ol'un" (iTf ie) had been briefly considered<br />

<strong>in</strong> process<strong>in</strong>g, along with <strong>the</strong> semantically related word "s h3 n g liln g " (E<br />

H<br />

2 ), <strong>and</strong> it can be assumed that <strong>the</strong> third tone <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> syllable "tgo" masked<br />

<strong>the</strong> syllable "s h2ngN.<br />

In what might be considered even more conv<strong>in</strong>c<strong>in</strong>g pro<strong>of</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>dependence <strong>of</strong> tone <strong>and</strong> segmentals <strong>in</strong> process<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g utterance<br />

conta<strong>in</strong>s what seems to be a stray English syllable with a M<strong>and</strong>ar<strong>in</strong> tone<br />

affixed to it:<br />

Ex. 15. " ff, bye [clearly uttered with a fourth tone] ... [g, 8 !"<br />

\ ... \<br />

"H~O, bye ... uh, zail Ian !' ("Okay, bye ... uh, goodbye!")<br />

The speaker, experienc<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terference from English, uttered <strong>the</strong> first<br />

syllable <strong>of</strong> "bye-bye" (which would normally have an <strong>in</strong>tonational contour<br />

someth<strong>in</strong>g like:<br />

- './<br />

b y e-b y e


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese"<br />

or perhaps: - L<br />

bye-bye)<br />

S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 1991)<br />

us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> fourth tone borrowed from <strong>the</strong> character $(z'ai) as <strong>in</strong> "z'aiji'anY<br />

("goodbye").<br />

One might assume that errors <strong>in</strong> speech production <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g tone<br />

would occasionally give rise to tonal hybrids or splices that differ from <strong>the</strong><br />

usual suprasegmental characteristics <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese. However, <strong>the</strong>re is a ra<strong>the</strong>r<br />

well-accepted pr<strong>in</strong>ciple <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> field <strong>of</strong> errors known as Well's First Law<br />

(Wells, 1951) which says that phonemes appear<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> speech errors are<br />

always consistent with <strong>the</strong> phonological rules <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> language. One almost<br />

never f<strong>in</strong>ds a phoneme <strong>in</strong> a slip <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> tongue that is not heard <strong>in</strong> regular<br />

utterances. For <strong>in</strong>stance, a spoonerism <strong>of</strong> "spoon <strong>and</strong> fork" might result <strong>in</strong><br />

"foon <strong>and</strong> spork" but never "fpoon <strong>and</strong> sork". Similarly, it seems likely that<br />

<strong>the</strong> four tones <strong>of</strong> M<strong>and</strong>ar<strong>in</strong>, hav<strong>in</strong>g basically <strong>the</strong> same status as segmental<br />

phonemes, can be exchanged, perseverated, anticipated, etc., but would<br />

seldom appear <strong>in</strong> hybrid forms that are not part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> language. My<br />

modest corpus <strong>of</strong> errors conta<strong>in</strong>s no counter examples, but it is difficult for<br />

me as a non-native speaker to verify this conjecture personally.<br />

Sometimes <strong>the</strong> anticipation merely phonetically <strong>in</strong>fluences <strong>the</strong> word <strong>in</strong><br />

question ra<strong>the</strong>r than mask<strong>in</strong>g it:<br />

Ex. 16. ''k $ E % 5 5 a & zhzng 3 z<br />

n-J ??'<br />

m m m ,g,<br />

3 3<br />

"~~GrbZiqii<strong>of</strong>a bfishi ~~li6n de *zhzngshIiji... zGngshTIj1 ma?" ("Isn't<br />

Gorbachev <strong>the</strong> General Secretary <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Soviet Union?")<br />

The preparation for <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial retr<strong>of</strong>lex consonant <strong>of</strong> "shU" (3 ) <strong>in</strong>terfered<br />

with <strong>the</strong> pronunciation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial dental sibilant <strong>of</strong> "zzng' result<strong>in</strong>g -<br />

<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> syllable ''zhzng". Such an error probably does not result from any<br />

k<strong>in</strong>d <strong>of</strong> placement error <strong>in</strong> process<strong>in</strong>g, but ra<strong>the</strong>r is a result <strong>of</strong> much<br />

lower-level error <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> execution <strong>of</strong> a motor comm<strong>and</strong> after <strong>the</strong> sentence<br />

was already completely formulated.<br />

Garrett (1 975) identified four generalizations about speech errors, one<br />

<strong>of</strong> which was that <strong>in</strong>terfer<strong>in</strong>g or mask<strong>in</strong>g elements tend to come from<br />

correspond<strong>in</strong>g or isomorphic locations <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sentence; that is, beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g<br />

segments tend to mask o<strong>the</strong>r beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g segments, middle segments mask<br />

middle segments, etc. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, if two polysyllabic words are <strong>in</strong>volved


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 1991)<br />

<strong>in</strong> an anticipation, <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>terfer<strong>in</strong>g syllable(s) tends to mask <strong>the</strong><br />

correspond<strong>in</strong>g syllable(s) <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> target word, as <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g example:<br />

Ex. 17. "His open<strong>in</strong>g night *p erformous ... performance was an enormous<br />

success."<br />

"Performance" <strong>and</strong> "enormous" both have three syllables, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>terference <strong>in</strong>volves last two syllables <strong>of</strong> each word. S<strong>in</strong>ce it is <strong>of</strong>ten<br />

unclear what constitutes a word <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese (<strong>in</strong>deed, <strong>the</strong> notion <strong>of</strong> what<br />

constitutes a word <strong>in</strong> English itself is somewhat murky), it is <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

look at such generalizations <strong>in</strong> light <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese examples such as <strong>the</strong><br />

follow<strong>in</strong>g:<br />

"~'<strong>in</strong>zheng ~h~nggu6 de y'undbng ... xi'mg ~Bnhu'a Diy'ue ... ~6nhu'a ~Bge'rn'<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

~'aybejln ..." ("Anyway, <strong>the</strong> political movements <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>a, like <strong>the</strong> Cultural<br />

Revolution, <strong>the</strong> Great Leap Forward...")<br />

Does this example constitute evidence for or aga<strong>in</strong>st Garrett's<br />

generalization? It depends on how one chooses to break up <strong>the</strong> sentence<br />

<strong>in</strong>to words. If, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> m<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese speakers, "~Qnhu'a D\ag6m<strong>in</strong>gN<br />

("Great Cultural Revolution") is composed <strong>of</strong> two words (as it is usually<br />

written), <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> error seems isomorphic to <strong>the</strong> previous English error. If,<br />

however, <strong>the</strong> phrase is perceived as be<strong>in</strong>g composed <strong>of</strong> three words -<br />

"w8nhu'aN ("cultural") + "dB" ("big") + "g6m<strong>in</strong>gN ("revolution") - or if it is it<br />

simply one long polysyllabic chunk like "<strong>in</strong>terdiscipl<strong>in</strong>ary" or<br />

"right-to-lifer", <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> implications for Garrett's generalization are<br />

somewhat different. The psychological reality <strong>of</strong> word boundaries is one<br />

k<strong>in</strong>d <strong>of</strong> issue that can perhaps be fruitfully explored through a comparison<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong> speech errors Ch<strong>in</strong>ese speakers <strong>and</strong> English speakers make.<br />

2.2. Perseveration errors (g $ tz)<br />

A perseveration error is simply <strong>the</strong> opposite <strong>of</strong> an anticipation error:<br />

some salient segment or syllable just uttered <strong>in</strong>fluences or replaces a sound<br />

<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> be<strong>in</strong>g uttered. As is <strong>the</strong> case with anticipation errors,<br />

<strong>the</strong>re are many possible mask<strong>in</strong>g patterns:<br />

Ex. 19. "That was our English *leshon... lesson for today."<br />

Ex. 20. "I had to leave my car at <strong>the</strong> Detroit Metro *airpoit."


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese"<br />

S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1)<br />

Ex. 21. "If you th<strong>in</strong>k that while you're pregnant you can stay <strong>in</strong> tip-top<br />

*tape... tip-top shape, forget it."<br />

There seems to be general agreement that perseveration errors are<br />

less common than anticipations. Researchers such as Nooteboom (1973)<br />

<strong>and</strong> van den. Broecke & Goldste<strong>in</strong> (1980), look<strong>in</strong>g at English, German, <strong>and</strong><br />

Dutch, have noted this difference <strong>in</strong> frequency. The latter ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> that<br />

anticipations are <strong>the</strong> more common because <strong>the</strong> part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> utterance still <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> programm<strong>in</strong>g stage is more salient than what has already been uttered.<br />

Abd-El & Abu-Salim (1987), <strong>in</strong> contrast, found <strong>the</strong> frequency <strong>of</strong><br />

anticipations <strong>and</strong> perseverations to be almost equal <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir corpus <strong>of</strong><br />

errors <strong>in</strong> Arabic. Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, G<strong>and</strong>our (1977) found that tonal<br />

perseveration errors outnumbered tonal anticipation errors by a ratio <strong>of</strong><br />

about two to one <strong>in</strong> his collection <strong>of</strong> speech errors <strong>in</strong> Thai, <strong>and</strong> it is not clear<br />

whe<strong>the</strong>r this represents some idiosyncracy <strong>of</strong> his corpus or perhaps some<br />

aspect <strong>of</strong> tone itself. G<strong>and</strong>our notes that <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> tone languages <strong>of</strong> West<br />

Africa, perseverative tone rules (rules that spread tones from left to right)<br />

are more common than anticipatory tone rules (rules that spread tones<br />

from right to left), <strong>and</strong> he speculates that this asymmetry might suggest<br />

some built-<strong>in</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> speech mechanism which would also expla<strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> preponderance <strong>of</strong> tonal perseveration errors. I have been able to<br />

collect very few perseveration errors, tonal or o<strong>the</strong>rwise, <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, but<br />

this may merely reflect <strong>the</strong> modest size <strong>of</strong> my collection, or perhaps some<br />

<strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic difficulty <strong>in</strong> collect<strong>in</strong>g such errors.<br />

At any rate, <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g examples culled from my small collection <strong>of</strong><br />

perseverations highlight many <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> same issues raised by anticipation<br />

errors:<br />

EX. 22. "$& - + jhc gan ... 7 ~ . . . 9 9<br />

"T3 shi yige cTIn*gZn ... g'anb 'u..." ("She's a cadre from <strong>the</strong> countryside...")<br />

Presumably <strong>the</strong> first tone <strong>of</strong> "tun" (H) was persevered <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> syllable "g<strong>in</strong>'<br />

( Note, however, that <strong>the</strong> character also has a first-tone read<strong>in</strong>g as<br />

well (g3n). Though <strong>the</strong> error was uttered <strong>in</strong> spontaneous conversation, not<br />

while read<strong>in</strong>g a text, it is hard to say what k<strong>in</strong>d <strong>of</strong> a contribut<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>fluence<br />

such factors may have.<br />

Ex. 23. "2 # fR @ fl # dl"aomV<br />

"~h\ei jtan shi hgn n6n yblho *di\ao." ("This was very hard to predict.")


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 1991)<br />

(a The target was "y'uli'ao d'ao" # $11 ). The f<strong>in</strong>al <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> syllable "liBoN<br />

<strong>in</strong>terfered with <strong>the</strong> pronounc<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> syllable "d'ao'.<br />

Ex. 24. "G rn I%, & ych6ng...M lE rn % 5..."<br />

"ChT<strong>in</strong>tiSn de shihou, ch6ngzi *ch6ng... c6ng d6ngmi6n de zhu'mg hi...' ("In<br />

<strong>the</strong> spr<strong>in</strong>gtime, <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>sects, from <strong>the</strong>ir hibernative state...")<br />

This is ano<strong>the</strong>r ra<strong>the</strong>r straightforward example <strong>of</strong> one <strong>in</strong>itial consonant<br />

mask<strong>in</strong>g ano<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>terference no doubt helped by phonetic (<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g<br />

tonal) similarity.<br />

Sometimes <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> persevered segment merely alters <strong>the</strong><br />

target segment <strong>in</strong> some way (as <strong>in</strong> example15 above) ra<strong>the</strong>r than mask<strong>in</strong>g<br />

it completely:<br />

Ex. 25. "... 2 & - 6 sh& (@)rnm8"<br />

"...l~ob~ix<strong>in</strong>g yi h6ng zhb*sh'u (s'u) ..." ("...<strong>the</strong> common people were <strong>in</strong> a<br />

section <strong>of</strong> dwell<strong>in</strong>g places...")<br />

The <strong>in</strong>itial retr<strong>of</strong>lex stop <strong>of</strong> "Z hb " ( was perseverated <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

pronunciation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial dental sibilant <strong>of</strong> "s'u' (B), result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

syllable "s h'u' .<br />

In some examples, it is difficult to say whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>terfer<strong>in</strong>g<br />

segment is anticipated or perseverated:<br />

Ex. 26. "rn8rn# $ kk.. fi $z, % 7 $ Smy9<br />

"... d5u yZoqic k5o ~u~i*kc ... ~u~ifli, kso w6nle c6i qb.'<br />

that you first take <strong>the</strong> TOEFL test, <strong>the</strong>n you can go.")<br />

("...<strong>the</strong>y all require<br />

("~uafii- $g is <strong>the</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese transliteration <strong>of</strong> "TOEFL", <strong>the</strong> Test <strong>of</strong> English<br />

as a Foreign Language.) Most likely <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial /k/ <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> syllable "kgo' (g )<br />

masked <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial <strong>of</strong> "fc" ($@, but s<strong>in</strong>ce that syllable occurs both before<br />

<strong>and</strong> after <strong>the</strong> target, it is hard to say which one is responsible for <strong>the</strong> error.<br />

It is <strong>in</strong> fact very likely that both were contributors, s<strong>in</strong>ce a word that<br />

appears several times <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> process<strong>in</strong>g stack is obviously highly active.<br />

Nooteboom (1969) notes that unstressed syllables are not usually<br />

<strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> anticipation or perseveration errors. This seems to be <strong>the</strong> case<br />

<strong>in</strong> almost all <strong>the</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese errors I have collected, though perhaps <strong>the</strong>se<br />

suprasegmental considerations are somewhat more complicated <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

than <strong>in</strong> English, s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese (where much <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tonational resources<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> language is taken up <strong>in</strong> communicat<strong>in</strong>g semantics) stress usually


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1,<br />

must be accomplished partly by an <strong>in</strong>tensification <strong>of</strong> whichever tone <strong>the</strong><br />

syllable carries.<br />

2.2. Blends (2 @-


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 1991)<br />

a. scratched <strong>the</strong> surface<br />

b. <strong>the</strong> tip <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> iceberg<br />

Ex. 29. "She really stuck her neck out on a limb."<br />

a. went out on a limb<br />

b. stuck her neck out<br />

Ex. 30. "He's an *easy-go-lucky fellow."<br />

a. happy-go-lucky<br />

b. easy-go<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Blends can also occur at <strong>the</strong> word level. For example:<br />

Ex. 31. "This computer is completely *kapunct."<br />

a. kaput<br />

b. defunct<br />

Ex. 32. "Smok<strong>in</strong>g isn't some k<strong>in</strong>d <strong>of</strong> character *default."<br />

a. character fault<br />

b. character defect<br />

Ex. 33. "That was pretty *upsettl<strong>in</strong>g."<br />

a. upsett<strong>in</strong>g<br />

b. unsettl<strong>in</strong>g<br />

One <strong>of</strong> Wells' laws <strong>of</strong> speech errors states: "If <strong>the</strong> two orig<strong>in</strong>al words<br />

conta<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> same sound <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> same position, a blend <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>m will conta<strong>in</strong><br />

that sound <strong>in</strong> that position" (Wells, 1951). The previous example is just<br />

such a case, as is <strong>the</strong> "easy-go-lucky" example above.<br />

Various o<strong>the</strong>r examples <strong>of</strong> blends can be found <strong>in</strong> H<strong>of</strong>stadter & Moser<br />

(1989) <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> work <strong>of</strong> Gerald Cohen (e.g. Cohen, 1987)' whose collection<br />

conta<strong>in</strong>s thous<strong>and</strong>s <strong>of</strong> subtle <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g examples.<br />

One f<strong>in</strong>ds such blends <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, too, <strong>of</strong> course. I have found it useful<br />

to divide my examples <strong>in</strong>to two categories: word blends <strong>and</strong> phrase blends.<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g are just a few examples:<br />

2.3. Word blends (u & 2 #$ ig)<br />

Ex. 34. "~ @ #."<br />

"TZi j<strong>in</strong>gfi *wBngp\ei.' ("She's full <strong>of</strong> vitality.") a<br />

A blend <strong>of</strong>:


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 1991)<br />

a. a a w'angsh'eng ("vigorous, exuberant")<br />

b. 3 ch~ngp'ei ("full, abundant")<br />

Ex4 35. "@ ik I%! z? + 3P $55 hR $. #! % I3 EI?J .IE % 1I-J &<br />

$J & gJm'9<br />

#~gi &g t3 q\U j?ao n'eige gnniang l6i. TZi l&i de shihou z6nmen *s'<strong>in</strong>gd\ong<br />

szngd'ong .' ("We should get that girl to show up. When she gets here we<br />

can get someth<strong>in</strong>g go<strong>in</strong>g." Said by someone try<strong>in</strong>g to encourage a romance<br />

between one <strong>of</strong> his male friends <strong>and</strong> a woman he knew.)<br />

a. & ,g, sEngy6ng ("<strong>in</strong>stigate, <strong>in</strong>cite, abet")<br />

b. 8 $J gGd'0ng ("agitate, arouse, <strong>in</strong>stigate")<br />

The fact that <strong>the</strong> word "dkng" ($J ), mean<strong>in</strong>g "to move", is used <strong>in</strong> many<br />

o<strong>the</strong>r semantically-related comb<strong>in</strong>ations (such as "jid'ong ' (B 3 ), "excite<br />

or agitate", "h5ngd\ongM (@ Z3f 1, "to cause a sensation", <strong>and</strong> "c\ed'on g"(g<br />

"to stir up") helps give <strong>the</strong> utterance greater plausibility.<br />

a),<br />

Ex. 36. ''S g!"<br />

"Z~U d'an.' ("Get out!")<br />

a. A z'iuren ("Get out, leave.")<br />

b. 7% gcnd'an ("Beat it! Scram!")<br />

In <strong>the</strong> previous three examples, <strong>the</strong> two compet<strong>in</strong>g compounds are<br />

spliced ra<strong>the</strong>r neatly toge<strong>the</strong>r, with both <strong>the</strong> segments <strong>and</strong> tones <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> two<br />

uttered syllables left <strong>in</strong>tact. The follow<strong>in</strong>g example is somewhat different:<br />

Ex. 37. "3 - hu'a ."<br />

"~gng yi*hu'a." ("Wait a moment.")<br />

a. $$ - dgng yixih ("wait")<br />

b. - 2 )I dgng yihcir ("wait a moment")<br />

Note that, logically, a blend <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> segments <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> syllables "xi'a' <strong>and</strong> "huir''<br />

could have produced comb<strong>in</strong>ations such as " GUad " or " X i A ", but s<strong>in</strong>ce<br />

<strong>the</strong>se sounds violate <strong>the</strong> phonological constra<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> language, <strong>the</strong>y<br />

would never be generated. (Or seldom so. Hockett <strong>in</strong> his 1967 paper<br />

provides some counterexamples to Wells' First Law, <strong>and</strong> argues for <strong>the</strong><br />

blurr<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>of</strong> phonological constra<strong>in</strong>ts.) The previous example is one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

few Ch<strong>in</strong>ese blends I've been able to collect <strong>in</strong> which a syllable actually<br />

undergoes some sort <strong>of</strong> morphological transformation as a result <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

blend. This state <strong>of</strong> affairs is very common <strong>in</strong> English:


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 1991)<br />

Ex. 38. "That didn't bo<strong>the</strong>r me <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> *sleast.<br />

a. <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> least<br />

b. <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> slightest<br />

Theoretically possible but highly unlikely splices that would have left <strong>the</strong><br />

syllable boundaries <strong>in</strong>tact would be "slightleast" <strong>and</strong> "leastest", <strong>in</strong> which<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gful syllables or bound morphemes ("-est") are patched toge<strong>the</strong>r (a<br />

state <strong>of</strong> affairs that is similar to all <strong>the</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese blends above). But here <strong>the</strong><br />

result <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> blend is a mean<strong>in</strong>gless syllable with phonetic features <strong>of</strong> both<br />

compet<strong>in</strong>g words. (The "kapunct" example above is ano<strong>the</strong>r <strong>in</strong>stance <strong>of</strong><br />

this.) My guess is that this latter phenomenon is more common <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

than my collection would suggest. And note that <strong>the</strong>re are many examples<br />

<strong>of</strong> morphological change <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> anticipation <strong>and</strong> perseveration errors above,<br />

though those errors <strong>in</strong>volve <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>terference <strong>of</strong> a word already retrieved<br />

<strong>and</strong> wait<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> process<strong>in</strong>g stack, whereas blends <strong>in</strong>volve simultaneously<br />

retrieved words <strong>and</strong> thus perhaps a different process<strong>in</strong>g stage.<br />

On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r h<strong>and</strong>, <strong>the</strong>re may be facts <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese phonology itself that<br />

would affect <strong>the</strong> characteristics <strong>and</strong> relative frequency <strong>of</strong> such blends. For<br />

example, M<strong>and</strong>ar<strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese has only two f<strong>in</strong>al consonant sounds, /n/ <strong>and</strong><br />

/ng/ (<strong>and</strong> perhaps <strong>the</strong> retr<strong>of</strong>lex /r/ <strong>of</strong> Beij<strong>in</strong>g dialect), whereas English has<br />

a large number <strong>of</strong> f<strong>in</strong>al consonants <strong>and</strong> consonant clusters. We know also<br />

that Ch<strong>in</strong>ese is morphologically less complex than English; that is, <strong>the</strong>re are<br />

fewer morphemes <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> morphology <strong>of</strong> each syllable is more constra<strong>in</strong>ed.<br />

A Ch<strong>in</strong>ese syllable can have a maximum <strong>of</strong> only four segmental phonemes<br />

(plus one <strong>of</strong> four tones), whereas English can have up to seven phonemes<br />

per syllable (DeFrancis, 1984), <strong>in</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ations sometimes devilishly hard<br />

to pronounce for non-native speakers (e.g. <strong>the</strong> word "sixths"). Does this<br />

difference between Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>and</strong> English have any implications for <strong>the</strong><br />

production <strong>of</strong> blends <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r errors, or for <strong>the</strong> relative importance <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

different stages <strong>of</strong> speech production <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese? Though it is probably<br />

reasonable to assume that <strong>the</strong> overall process<strong>in</strong>g load for speech production<br />

<strong>in</strong> all languages is very nearly <strong>the</strong> same, that doesn't necessarily imply that<br />

<strong>the</strong> load <strong>in</strong>volved at each <strong>in</strong>dividual process<strong>in</strong>g stage (lexical retrieval,<br />

syntactic structur<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong>tonational contour, etc.) is equivalent across<br />

languages. A close look at <strong>the</strong> phonological regularities <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> blends<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir relative frequency <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> language could provide clues to such<br />

questions.<br />

2.4. Phrase blends


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese"<br />

S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1 )<br />

Ex. 39. " {$ iSt, @ fE - 8 8 ."<br />

"NY sh7iode gen *tianhua yiy'ang hgo.' ("What you said was beautifully<br />

* eloquent .")<br />

a. @ & 8 - @ g5n ti~nnc sznhua yifang ("like heavenly<br />

maidens scatter<strong>in</strong>g flowers")<br />

b. fE - # ff gen hua yiy<strong>in</strong>g h5o ("as good as a flower")<br />

The fact that this blend <strong>of</strong> two stock phrases happened to produce<br />

(z x,<br />

a<br />

genu<strong>in</strong>e (but here ludicrously <strong>in</strong>appropriate) word "ti n h u a"<br />

"smallpox"), is no doubt partly what enabled <strong>the</strong> mistake to slip past <strong>the</strong><br />

syntactic <strong>and</strong> semantic edit<strong>in</strong>g mechanisms <strong>in</strong> speech. Note also <strong>the</strong><br />

complex way <strong>in</strong> which elements from <strong>the</strong> two phrases were comb<strong>in</strong>ed to<br />

result <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> hybrid<br />

straightforward splice:<br />

utterance. The follow<strong>in</strong>g example is a more<br />

Ex. 40. u~ x<br />

a ~ 6<br />

2 a g fi f<strong>in</strong> a 5 g ~...~~<br />

dgi xiZn h g Sjr z'ai ~Ziguo' *yo'ur'enzirG de b'm xiG shi ..." ("I first have<br />

to skilfully manage some affairs <strong>in</strong> America...")<br />

a. @ 8 y6ur'bnyiiuyG ("h<strong>and</strong>le a butcher's cleaver with ease -<br />

do a job with skill <strong>and</strong> ease")<br />

b. $ f<strong>in</strong> zirc ("smoothly <strong>and</strong> easily")<br />

Though <strong>the</strong> second compet<strong>in</strong>g element here, "z>rc' ($ f<strong>in</strong>), could not have<br />

grammatically functioned by itself <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> utterance, it never<strong>the</strong>less was<br />

active <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> lexical retrieval as a compositional element <strong>in</strong> a<br />

class <strong>of</strong> phrases such as "b % w'ozir 6' ( $ $a, "easily master"),<br />

Ny\uny'ongzh? (5 )# $ $a, "use skillfully"), etc., so that <strong>the</strong> result<strong>in</strong>g<br />

blend is a very plausible utterance. Also, <strong>in</strong> keep<strong>in</strong>g with Wells' Third Law,<br />

which states that if <strong>the</strong> two orig<strong>in</strong>al words conta<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> same sound <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

same position, a blend <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>m will conta<strong>in</strong> that sound <strong>in</strong> that position, note<br />

that <strong>the</strong> compet<strong>in</strong>g phrases end with phonetically similar morphemes, both<br />

carry<strong>in</strong>g a second tone. Presumably, similar sounds can have <strong>the</strong> same<br />

k<strong>in</strong>d <strong>of</strong> contributory effect to a speech error that identical sounds can, <strong>and</strong><br />

obviously tone must be considered here as part <strong>of</strong> any metric <strong>of</strong> similarity.<br />

Ex. 41. "E g..."<br />

n~Bi wYo liii k'an..: ("AS 1 see it...")<br />

a. z'ki w6 k'm 16i ("As I see it...")<br />

b. j(;f<br />

i$&<br />

D U ~ W 1% ~ shu5 ("For me ...)


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1 !<br />

This is a good example <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> k<strong>in</strong>d <strong>of</strong> blend that is so smooth <strong>and</strong> subtle<br />

that it is immediately mentally corrected by both speaker <strong>and</strong> hearer, <strong>and</strong><br />

thus goes completely unnoticed.<br />

One <strong>of</strong>ten encounters blends <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g idioms, as well. For example:<br />

EX. 42. "3 3 8 $l= gmY9<br />

"~h\e jib sh) zh3o san m'u chc." ("That's be<strong>in</strong>g fickle.")<br />

a. $fl 3 Zh3o s3n rnb sh ('TO receive three portions <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

morn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> four <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> afternoon - play fast <strong>and</strong> loose, blow hot <strong>and</strong><br />

cold.")<br />

b. $fl @ Zh3o <strong>in</strong> m'u chc ("To serve <strong>the</strong> state <strong>of</strong> Q<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

morn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> state <strong>of</strong> Chu <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> afternoon - to be fickle.")<br />

Gibbs <strong>and</strong> Nayak (1989) have done experiments that suggest that <strong>the</strong><br />

idioms <strong>in</strong> a language do not form a unique class <strong>of</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistic items, but<br />

ra<strong>the</strong>r that some idioms are more "transparent" (that is, <strong>the</strong>ir semantic<br />

content is more obvious) <strong>and</strong> thus more syntactically flexible <strong>and</strong><br />

productive, while o<strong>the</strong>r idioms are more "opaque" (i.., more like dead<br />

metaphors) <strong>and</strong> cannot be syntactically altered without los<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g. For example, <strong>the</strong> sentence "Jane laid down <strong>the</strong> law" can be<br />

passivized to read "The law was laid down by Jane" <strong>and</strong> still reta<strong>in</strong> its<br />

orig<strong>in</strong>al mean<strong>in</strong>g, whereas <strong>the</strong> sentence "Jane kicked <strong>the</strong> bucket" loses its<br />

figurative mean<strong>in</strong>g when passivized to "The bucket was kicked by Jane".<br />

Such a fact might have implications for errors <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, a language<br />

with thous<strong>and</strong>s <strong>of</strong> four-character idioms (& g), many <strong>of</strong> which draw<br />

upon classical Ch<strong>in</strong>ese for <strong>the</strong>ir vocabulary <strong>and</strong> syntax, <strong>and</strong> which might<br />

thus be expected to be relatively opaque to many native speakers <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

language. Idioms that have semantically "fossilized" are probably less<br />

likely to mix <strong>and</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>e with o<strong>the</strong>r idioms <strong>in</strong> speech errors, whereas<br />

idioms that are semantically transparent or merely somewhat "moribund"<br />

(that is, <strong>the</strong>ir literal mean<strong>in</strong>g is <strong>the</strong>oretically available but not necessarily<br />

highly active) are more likely to be <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> blends.<br />

Look<strong>in</strong>g at blends, one can see a glimpse <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> compet<strong>in</strong>g concepts<br />

<strong>and</strong> words <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> production <strong>of</strong> a particular sentence. But <strong>of</strong><br />

course, sometimes one can see this process at work not because a blend is<br />

produced, but simply because <strong>the</strong> speaker tries several compet<strong>in</strong>g words<br />

before decid<strong>in</strong>g on one, as <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g:


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 1991 i<br />

+$ g q.,,<br />

"~<strong>in</strong>wdi shu6 shi BGi DB de d6u shi xi5 shi de pdngyoumen, me'iyzu ... b'u<br />

yidlng y'ao shhrne, w'ei le sh6nme ... y8u sh6nme h~ngd'ongsng ... sh<strong>and</strong>'ong ...<br />

sh<strong>and</strong>'ongx<strong>in</strong>g de zh'e zh8ng ycci." ("Because say<strong>in</strong>g all our friends at Pek<strong>in</strong>g<br />

University are poets doesn't mean... I don't necessarily want to ... I don't<br />

mean it to have some k<strong>in</strong>d <strong>of</strong> sensational ... <strong>in</strong>flam ... <strong>in</strong>flammatory tone.")<br />

(z<br />

"~5ngd'ong' 3, "cause a sensation") <strong>and</strong> "sh3nd'ong" (G 3, "<strong>in</strong>stigate,<br />

stir up") are words with similar mean<strong>in</strong>gs, <strong>of</strong> course, <strong>and</strong> one can hear <strong>the</strong><br />

speaker flounder<strong>in</strong>g around try<strong>in</strong>g to decide on <strong>the</strong> most appropriate word<br />

choice. No doubt o<strong>the</strong>r compet<strong>in</strong>g words were active, but did not appear <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> utterance itself. In a slightly more hurried or flustered context, this<br />

<strong>in</strong>decision might have resulted <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> same k<strong>in</strong>d <strong>of</strong> blend seen <strong>in</strong> some <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> examples above.<br />

2.5. Exchanges (a iz)<br />

Exchanges <strong>in</strong>volve <strong>the</strong> swapp<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> two phonemes or words, usually<br />

but not always with<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> same phonemic clause. For example:<br />

Ex. 44. "Imag<strong>in</strong>e own<strong>in</strong>g a *bet pat." (pet bat)<br />

Ex. 45. "He's cry<strong>in</strong>g because he just slammed his door <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> f<strong>in</strong>ger." (his<br />

f<strong>in</strong>ger <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> door)<br />

Ex. 46. "Did you remember to bike your lock?" (lock your bike)<br />

In errors <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> exchange <strong>of</strong> two whole words, <strong>the</strong> two words are<br />

almost always <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> same word-class, i.e., nouns swap with nouns, verbs<br />

with verbs, etc. (Hotopf, 1980, Nooteboom, 1973). The last example seems<br />

to be an exception to this, but note that both "bike" <strong>and</strong> "lock" can be ei<strong>the</strong>r<br />

a noun or a verb, <strong>and</strong> this is what enables <strong>the</strong> utterance to slip past <strong>the</strong><br />

syntactic editors <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> speech production.<br />

My examples <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese seem to adhere to this pr<strong>in</strong>ciple <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>variant<br />

word-class as well:<br />

EX. 470 ''3 % 1 Em"<br />

"Shii z\ai by shang ne." ("The book is on <strong>the</strong> pen.")<br />

Where what was meant was "BY 2ai shnshang" (% 3 1,<br />

"The pen is on<br />

<strong>the</strong> book"). <strong>and</strong> 3 are both nouns, <strong>of</strong> course. Exchanges <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 1991)<br />

different word classes - for example, noun-verb exchanges such as "BY ~6<br />

ggi' (3 e, "<strong>Pen</strong> me <strong>the</strong> give") for "~gi ~8 br' (g z, "Give me <strong>the</strong><br />

pen") - are <strong>the</strong>oretically possible but virtually nonexistent <strong>in</strong> everyday<br />

speech.<br />

In <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g example, <strong>the</strong> symmetrical prosody <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> phrase<br />

contributes to <strong>the</strong> error:<br />

Ex. 48. "5 3, fl a,"<br />

"M\ bb huan shzu, da b\u huh k6u.' ("If someone curses you, don't strike<br />

back; if <strong>the</strong>y strike you, don't curse <strong>the</strong>m back.")<br />

The target was ''M\ bb hu6n kh, d'i b'u hu6n shzu" (s a,<br />

T, literally, "If someone curses you, don't curse back; if <strong>the</strong>y strike<br />

you, don't strike back"), mean<strong>in</strong>g "not to return an <strong>in</strong>sult, to turn <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

cheek". The fact that "s h iju" (q) <strong>and</strong> "k zu" (a) share <strong>the</strong> same f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

makes <strong>the</strong> swap more plausible.<br />

Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, <strong>the</strong>re can sometimes be o<strong>the</strong>r factors <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sentence<br />

that give rise to an exchange, as <strong>in</strong> this example, where <strong>the</strong> exchange seems<br />

to be <strong>the</strong> result <strong>of</strong> an anticipation:<br />

EX. 49. ''8<br />

"Y~U<br />

sell electric fans?")<br />

am.. @?"<br />

m'ai *sh'<strong>and</strong>i'an... di\ansh\n de shangdtan ma?' ("Are <strong>the</strong>re stores that<br />

Here <strong>the</strong> fact that <strong>the</strong> word "sh2ngdi'anN (E , "store") was on <strong>the</strong><br />

process<strong>in</strong>g stack <strong>of</strong> words about to be uttered seems to have caused a<br />

syllable exchange <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> word "di\ansh\anM (e g, "electric fan") due to<br />

phonetic similarity.<br />

2.6. Substitutions (R Ts)<br />

Substitutions occur when <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>trud<strong>in</strong>g segment or word does not<br />

come from <strong>the</strong> sentence itself:<br />

Ex. 50. "Cutt<strong>in</strong>g beards requires special pliers." (mean<strong>in</strong>g "special scissors")<br />

Ex. 51. "You'll have to talk to <strong>the</strong> guy who waters <strong>the</strong> gardener."<br />

Here <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tended mean<strong>in</strong>g was " You'll have to talk to <strong>the</strong> guy who waters<br />

<strong>the</strong> flowers." The word "gardener", be<strong>in</strong>g also highly active, seems to have<br />

been substituted for <strong>the</strong> word "flowers", but <strong>the</strong> phrase could also be


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 1991)<br />

analyzed as a blend <strong>of</strong> "You'll have to talk to <strong>the</strong> guy who waters <strong>the</strong><br />

garden" <strong>and</strong> "You'll have to talk to <strong>the</strong> gardener". The follow<strong>in</strong>g is a<br />

somewhat isomorphic example <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese:<br />

EX. 52. L6 ap + 5 3 .?,<br />

"~'eige k3i siji de .. .' ("That driver..."; literally, "driver <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> driver")<br />

The word "siji" (3 fi, "driver"), seems to have substituted for <strong>the</strong> word<br />

"ch8" (q, "car"), but it is also possible that <strong>the</strong> phrase is merely a blend <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> words "k3ich8deN (R mjtJ, "<strong>the</strong> one who drives <strong>the</strong> car"), <strong>and</strong> "siji"<br />

(3<br />

Substitution errors are <strong>of</strong>ten <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g examples <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> phenomenon<br />

<strong>of</strong> spread<strong>in</strong>g activation, <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>ten reveal aspects <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> "semantic halo" <strong>of</strong><br />

associated concepts that surround each word. For example, <strong>the</strong> substitution<br />

<strong>of</strong> "pliers" for "scissors" above is hardly a r<strong>and</strong>om <strong>in</strong>trusion; <strong>the</strong> concept <strong>of</strong><br />

"pliers" is clearly a close neighbor<strong>in</strong>g concept to that <strong>of</strong> "scissors". (This<br />

issue is dealt with fur<strong>the</strong>r below. See Aitchison, 1987, Dell & Reich, 1980,<br />

<strong>and</strong> H<strong>of</strong>stadter, 1985 for more on <strong>the</strong>se ideas.)<br />

2.7. Errors <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g haplology (z i.)<br />

The phenomenon <strong>of</strong> haplology <strong>in</strong>volves <strong>the</strong> collaps<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> two<br />

successive words or phrases <strong>in</strong>to one, <strong>in</strong>variably with some truncation or<br />

loss <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation at <strong>the</strong> po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>of</strong> union <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> two elements:<br />

Ex. 53. "Here's Mozart symphony number *tw<strong>in</strong>e... twen ty -n<strong>in</strong>e."<br />

Ex. 54. "If you're will<strong>in</strong>g to go through some *momenterror... momentary<br />

terror, you'll be okay."<br />

This sort <strong>of</strong> process <strong>of</strong> "fusion" <strong>of</strong>ten occurs at a po<strong>in</strong>t where <strong>the</strong> two words<br />

share a common phoneme, as <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> last example, but this is by no means<br />

always <strong>the</strong> case.<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g is a similar error <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese:<br />

*kgng... hgn kgngng ch3ogub y> bzi y> ~giyu6n.' ("There's noth<strong>in</strong>g to worry<br />

about with <strong>the</strong> economy; if imports don't <strong>in</strong>crease, <strong>the</strong> trade balance will<br />

very likely be more than ten billion American dollars.")


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 1991)<br />

In this example <strong>the</strong> syllable "kg' <strong>and</strong> "n8ngm were simply melded <strong>in</strong>to one<br />

morpheme, "kgng', with <strong>the</strong> tone <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> first element <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> f<strong>in</strong>al <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

second left <strong>in</strong>tact.<br />

A special case <strong>of</strong> haplology is <strong>the</strong> phenomenon <strong>of</strong> "cannibalism"<br />

(H<strong>of</strong>stadter & Moser, 1989), <strong>in</strong> which two successive <strong>in</strong>stances <strong>of</strong> a word or<br />

syllable appear, <strong>and</strong> one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>m is "eaten" by <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r:<br />

Ex. 56. "Look! An MIT-shirt!" (MIT T-shirt)<br />

Ex. 57. "You ei<strong>the</strong>r have to ei<strong>the</strong>r (a) have a good job or (b) <strong>in</strong>dependently<br />

wealthy." (Mean<strong>in</strong>g "...or (b) be <strong>in</strong>dependently wealthy.")<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g is an example <strong>of</strong> cannibalism (g fl iz) <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese:<br />

("This k<strong>in</strong>d <strong>of</strong> car is very, very common.")<br />

Certa<strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong> haplology are so common <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese that it is difficult<br />

to say whe<strong>the</strong>r or not such cases constitute an error. For example, Zhao<br />

Yuanren (1968) notes that two successive <strong>in</strong>stances <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> particle "de" (m)<br />

are <strong>of</strong>ten reduced to one, as <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> case where someone asks "~h'e shi shei<br />

de kugngzi?" ("3 8 if$ &<br />

("a<br />

y?', "Whose basket is this?"), <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

reply is hi n\eige mki cbide" ap , A*<br />

1 &", "It's <strong>the</strong> vegetable<br />

seller's"). Logically, <strong>the</strong> response should be hi n'eige m'ai c'ai de de' ("3<br />

A* $p I & my'), but no native speaker ever says such a th<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

In <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g example, a woman was express<strong>in</strong>g concern over <strong>the</strong><br />

possibility that her husb<strong>and</strong> might have been on board a hijacked airplane:<br />

EX. 59. u~ 9 a st^ 3 a,%<br />

5 j<br />

~ l<br />

7-,,<br />

"~h: y%o bhh\ WE q~n'Bi de fGiji, wz jib fbngri<strong>in</strong> le.' ("As long as it wasn't my<br />

darl<strong>in</strong>g['s] plane, I have noth<strong>in</strong>g to worry about.")<br />

Logically, she should have said "...w6 q<strong>in</strong>J8i de de fGijT ...' 8 2 @J<br />

) (The particle 7 (le) is also subject to similar k<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong><br />

deletion due to haplology.) It might be hypo<strong>the</strong>sized that this phenemenon<br />

is partly <strong>the</strong> result <strong>of</strong> a monitor<strong>in</strong>g mechanism <strong>in</strong> speech production that<br />

balks at exact repetition; that is, a k<strong>in</strong>d <strong>of</strong> edit<strong>in</strong>g greml<strong>in</strong> that, when it<br />

encounters duplicated morphemes, says "This has already been said. No


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1 j<br />

need to say it aga<strong>in</strong>."<br />

These examples are perhaps <strong>in</strong>stances <strong>of</strong> a broader phenomenon <strong>in</strong><br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, namely a general avoidance <strong>of</strong> repetition <strong>of</strong> elements <strong>in</strong> compound<br />

nouns. Zhao Yuanren has noted this phenomenon <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> noun phrases<br />

he calls "telescoped compounds". For example, <strong>the</strong> word "1 ic xu 8s h8 n g ' (g<br />

&, "a student study<strong>in</strong>g abroad") is properly analyzed as a shortened<br />

form <strong>of</strong> "li~xu6xu~sh~ng' (a G *a &). O<strong>the</strong>r examples from Zhao are:<br />

human + study + study + society<br />

i.e. "Anthropological Institute"<br />

becomes<br />

%A<br />

~dnl\eixue'hu><br />

human + study + society<br />

WF$+$% becomes l%F$+?<br />

~'uji<strong>in</strong>b'u + b'uzhgng<br />

~'uj~nbbzhzng<br />

army + m<strong>in</strong>istry + m<strong>in</strong>istry + head army + m<strong>in</strong>istry + head<br />

i.e. "army m<strong>in</strong>ister"<br />

O<strong>the</strong>r examples that are perhaps questionable are:<br />

EX. 60. "F $ 2 # 'k @ Em" ('k @ @ g)<br />

"~'u shu2n zh'e zhzng ku'aic~ngukf' (ku\aicgn csngugn?)<br />

("I don't like this k<strong>in</strong>d <strong>of</strong> fast food restaurant.")<br />

Ex* 61* 6?!t <strong>in</strong> 1-7- EiZ M 2 z + 3% ?? z 3% 2z"(* z 3%<br />

% Z?)<br />

"~zmen sh'angge x<strong>in</strong>gqiti3n z'ai n'eige ~ianzhcji'aot6ng." (Ti~nzh6ji'ao<br />

ji'aodng?) ("We were at that Catholic church last Sunday.")<br />

Haplology is also a very common phenomenon <strong>in</strong> written errors, as<br />

will be seen later <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g section.<br />

3. Written errors (3 $$)<br />

Many error researchers (e.g., Ellis, 1979 <strong>and</strong> Hotopf, 1983) have noted<br />

that written errors <strong>of</strong>ten <strong>in</strong>volve <strong>the</strong> same k<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong> phenomena<br />

(anticipations, perseverations, etc.) as are observed <strong>in</strong> spoken errors. This<br />

is not surpris<strong>in</strong>g, given that speak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>volve many <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> same<br />

underly<strong>in</strong>g cognitive mechanisms. However, it is obvious that <strong>the</strong> tim<strong>in</strong>g<br />

conditions under which we speak are very different from those <strong>of</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g.


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1 j<br />

Given this, we would expect fewer lexical errors such as blends <strong>and</strong><br />

substitutions <strong>in</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g than <strong>in</strong> speak<strong>in</strong>g, s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>the</strong>re is simply more time to<br />

retrieve <strong>the</strong> proper word <strong>and</strong> to edit errors. But this longer time span<br />

between a word's retrieval <strong>and</strong> its writ<strong>in</strong>g gives rise to o<strong>the</strong>r k<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong><br />

errors that will be dealt with <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g sections.<br />

The hypo<strong>the</strong>sis that, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g, words are first<br />

retrieved <strong>in</strong> phonological form has never been contested, <strong>and</strong> it seems<br />

likely that this is true <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese as well, despite some controversy<br />

regard<strong>in</strong>g such th<strong>in</strong>gs as <strong>the</strong> relative extent <strong>of</strong> phonological recod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><br />

read<strong>in</strong>g Ch<strong>in</strong>ese characters vs. read<strong>in</strong>g alphabetic scripts. (See Hung &<br />

Tzeng, 1981 for a good overview <strong>of</strong> this issue.) As will be seen below,<br />

many <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong> errors that occur <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> English (<strong>and</strong><br />

presumably any alphabetic script) have analogous counterparts <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese,<br />

<strong>and</strong> this fact argues for <strong>the</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sis that <strong>the</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> both Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

script <strong>and</strong> alphabetic scripts <strong>in</strong>volve many <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> same mechanisms <strong>and</strong><br />

strategies <strong>of</strong> retrieval <strong>and</strong> execution.<br />

3.1. Substitution errors (fi & i2)<br />

I will deal here with three k<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong> substitution errors <strong>in</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g: (a)<br />

high-level semantic substitutions; (b) <strong>the</strong> substitution <strong>of</strong> a homophone or<br />

quasi-homophone <strong>and</strong> (c) substitutions as a result <strong>of</strong> capture errors.<br />

3.1 .a. Semantic substitutions<br />

As was mentioned earlier <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> section on substitutions <strong>in</strong> speech, <strong>the</strong><br />

substitution <strong>of</strong> one word for ano<strong>the</strong>r can <strong>of</strong>ten provide <strong>in</strong>formation about<br />

<strong>the</strong> semantic halo surround<strong>in</strong>g a particular word, as well as <strong>in</strong>formation<br />

about category boundaries <strong>and</strong> distances between concepts <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d.<br />

The fact that so many substitution errors <strong>in</strong>volve opposite concepts (e.g.,<br />

"In <strong>the</strong> case <strong>of</strong> a mo<strong>the</strong>r divorc<strong>in</strong>g her wife ... er, husb<strong>and</strong>...") or concepts<br />

from <strong>the</strong> same region <strong>of</strong> semantic space (e.g. "There's noth<strong>in</strong>g like that<br />

moment a football player crosses <strong>the</strong> f<strong>in</strong>ish l<strong>in</strong>e ... I mean, goal l<strong>in</strong>e...") is<br />

evidence for a spread<strong>in</strong>g-activation model <strong>of</strong> language process<strong>in</strong>g, which<br />

features active concepts spread<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>ir activation to o<strong>the</strong>r close<br />

neighbor<strong>in</strong>g concepts, one <strong>of</strong> those close neighbors be<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>of</strong> course, <strong>the</strong><br />

opposite concept.<br />

This is an area <strong>of</strong> error-mak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> which one would expect to f<strong>in</strong>d<br />

exactly <strong>the</strong> same k<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong> phenomena <strong>in</strong> both Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>and</strong> English (or any<br />

language), <strong>in</strong> that this doma<strong>in</strong> seems unlikely to have differ<strong>in</strong>g frequencies<br />

based on language-specific effects. I will not deal with examples such as<br />

<strong>the</strong> two given <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> previous paragraph, except to say that I have, not too


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 1991)<br />

surpris<strong>in</strong>gly, observed many such examples <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese. I will, however,<br />

give a couple <strong>of</strong> examples where <strong>the</strong> general mechanism <strong>of</strong> spread<strong>in</strong>g<br />

activation <strong>and</strong> specific facts <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> language both play a role <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

explanation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> error. Such an explanation is called for <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g<br />

written error:<br />

EX. 62. 4bR Z fth {IJ Z 8 Ej;;f 1% 71; E, a4: @ 7."<br />

"wG z\ai Eimen ji3 y6u shihou b\u xikg *chi kukzi.'' ("Sometimes, when I'm<br />

at <strong>the</strong>ir place, I don't feel like *eat<strong>in</strong>g chopsticks.")<br />

What was <strong>in</strong>tended was "shy ku\aiziN (E @ 7, "use chopsticks"), not "chi<br />

ku\aiziN (@' @ 7, "eat chopsticks"). Obviously <strong>the</strong> concept <strong>of</strong> eat<strong>in</strong>g was<br />

highly active dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sentence, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> slight phonetic<br />

resemblance <strong>of</strong> "chi' (6) <strong>and</strong> "shi' (@) was enough to cause @' to be<br />

substituted. The phonetic similarity <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> target <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> substituted word<br />

is also evidence for <strong>the</strong> phenomenon <strong>of</strong> phonetic decay <strong>in</strong> short-term<br />

memory, which is dealt with more below.<br />

Often an error seems to have several different contribut<strong>in</strong>g pressures,<br />

any <strong>of</strong> which might expla<strong>in</strong> it, as <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g:<br />

Ex. 63. ''I$<br />

$I] & @ {$ j<br />

"N? d'ao Zhiji3gB w6 ji'u ggi n? mzi er shuang.' ("When you get to Chicago I'll<br />

buy you two pairs.")<br />

d<br />

Here <strong>the</strong> person <strong>in</strong>tended to write - jlx, "one pair", <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>stead wrote ,<br />

XX, "two pair". One explanation is that <strong>the</strong>re was a sort <strong>of</strong> low-level<br />

motor-comm<strong>and</strong><br />

xx<br />

anticipation error <strong>in</strong>volved; that is, <strong>the</strong> fact that <strong>the</strong><br />

writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>volves a simple repetition <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> component x, <strong>and</strong> this<br />

"doubl<strong>in</strong>g comm<strong>and</strong>" was anticipated when writ<strong>in</strong>g -. (This sort <strong>of</strong> error<br />

is extremely common <strong>in</strong> English, as when one writes or types "leeter" for<br />

"letter".) Ano<strong>the</strong>r explanation is that <strong>the</strong> notion <strong>of</strong> "pair" spread activation<br />

to <strong>the</strong> concept "two", <strong>and</strong> this made it very easy for a slip <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> pen to<br />

.-.<br />

occur, mak<strong>in</strong>g , out <strong>of</strong> -. These two explanations are not necessarily<br />

compet<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>of</strong> course, <strong>and</strong> it is very likely that both contributed somewhat<br />

to <strong>the</strong> result<strong>in</strong>g error.<br />

3.1.b. Homophone (<strong>and</strong> quasi-homoohone) substitutions (n 8 78 3<br />

;z 1<br />

The substitution <strong>of</strong> a homophone <strong>in</strong> a written sentence is very<br />

common <strong>in</strong> English:<br />

@ m 9 '


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese"<br />

S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 1992)<br />

Ex. 64. "This goes aga<strong>in</strong>st *won's pr<strong>in</strong>ciples ..." (one's pr<strong>in</strong>ciples)<br />

Ex. 65. "...if we can get <strong>the</strong> problem *sordid out ..." (sorted out)<br />

Ex. 66. "If I were you, I *wouldent want it ..." (wouldn't want it)<br />

Ex. 67. "...unless she's an exceptionally bright *puplev (pupil)<br />

These errors seem to <strong>in</strong>dicate that <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g, words <strong>and</strong><br />

phrases are retrieved <strong>and</strong> stored <strong>in</strong> a k<strong>in</strong>d <strong>of</strong> short-term memory buffer (<strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong>ir phonological ra<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir graphological form). When <strong>the</strong> time<br />

comes to transfer <strong>the</strong>se words to <strong>the</strong> page, <strong>the</strong> writer <strong>of</strong>ten uncritically<br />

transcribes <strong>the</strong> sound <strong>in</strong> memory, result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a homophonic but<br />

<strong>in</strong>appropriate word or words. It is important to note that this process does<br />

not merely <strong>in</strong>volve <strong>the</strong> phonetic spell<strong>in</strong>g out <strong>of</strong> phonemes, but ra<strong>the</strong>r<br />

<strong>in</strong>volves <strong>the</strong> substitution <strong>of</strong> a pre-exist<strong>in</strong>g homophonic word or at least a<br />

plausible-look<strong>in</strong>g non-word. In o<strong>the</strong>r words, one never sees such th<strong>in</strong>gs as<br />

"surchiz" for "searches" or "digz" for "digs", but ra<strong>the</strong>r one sees ei<strong>the</strong>r a<br />

straightforward homonym substitution (as <strong>in</strong> "sordid" for "sorted"), or a<br />

slightly modified version <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> target word (such as "wouldent"<br />

"wouldn ' t" or "nonsen ts" for "nonsense").<br />

One also sees this phenomenon <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese:<br />

Ex. 68. ''g & $ $ 7 ' q *GI He'' (q<br />

for<br />

"~h'eli de z\llAi shuy kgyy yyny'ong.'<br />

"You can quote <strong>the</strong> tapwater here." (Should be "dr<strong>in</strong>k <strong>the</strong> tapwater".)<br />

Ex. 69. "8 2 flfl *g -& @ $ j@ 9..." z)<br />

"w; gu'o qb de pengyou yg yu'e 16i yu'e shgo ...' ("My former friends are also<br />

fewer <strong>and</strong> fewer...")<br />

The substitutions here are exact homophones, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>volve no semantic<br />

similarity whatsoever, which <strong>in</strong>dicates that <strong>the</strong> errors are truly <strong>the</strong> result<br />

<strong>of</strong> phonetic transcription <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> stored word, <strong>and</strong> not attributable to<br />

semantic factors. Of course, o<strong>the</strong>r factors can very <strong>of</strong>ten contribute to<br />

homophone substitutions, as <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g example:<br />

"N? m6i yiju qih, zhgod'ao g5ngzu'o zhi qi6n, y<strong>in</strong>ggsi ..." ("You haven't got


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1 )<br />

any money. Before f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g a job, you should...")<br />

Here <strong>the</strong> homophonic character (qi<strong>in</strong>,"money") was substituted for<br />

(qi6n. "before"), but <strong>the</strong> error was clearly facilitated by <strong>the</strong> fact that <strong>the</strong><br />

concept <strong>of</strong> "money" was already highly active <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> character for <strong>the</strong><br />

concept had just been written. The slip could also be classified as a<br />

straightforward perseveration error, but it is likely that all three factors -<br />

perseveration, spread<strong>in</strong>g activation, as well as simple homophonic<br />

substitution - contributed someth<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> result.<br />

There has been much debate about <strong>the</strong> extent to which speakers <strong>of</strong><br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese utilize <strong>the</strong> phonetic <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese characters <strong>in</strong> process<strong>in</strong>g<br />

or produc<strong>in</strong>g written text. DeFrancis (1984, 1989), Hung & Tzeng (1981),<br />

<strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rs have argued that, despite its reputation to <strong>the</strong> contrary, Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

script is by any def<strong>in</strong>ition <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> term a phonetic script, <strong>and</strong> native<br />

speakers can thus be expected to use <strong>the</strong> same k<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong> mechanisms for<br />

retriev<strong>in</strong>g, writ<strong>in</strong>g, or phonetically recod<strong>in</strong>g Ch<strong>in</strong>ese characters as speakers<br />

<strong>of</strong> English use <strong>in</strong> process<strong>in</strong>g English text. Much <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> evidence from<br />

written errors <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese also seems to support this view, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> previous<br />

three examples <strong>in</strong>dicate that <strong>the</strong> process<strong>in</strong>g strategies used by writers <strong>of</strong><br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese characters are similar to those employed by writers <strong>of</strong> English <strong>and</strong><br />

o<strong>the</strong>r alphabetic scripts.<br />

If a target word, once retrieved, waits <strong>in</strong> short-term memory long<br />

enough, very <strong>of</strong>ten <strong>the</strong> result is partial forgett<strong>in</strong>g or "phonetic decay" - a<br />

loss or degradation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> phonological <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> word<br />

(Hotopf,1983). The follow<strong>in</strong>g are a few examples <strong>in</strong> English from written<br />

texts:<br />

Ex. 71. "...particularly with this letter ..." (with this wea<strong>the</strong>r)<br />

Ex. 72. "I might as well tw<strong>in</strong> around <strong>and</strong> do it aga<strong>in</strong>." (turn around)<br />

Ex. 73. "Okay, so far, f<strong>in</strong>d." (so far, f<strong>in</strong>e)<br />

Here as well, we f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong> same phenomenon <strong>in</strong> written Ch<strong>in</strong>ese:<br />

EX. 74. "I$ **<br />

$$ UE,*q 2 8 A @ @I {$." (a E)<br />

"NY xian 16i ba, *kgd<strong>in</strong>g hbi y6u r6n b3ngzh'u n'i.' ("Just come, <strong>the</strong>re will<br />

certa<strong>in</strong>ly be someone to help you.")<br />

It seems clear that what was lost here <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> phonetic decay was<br />

<strong>the</strong> end<strong>in</strong>g consonant /n/ <strong>of</strong> "kgn' (R) result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> substitution <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese"<br />

S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1 1<br />

character (kg). It is also possible that <strong>the</strong> writer was subconsciously<br />

consider<strong>in</strong>g ano<strong>the</strong>r word such as kgneng ( @, "possibly"), <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

phonetic similarity <strong>of</strong> kgn <strong>and</strong> kg contributed to one be<strong>in</strong>g substituted for<br />

<strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r.<br />

As with many o<strong>the</strong>r earlier examples, <strong>the</strong> mechanism <strong>of</strong> spread<strong>in</strong>g<br />

activation can re<strong>in</strong>force or be a contribut<strong>in</strong>g factor to a homophone<br />

substitution:<br />

EX. 75. "{$ TOEFL 3 $# **ff fi,m9' (# 5 E)<br />

"~i kzo w<strong>in</strong> TOEFL y? hiu, h6i y'ao yzu s2n fGng *tu6jtan xln." ("After you've<br />

taken <strong>the</strong> TOEFL, you also have to get three letters <strong>of</strong> recommendation.")<br />

Even though here <strong>the</strong> word "TOEFL" was written <strong>in</strong> English, <strong>the</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

word for <strong>the</strong> term, "TUS~ fi' (R $$), was no doubt also highly active <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

writer's m<strong>in</strong>d (<strong>the</strong> writer may even have briefly considered writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong><br />

word <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese ra<strong>the</strong>r than English), <strong>and</strong> this resulted <strong>in</strong> a substitution <strong>of</strong><br />

(tuG) for (tui), no doubt helped along by <strong>the</strong> phonetic similarity<br />

between "tuTi" <strong>and</strong> "tui". Note also that <strong>the</strong> two characters share <strong>the</strong> same<br />

left h<strong>and</strong> component, <strong>and</strong> this also could have helped to push <strong>the</strong><br />

substituted character over a certa<strong>in</strong> threshold <strong>of</strong> activation, caus<strong>in</strong>g it to be<br />

written <strong>in</strong> place <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> target. (See <strong>the</strong> section on "capture errors" below for<br />

more on this phenomenon.)<br />

There are a surpris<strong>in</strong>g number <strong>of</strong> written substitution errors <strong>in</strong> which<br />

<strong>the</strong> substituted character is a homophone with respect to its segments, but<br />

<strong>the</strong> tone <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> character differs from that <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> target:<br />

Ex. 76. ''!$ nfi a - 8 ;% ~ *a $ @<br />

EIg a-mm" (& rx,<br />

"NT kgyi t<strong>in</strong>gcha yige sh'enzhi b<strong>in</strong>g m8iy6u chTI*xisn z'ai jhzi zh5ng de ci ...'<br />

("You can detect a word that didn't even appear <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sentence.")<br />

"ZhGn *xiZng, zhGn hao chi.' ("Really tasty, really delicious.")<br />

This phenomenon is to be expected given <strong>the</strong> fact that, as was mentioned<br />

earlier, <strong>the</strong> tones <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese play a role similar to that <strong>of</strong> vowels <strong>and</strong><br />

consonants <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r languages <strong>in</strong> dist<strong>in</strong>guish<strong>in</strong>g semantically dist<strong>in</strong>ct<br />

morphemes or words from one ano<strong>the</strong>r. In o<strong>the</strong>r words, <strong>the</strong> tone <strong>of</strong> a<br />

syllable is just one more <strong>of</strong> its <strong>in</strong>formational aspects that can be modified or<br />

degraded while <strong>in</strong> short-term memory. (This does not necessarily imply


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1)<br />

that <strong>the</strong> suprasegmental aspect <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> syllable <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> segmental aspect are<br />

<strong>of</strong> equal importance - just <strong>the</strong>y are is similar <strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>d.) And here aga<strong>in</strong>, it<br />

can be seen that, just as with correspond<strong>in</strong>g errors <strong>in</strong> English, <strong>the</strong> error <strong>in</strong><br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese is a result <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> phonetic transcription <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> phonological<br />

<strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> memory buffer.<br />

While it is certa<strong>in</strong>ly <strong>the</strong> case that Ch<strong>in</strong>ese speakers, like English<br />

speakers, employ text-process<strong>in</strong>g strategies that make use <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> phonetic<br />

<strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> script, it is by no means <strong>the</strong> case that both languages are<br />

phonetic to <strong>the</strong> same extent. As just one example, note that <strong>the</strong> phonetic<br />

<strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> an English word conta<strong>in</strong>s a great many clues as to <strong>the</strong><br />

pronunciation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> various segments <strong>of</strong> that word, whereas <strong>the</strong> phonetic<br />

<strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> a Ch<strong>in</strong>ese character at best merely codes for an entire<br />

morpheme <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> language. (The letters <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> word "bell" can be<br />

analytically broken up to represent phonemes thus: b + e + 11. But <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

correspond<strong>in</strong>g Ch<strong>in</strong>ese character, #$, <strong>the</strong> phonetic half <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> character (p)<br />

represents <strong>the</strong> entire syllable "Z h 5 n g ' .<br />

One important consequence <strong>of</strong> this difference is that, as an English<br />

speaker writes a sentence on <strong>the</strong> page, <strong>the</strong>re is a constant stream <strong>of</strong> visual<br />

phonetic feedback (albeit imperfect <strong>in</strong> nature) that is ra<strong>the</strong>r f<strong>in</strong>e-gra<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

<strong>and</strong> constant, whereas for a Ch<strong>in</strong>ese speaker <strong>the</strong> phonetic <strong>in</strong>formation is<br />

more sporadic (not all characters conta<strong>in</strong> phonetic components) <strong>and</strong><br />

coarse-gra<strong>in</strong>ed (i.., at <strong>the</strong> morpheme level ra<strong>the</strong>r than at <strong>the</strong> phoneme<br />

level). It is possible that this difference is reflected <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> relative<br />

<strong>in</strong>cidence <strong>of</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong> written substitution errors <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>and</strong><br />

English. For example:<br />

"First look at <strong>the</strong>se portraits ..." ("Draw<strong>in</strong>gs" was <strong>in</strong>tended)<br />

"As for what he said, I can easily separate <strong>the</strong> fact from <strong>the</strong> fiction." (The<br />

<strong>in</strong>tended phrase was "f<strong>in</strong>d it hard to separate fact from fiction".)<br />

Both <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se errors occurred dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> transcription <strong>of</strong> a tape, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

first example, <strong>the</strong> writer was actually say<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> word ''tGxi\angM (E f$)<br />

out loud as he was writ<strong>in</strong>g @ f$ (hu'axi'ang). The equivalent error <strong>in</strong><br />

English seems very unlikely; that is, someone say<strong>in</strong>g out loud <strong>the</strong> word<br />

"draw<strong>in</strong>g" while writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> word "portrait". The <strong>in</strong>formation given to <strong>the</strong>


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 1991)<br />

writer <strong>of</strong> English <strong>in</strong> visual phonetic feedback would very quickly tell <strong>the</strong><br />

writer that <strong>the</strong> letters <strong>in</strong> "portrait" could not possibly code for <strong>the</strong> sounds <strong>of</strong><br />

"draw<strong>in</strong>g". (That English speakers use knowledge <strong>of</strong> phonetic segmentation<br />

<strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g is generally accepted, though <strong>the</strong> extent to which<br />

this skill is present <strong>in</strong> all users <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> language is somewhat controversial.<br />

See Liberman et al, 1977.) By contrast, once <strong>the</strong> writer <strong>of</strong> a Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

sentence sends <strong>the</strong> motor comm<strong>and</strong>s to execute <strong>the</strong> character a (hu'a),<br />

<strong>the</strong>re is no phonetic <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> components <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> character to prod<br />

him or her <strong>in</strong>to an awareness that <strong>the</strong> sound <strong>of</strong> this character is not <strong>the</strong><br />

sound <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> desired character a (tfi). (There is evidence that speakers <strong>of</strong><br />

Japanese, whose written language uses a syllabic script along with Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

characters, do not develop a l<strong>in</strong>guistic awareness <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual phonemes as<br />

English speakers do, <strong>and</strong> this difference suggests that users <strong>of</strong> a script that<br />

represents phonemic <strong>in</strong>formation rout<strong>in</strong>ely- make use <strong>of</strong> this <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong><br />

read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g. See Mann, 1986.)<br />

A fur<strong>the</strong>r example: A ra<strong>the</strong>r common error <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese is<br />

<strong>the</strong> swapp<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> two characters <strong>of</strong> a two-character word, as <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g:<br />

Ex. 80. "...a a M $ ig $g 2 *$ $fl..." ($3 3)<br />

"...y<strong>in</strong>w8i w6 c6nglGi mdi d'aoguo *~u@niG...' (~iGyu@)<br />

"...because I've never been to York New ..." (New York)<br />

It has been my experience observ<strong>in</strong>g Ch<strong>in</strong>ese people writ<strong>in</strong>g that such<br />

errors are usually caught before <strong>the</strong> completion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> second character, but<br />

completed swaps such as <strong>the</strong> above are by no means uncommon. It is<br />

ra<strong>the</strong>r hard to imag<strong>in</strong>e <strong>the</strong> correspond<strong>in</strong>g error <strong>in</strong> English; that is, <strong>the</strong><br />

writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> "York New" for "New York". Such <strong>in</strong>tuitions argue for <strong>the</strong> view<br />

that <strong>the</strong> phonetic <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese script is less available to Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

speakers than <strong>the</strong> phonetic <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> English is to speakers <strong>of</strong> that<br />

language.<br />

Tzeng & Wang (1983) also speculate that <strong>the</strong>re may be some<br />

fundamental differences <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>and</strong> English pr<strong>in</strong>t.<br />

They suggest that "a reader <strong>of</strong> English cannot keep from apply<strong>in</strong>g a system<br />

<strong>of</strong> abstract rules to tackle <strong>the</strong> correspondence between letters <strong>and</strong> spoken<br />

segments, whereas a reader <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese automatically scans <strong>the</strong><br />

configuration <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> logographs" (p. 239). This difference would no doubt<br />

apply to writ<strong>in</strong>g as well as read<strong>in</strong>g. Note <strong>the</strong> k<strong>in</strong>d <strong>of</strong> English homophone<br />

error mentioned earlier, where a writer, apply<strong>in</strong>g an English spell<strong>in</strong>g<br />

heuristic, writes "shop tock" <strong>in</strong>stead <strong>of</strong> "shop talk". This sort <strong>of</strong> error is<br />

impossible <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese (unless one counts <strong>the</strong> substitution <strong>of</strong> a reasonable<br />

but mistaken phonetic component, such as writ<strong>in</strong>g fl for (both


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1 )<br />

pronounced "ji'iox~ng') but this does not <strong>in</strong>volve <strong>the</strong> same k<strong>in</strong>d <strong>of</strong> analytic<br />

piec<strong>in</strong>g-toge<strong>the</strong>r <strong>of</strong> phonemes as <strong>the</strong> English example). These differences<br />

once aga<strong>in</strong> suggest that <strong>the</strong>re are process<strong>in</strong>g strategies open to users <strong>of</strong> an<br />

alphabetic script that are closed <strong>of</strong>f, or are much less useful to, Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

speakers.<br />

Various scholars through <strong>the</strong> years, utiliz<strong>in</strong>g various metrics, have<br />

tried to rate <strong>the</strong> phoneticity <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>in</strong> comparison with o<strong>the</strong>r languages.<br />

Zhao Yuanren once (ra<strong>the</strong>r impressionistically) estimated that Ch<strong>in</strong>ese was<br />

about 25% phonetic, whereas English was about 75% so (Zhao, 1976).<br />

DeFrancis (1989), tak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>to account all <strong>the</strong> characters that have at least<br />

some phonetic value (if only to experts <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> language) concludes that it is<br />

reasonable to say that Ch<strong>in</strong>ese is actually about 90% phonetic. In ano<strong>the</strong>r<br />

study, Zhou (1978) found that <strong>the</strong> success rate <strong>in</strong> guess<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong><br />

pronunciation <strong>of</strong> a given character based upon its phonetic component was<br />

only 39%. There are various criteria used to assess phoneticity, <strong>and</strong> a full<br />

treatment <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> subject is quite beyond <strong>the</strong> scope <strong>of</strong> this paper. I br<strong>in</strong>g up<br />

<strong>the</strong> issue merely to suggest that perhaps a cross-l<strong>in</strong>guistic statistical<br />

analysis <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> frequency <strong>of</strong> various types <strong>of</strong> homophonic substitutions or<br />

errors <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> phonetic <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>and</strong> alphabetic scripts<br />

could provide evidence for <strong>the</strong> relative phoneticity <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

languages such as English.<br />

But <strong>of</strong>ten such evidence, like some <strong>of</strong> what is presented <strong>in</strong> this section,<br />

can be contradictory. For example, contrast <strong>the</strong> $3 E[ -5 swap<br />

above with <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g error <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g aloud a written text:<br />

Ex. 81. "F $ fl (m *ss'u..." g)<br />

a~Gy'i~ j~*ss'u ..." ("Don't feel uncomfortable...")<br />

where <strong>the</strong> speaker reported that <strong>the</strong> error was due to <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

phonetic I -ole <strong>the</strong> component (shb) plays <strong>in</strong> characters like 2 (sb) <strong>and</strong><br />

(st). So <strong>the</strong> question rema<strong>in</strong>s: do readers <strong>and</strong> writers <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese use <strong>the</strong><br />

phonetic <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir script <strong>in</strong> written language process<strong>in</strong>g or not?<br />

The answer may be that <strong>the</strong>re are many possible strategies open to Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

speakers for <strong>the</strong> process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> text, each <strong>of</strong> which is employed separately or<br />

<strong>in</strong> t<strong>and</strong>em with o<strong>the</strong>r strategies accord<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> particular l<strong>in</strong>guistic<br />

situation. Careful collection <strong>and</strong> study <strong>of</strong> various types <strong>of</strong> written errors<br />

could perhaps be useful <strong>in</strong> clear<strong>in</strong>g up some apparent contradictions <strong>in</strong> this<br />

area <strong>of</strong> research.<br />

3. I .c. Capture errors (fi & ig)


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese"<br />

S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1)<br />

Donald Norman <strong>in</strong>cludes capture errors <strong>in</strong> his categorization <strong>of</strong> various<br />

action slips (Norman, 1981). A capture error occurs when one action<br />

sequence smoothly switches over <strong>in</strong>to ano<strong>the</strong>r (usually more habitual)<br />

action sequence. This phenomenon occurs frequently <strong>in</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g:<br />

Ex, 82. "...<strong>and</strong> if you do this *enought, you will succeed." (enough)<br />

Here <strong>the</strong> familiar sequence <strong>of</strong> letters "ought" as <strong>in</strong> words like "thought" <strong>and</strong><br />

"fought" took over as <strong>the</strong> writer began <strong>the</strong> word "enough". O<strong>the</strong>r<br />

lower-level capture errors can occur <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual letters, as<br />

when <strong>the</strong> loop <strong>of</strong> an 'h' is <strong>in</strong>advertently closed to become a 'b', or when one<br />

starts to write an 'n' <strong>and</strong> ends up writ<strong>in</strong>g an 'm'.<br />

Given <strong>the</strong> relative complexity <strong>and</strong> diversity <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese script, one<br />

would expect. similar errors to arise <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> characters. The<br />

follow<strong>in</strong>g written error is a possible example:<br />

Ex. 83. "...gP $$ *% ~..." (P)S A)<br />

"...n'ei zhzng f&ng*guZ<strong>in</strong>g de zhi ..."<br />

"That k<strong>in</strong>d <strong>of</strong> light-resistant paper ..." (should be "flame resistant ")<br />

The person committ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> error reports that, once he had completed <strong>the</strong><br />

first two strokes ( \ <strong>and</strong> ) <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> target character ()A), a short vertical<br />

stroke ( 1 ) followed <strong>in</strong>stead <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> longer curved stroke () ), result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

component ( \I/ ), which is a much more common stroke configuration,<br />

appear<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> dozens <strong>of</strong> characters like , * ;f; , E, <strong>and</strong>, <strong>of</strong> course, x,<br />

which might have <strong>the</strong>n been selected over o<strong>the</strong>r c<strong>and</strong>idates because <strong>of</strong> its<br />

slight semantic <strong>and</strong> visual similarity to <strong>the</strong> target. (Note here, however,<br />

that <strong>the</strong> stroke order for <strong>the</strong> first three strokes differs <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> y< .)<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g is a similar substitution result<strong>in</strong>g from a capture error:<br />

Ex. 84. "{$ 7 fi & *& a@?'' (& !&, & 5)<br />

"N? bchu; y<strong>in</strong> c? e'r shGng WE de *shSng ba?' (shGng WE de q;) ("You<br />

wouldn't get mad at me for this, would you?")<br />

- The first three strokes <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> character 9 (namely ) , , <strong>and</strong> below that<br />

ano<strong>the</strong>r ) are basically <strong>the</strong> same as <strong>the</strong> first three strokes <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

character 7. This similarity, plus <strong>the</strong> probable perseverative effect <strong>of</strong><br />

hav<strong>in</strong>g just written <strong>the</strong> character , resulted <strong>in</strong> this capture error.<br />

3.2. Anticipations (E i.)


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1)<br />

Written anticipation errors basically come <strong>in</strong> two types: those that are<br />

a result <strong>of</strong> a process<strong>in</strong>g mistake <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> order<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> sentence, <strong>and</strong> those<br />

that are <strong>the</strong> result <strong>of</strong> errors <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> motor comm<strong>and</strong>s <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

The mechanisms <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> first type are usually ra<strong>the</strong>r high-level<br />

ones that only <strong>in</strong>cidentally <strong>in</strong>volve writ<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> issues raised <strong>in</strong> such<br />

errors are not much different from those raised <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>ation <strong>of</strong><br />

spoken anticipations. The second type is more low-level <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>volves <strong>the</strong><br />

character components <strong>the</strong>mselves, or o<strong>the</strong>r aspects <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g system.<br />

We will deal only with <strong>the</strong> second type here.<br />

One <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> most common k<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong> written anticipation errors is when a<br />

character component about to be written appears prematurely, usually <strong>in</strong><br />

part due to some visual similarity to <strong>the</strong> target component:<br />

3 9,<br />

Ex* 85- ''g * ($2 g ,%)<br />

"Zhgn me'i [yisi I" ("Really un[<strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g] .")<br />

The El component <strong>of</strong> g, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> component <strong>of</strong> ,g share <strong>the</strong> same<br />

rectangular outl<strong>in</strong>e (<strong>the</strong> first three strokes are exactly <strong>the</strong> same), <strong>and</strong> this<br />

caused <strong>the</strong> component a to be anticipated. Note that this state <strong>of</strong> affairs<br />

has someth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> common with a capture error, though a capture error does<br />

not typically <strong>in</strong>volve <strong>in</strong>terference from a word or shape already <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

- process<strong>in</strong>g stack wait<strong>in</strong>g to be executed.<br />

If a character component has to be repeated two or three times <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> a character, this can contribute to <strong>the</strong> component be<strong>in</strong>g<br />

anticipated:<br />

.J\<br />

Ex* 86. &&;ei ij IyJ 8 5 El ;fs 4% ,y<br />

"Xifang de y<strong>in</strong>yu\e ne, y6u xigo [tiq<strong>in</strong>]<br />

viol<strong>in</strong> I...')<br />

(.J\ t: F)<br />

...' ("As for Western music, <strong>the</strong>re's [<strong>the</strong><br />

The implicit knowledge that two <strong>in</strong>stances <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> component 5 were about<br />

to be written caused a premature motor comm<strong>and</strong> to execute <strong>the</strong><br />

component dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> $g.<br />

Ex. 87. "f&F ag,f& " (B @ 3<br />

"Ta bfich'mg, t3 [sgngzi I..' ("He didn't s<strong>in</strong>g, his [voice] ...")<br />

Similarly, <strong>the</strong> character component was anticipated as <strong>the</strong> writer began<br />

to write @.


I<br />

David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1)<br />

Ex. 88. '6. $J % fig ($3 &) ' $ jH)<br />

"...dSnx<strong>in</strong> di'aodsn ...' (tix<strong>in</strong> di'aodgn) ("to be on p<strong>in</strong>s <strong>and</strong> needles")<br />

Here <strong>the</strong> fact that <strong>the</strong> character $g was on <strong>the</strong> process<strong>in</strong>g stack caused one<br />

<strong>of</strong> its components, a, to be anticipated, probably helped along by <strong>the</strong> facts<br />

that (1) <strong>the</strong> result<strong>in</strong>g character is an exist<strong>in</strong>g character <strong>and</strong> @ &) is a<br />

word, thus enabl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> error to escape detection by <strong>the</strong> various visual<br />

feedback mechanisms; (2) <strong>the</strong> two characters (dan or d'm) <strong>and</strong> @ (dzn)<br />

are near homophones; <strong>and</strong> (3) <strong>the</strong> first five strokes <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> substituted<br />

component a <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> component a are exactly <strong>the</strong> same.<br />

Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Hotopf (1983), <strong>the</strong> programm<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> conversion <strong>of</strong> words<br />

<strong>in</strong> storage buffer to <strong>the</strong>ir graphological forms is usually done no more than<br />

one word ahead. It is <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to consider whe<strong>the</strong>r errors such as <strong>the</strong><br />

one above (<strong>in</strong> which <strong>the</strong> source <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>terference <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> target are four<br />

characters apart) constitute evidence for or aga<strong>in</strong>st this claim, s<strong>in</strong>ce first <strong>of</strong><br />

all, it is <strong>of</strong>ten unclear what corresponds to a word <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese (does <strong>the</strong><br />

above example conta<strong>in</strong> four words, two words, or perhaps one?), <strong>and</strong><br />

secondly, words <strong>in</strong> both Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>and</strong> English can have several syllables,<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>the</strong> word as a unit <strong>of</strong> time here is not particularly useful.<br />

3.3. Perseverations (j@ @ iz)<br />

Much <strong>of</strong> what has been said above about written anticipations also<br />

applies to perseveration errors. Perseverations can <strong>in</strong>volve words, parts <strong>of</strong><br />

words, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual letters:<br />

Ex. 89. "After thirty years <strong>of</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g *bath<strong>in</strong>g approaches ..." (both<br />

approaches)<br />

Ex. 90. "That's someth<strong>in</strong>g we don't *wan't..." (don't want)<br />

Ex. 91. "I can really a PP<br />

" (The target was "appreciate".)<br />

In example 91, <strong>the</strong> curved f<strong>in</strong>al stroke <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> 'r' cont<strong>in</strong>ued downward to<br />

become a loop due to <strong>the</strong> perseverative <strong>in</strong>fluence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> execution <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

previous two 'p"s.<br />

Some examples <strong>of</strong> this k<strong>in</strong>d <strong>of</strong> phenomenon <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese:<br />

Ex.92."z+G "(E fa)


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 1991)<br />

"~h'ege pyn [~hzng]~ (p<strong>in</strong>zhzng ) ("This [type] ...)<br />

The box which is written first <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> component (<strong>the</strong> right half <strong>of</strong> +$)<br />

was written as a square ra<strong>the</strong>r than a rectangle because <strong>of</strong> a perseverative<br />

<strong>in</strong>terference from <strong>the</strong> character 8.<br />

Ex. 93. "# @ fi @ 77 " (4s<br />

ta *<br />

H & $$)<br />

"TZi b\ei dg de bi ...' ("He got beaten [to a pulp]." lit., "nose black, face<br />

swollen")<br />

The bottom element <strong>of</strong> was perseverated <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> execution <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g strokes <strong>of</strong> s.<br />

3.4. Haplology (R $$ i2)<br />

Because <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> relatively greater length <strong>of</strong> time required to write out a<br />

word as opposed to pronounc<strong>in</strong>g it, errors <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>attention are common <strong>in</strong><br />

writ<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> this <strong>in</strong>attention most <strong>of</strong>ten results <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> omission <strong>of</strong> letters,<br />

usually <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> middle ra<strong>the</strong>r than at <strong>the</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g or end <strong>of</strong> a word (Hotopf,<br />

1983). This fact seems to suggest that errors <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g haplology would be<br />

among <strong>the</strong> most frequent <strong>in</strong> written errors. (An example is <strong>the</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong><br />

"<strong>in</strong>stition" for "<strong>in</strong>stitution", or, across word boundaries, <strong>the</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong><br />

"latestyle" for "latest style".)<br />

This sort <strong>of</strong> error can take several forms <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese. One <strong>in</strong>volves a<br />

simple omission <strong>of</strong> a character, which is certa<strong>in</strong>ly common enough. Ano<strong>the</strong>r<br />

<strong>in</strong>volves <strong>the</strong> omission <strong>of</strong> some character component, result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a hybrid<br />

nonsense character, as <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g example:<br />

Ex. 94. 6 6 m a a E bh $& & + 2 m m m Y 9 (bk $2 g)<br />

"...& bi[ji\ao gaol de c6ng &..' ("...at a [relatively high] level...")<br />

As was <strong>the</strong> case <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> section above on haplology <strong>in</strong> speech,<br />

cannibalisms (where one <strong>of</strong> two successive identical elements <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

sentence seems to "eat" <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r one) are very common <strong>in</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g as well,<br />

as <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> case where someone wanted to write <strong>the</strong> word "correct" <strong>in</strong><br />

paren<strong>the</strong>ses <strong>and</strong> ended up writ<strong>in</strong>g "(orrect)" <strong>in</strong>stead <strong>of</strong> "(correct)".<br />

Apparently, <strong>the</strong> just-drawn paren<strong>the</strong>sis was perceived by <strong>the</strong> visual<br />

feedback mechanism as an abstract shape - a crescent - <strong>and</strong> this<br />

perception <strong>in</strong>terfered with <strong>the</strong> arriv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>structions to draw ano<strong>the</strong>r<br />

crescent shape (<strong>the</strong> letter 'c'). Some o<strong>the</strong>r examples:


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1)<br />

Ex. 95. "What is so English about *Englis humor?" (English humor)<br />

Ex. 96. "... 5 econds ..." (5 seconds)<br />

Ano<strong>the</strong>r example <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese is <strong>the</strong> case where someone wanted to<br />

94-<br />

write <strong>the</strong> word (~gngyuhn), "Party member", <strong>and</strong> found <strong>the</strong>mselves<br />

writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> character (sh'<strong>in</strong>g, "to grant or bestow"). Because both 2<br />

<strong>and</strong> 3 share <strong>the</strong> element a, <strong>the</strong> 5 character was able to "cannibalize"<br />

<strong>the</strong> 2 character, result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a smooth meld<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> two.<br />

4. Possible areas for fur<strong>the</strong>r research<br />

4.1. Errors <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> four tones <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese can shed light on<br />

various psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic <strong>and</strong> psychoacoustic aspects <strong>of</strong> tones. As I have<br />

said many times <strong>in</strong> this paper, <strong>the</strong> tones <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese play much <strong>the</strong> same<br />

role as do phonemes, but to what extent do <strong>the</strong>se roles differ? And to what<br />

extent are <strong>the</strong> tones <strong>and</strong> segments processed separately dur<strong>in</strong>g speech<br />

production? Li & Thompson (1977), <strong>in</strong> study<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> acquisition <strong>of</strong> tone <strong>in</strong><br />

children's speech, found that <strong>the</strong> tone system is mastered well <strong>in</strong> advance<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> segmental system. Focus<strong>in</strong>g on errors <strong>in</strong> which tones are swapped,<br />

anticipated, or o<strong>the</strong>rwise behave <strong>in</strong>dependently <strong>of</strong> segments might provide<br />

clues to <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>terdependence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> segmentals <strong>and</strong> suprasegmentals <strong>in</strong><br />

speech process<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

In <strong>the</strong> same study by Li & Thompson, it was found that <strong>the</strong> high <strong>and</strong><br />

fall<strong>in</strong>g tones (first <strong>and</strong> fourth, respectively) are mastered earlier <strong>and</strong> with<br />

less difficulty than <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r two tones. Kiril<strong>of</strong>f (1969) also reported that<br />

adult native speakers were more likely to confuse tones two <strong>and</strong> three.<br />

What o<strong>the</strong>r <strong>in</strong>herent psychoacoustic process<strong>in</strong>g difficulties are <strong>in</strong>volved<br />

with <strong>the</strong> tonal system? A careful study <strong>of</strong> tone errors <strong>in</strong> both adults <strong>and</strong><br />

children might provide anwers to such questions.<br />

G<strong>and</strong>our (1977) studied tone errors committed by Thai bil<strong>in</strong>guals that<br />

were partly <strong>the</strong> result <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>terference from a second dialect. Such evidence<br />

from speech errors might be useful for <strong>the</strong> study <strong>of</strong> dialects <strong>and</strong><br />

second-language acquisition, as well as for <strong>the</strong> study <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic<br />

implications <strong>of</strong> bil<strong>in</strong>gualism <strong>in</strong> general.<br />

The study <strong>of</strong> tone errors might provide new <strong>in</strong>sights <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong><br />

acquisition <strong>and</strong> application <strong>of</strong> tone s<strong>and</strong>hi rules <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r tonal<br />

languages. Though I have not yet observed any errors <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g tone<br />

s<strong>and</strong>hi, it is not unreasonable to suspect that such errors exist, <strong>and</strong> research<br />

on such topics could be built on such speech-error phenomena as<br />

"accommodation". Accommodation can be illustrated by <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1)<br />

example:<br />

Ex. 97. "Put <strong>the</strong> heaviers book on top." (Put <strong>the</strong> heavier books on top.)<br />

Here <strong>the</strong> grammatical morpheme /s/ was simply affixed to <strong>the</strong> wrong word.<br />

Had <strong>the</strong> /s/ come at <strong>the</strong> end <strong>of</strong> "books" as it was supposed to, it would have<br />

been unvoiced, but <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> utterance it was voiced; <strong>the</strong> 1st "accomodated" to<br />

its l<strong>in</strong>guistic environment. This <strong>of</strong> course suggests that <strong>the</strong> misplacement<br />

error occurred at an earlier stage <strong>in</strong> process<strong>in</strong>g before <strong>the</strong> phonetic<br />

representation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> utterance took shape, <strong>and</strong> thus provides clues to <strong>the</strong><br />

order <strong>in</strong> which various grammatical <strong>and</strong> phonetic aspects <strong>of</strong> speech are<br />

processed (Garrett, 1980). One might look for similar accommodation<br />

effects <strong>in</strong> errors where tone s<strong>and</strong>hi is <strong>in</strong>volved. For example, take a<br />

hypo<strong>the</strong>tical error <strong>in</strong> which <strong>the</strong> sentence<br />

Ex. 98. ''& 1: 8 #,."<br />

"Zhu5zi shang y6u zhi." ("There's paper on <strong>the</strong> table.")<br />

is transformed <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong> sentence<br />

Ex. 99. ''3 8 y."<br />

"~h? shang y6u zhu5.ziN ("There's table on <strong>the</strong> paper.")<br />

by <strong>the</strong> error <strong>of</strong> simple exchange. Note that, accord<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> tone s<strong>and</strong>hi<br />

rule that <strong>in</strong> two consecutive third tones <strong>the</strong> first one changes to a second<br />

tone, <strong>the</strong> tones <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> error-free sentence should be "Zhu5zi shang y6u zhr";<br />

that is, <strong>the</strong> third tone <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> character 8 ( yh) changes to second tone. But<br />

<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> sentence where <strong>the</strong> exchange error takes place, <strong>the</strong> syllable "Y~u" is<br />

no longer followed by ano<strong>the</strong>r third tone, but ra<strong>the</strong>r by <strong>the</strong> first tone <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

character (z~uG), <strong>and</strong> thus is not subject to a tone s<strong>and</strong>hi change. If, <strong>in</strong><br />

such an error, <strong>the</strong> sentence was uttered as hi shang y'iu zhu5ziN, this<br />

would be an <strong>in</strong>stance <strong>of</strong> accommodation similar to <strong>the</strong> English example<br />

above. If, on <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r h<strong>and</strong>, such an error were ever to be uttered as hi<br />

shang y6u zhu5ziN, it would constitute evidence that <strong>the</strong> suprasegmental<br />

tones are processed prior to (or at least <strong>in</strong>dependently <strong>of</strong>) phonemes.<br />

Suprasegmental aspects <strong>of</strong> language such as <strong>in</strong>tonation <strong>and</strong> stress are<br />

much-slighted <strong>and</strong> somewhat poorly understood areas <strong>of</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistic<br />

research, <strong>and</strong> it would be worthwhile to <strong>in</strong>vestigate evidence from tone<br />

errors <strong>in</strong> order to ga<strong>in</strong> fresh <strong>in</strong>sight <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong>se topics. The fact that a<br />

greater percentage <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tonational resources <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese are utilized <strong>in</strong><br />

mak<strong>in</strong>g semantic dist<strong>in</strong>ctions might call for much reth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> exist<strong>in</strong>g


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1)<br />

<strong>the</strong>ories about <strong>in</strong>tonation, but no doubt a lot <strong>of</strong> new research could be built<br />

upon excellent work that has already been done <strong>in</strong> English by Cutler (1980)<br />

<strong>and</strong> Bol<strong>in</strong>ger (1986).<br />

4.2. As has been mentioned above, <strong>the</strong> study <strong>of</strong> errors has been used<br />

to provide evidence for <strong>the</strong>ories about <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> mental lexicon. For<br />

example, Fromk<strong>in</strong> (1973) <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rs have noted that <strong>in</strong> exchange errors,<br />

grammatical morphemes <strong>of</strong>ten get left beh<strong>in</strong>d <strong>and</strong> affixed to <strong>the</strong><br />

substituted word:<br />

Ex. 100. "Take <strong>the</strong> freezes out <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> steaker." (Take <strong>the</strong> steaks out <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

freezer.)<br />

Ex. 101. "She's already trunked two packs." (packed two trunks)<br />

And <strong>of</strong>ten it is <strong>the</strong> grammatical morpheme itself that moves:<br />

Ex. 102. "Can you expla<strong>in</strong> what first gave you this unfeel<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong><br />

worth<strong>in</strong>ess?" (feel<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> unworth<strong>in</strong>ess)<br />

Ex. 103. "Is this coke's Julie?" (Is this coke Julie's?)<br />

Fromk<strong>in</strong>, Garrett (1980), <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rs have used such evidence to suggest<br />

that entries <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> mental lexicon conta<strong>in</strong> only word stems, <strong>and</strong>, with a few<br />

exceptions, grammatical <strong>and</strong> derivational morphemes are affixed by rule<br />

procedures. In addition, errors <strong>in</strong> which non-words appear such as:<br />

7 9<br />

Ex. 104. "You improve by observat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> problem ...<br />

seem to suggest that at least some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> time morphological rules must<br />

apply to roots or stems, s<strong>in</strong>ce such words obviously don't exist <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

lexicon.<br />

It might be <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to look for errors correspond<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> above<br />

examples <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese. S<strong>in</strong>ce Ch<strong>in</strong>ese verbs never undergo <strong>the</strong> sort <strong>of</strong><br />

morphological change that many English verbs do ("go" --> "went",<br />

"th<strong>in</strong>k9'--> "thought") but ra<strong>the</strong>r are only subject to an additive process <strong>in</strong><br />

which grammatical morphemes are affixed to <strong>the</strong> verb <strong>in</strong> order to express<br />

tense or aspect, Ch<strong>in</strong>ese perhaps constitutes a ra<strong>the</strong>r pure test<strong>in</strong>g ground<br />

for <strong>the</strong>ories <strong>in</strong> this area. One might expect, for example, to f<strong>in</strong>d errors <strong>in</strong><br />

@ which an exchange leaves beh<strong>in</strong>d grammatical particles like 7 <strong>and</strong> g.<br />

One might also look for exchanges or substitutions <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1)<br />

complements <strong>in</strong> directional verbs such as & $, $, 3, etc., or<br />

resultative verbs such as j& (as <strong>in</strong> 6 @) <strong>and</strong> 7 (as <strong>in</strong> @ 5 T), etc.<br />

4.3. Ch<strong>in</strong>ese measure words, or classifiers, might be ano<strong>the</strong>r place to<br />

look for <strong>in</strong>structive errors. One <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> ongo<strong>in</strong>g questions asked about<br />

classifiers is: To what extent are <strong>the</strong>y applied mechanically (like gender <strong>in</strong><br />

Romance languages) <strong>and</strong> to what extent do <strong>the</strong>y reflect deep cognitive<br />

aspects <strong>of</strong> human category boundaries? There is general agreement among<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guists that classifiers <strong>in</strong> most cases have some semantic function, <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong>re are ongo<strong>in</strong>g research projects <strong>in</strong> this area. For example, Tai & Wang<br />

(1990) have studied similarities <strong>and</strong> differences <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> use <strong>of</strong> classifiers<br />

across various Ch<strong>in</strong>ese dialects.<br />

Children's errors <strong>in</strong> acquir<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> classifier system <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese have<br />

already been used to support <strong>the</strong>ories about <strong>the</strong> relevance <strong>of</strong> classifiers to<br />

human conceptual mechanisms. Loke & Harrison (1986) <strong>and</strong> Clark (1977)<br />

have studied overextensions <strong>and</strong> misapplications <strong>of</strong> shape classifiers <strong>in</strong><br />

young children learn<strong>in</strong>g Cantonese or M<strong>and</strong>ar<strong>in</strong>, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>y have found that<br />

<strong>the</strong> stages Ch<strong>in</strong>ese children go through <strong>in</strong> master<strong>in</strong>g classifiers corresponds<br />

to <strong>the</strong> acquisition stages <strong>of</strong> shape concepts. One <strong>of</strong> Clark's f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs was that<br />

nearly all <strong>the</strong> criteria used by <strong>the</strong> young children <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>correct<br />

over-extensions <strong>of</strong> shape classifiers have correspond<strong>in</strong>g classifiers <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

languages with classifier systems. This would seem to imply that both<br />

classifier systems <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>g semantic system <strong>of</strong> children seem to<br />

have been cognitively based on <strong>the</strong> same universal pr<strong>in</strong>ciples govern<strong>in</strong>g<br />

categorization. That is, children's overextensions provide <strong>in</strong>formation about<br />

<strong>the</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong> organizational pr<strong>in</strong>ciples humans use <strong>in</strong> try<strong>in</strong>g to make sense<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> world. Mistakes <strong>in</strong> both children <strong>and</strong> adults <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g classifiers<br />

could be used to extend exist<strong>in</strong>g work on categories by researchers such as<br />

Rosch (1 973) <strong>and</strong> Lak<strong>of</strong>f (1 987).<br />

Allen (1977) br<strong>in</strong>gs up three ways <strong>of</strong> decid<strong>in</strong>g whe<strong>the</strong>r or not<br />

classifiers have a semantic function ( e . denote certa<strong>in</strong> perceived<br />

characteristics <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> th<strong>in</strong>g to which <strong>the</strong> associated noun refers). One is to<br />

use <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tuition <strong>of</strong> native <strong>in</strong>formants, suspect though it may be. A second<br />

is to use a foreign observer's <strong>in</strong>tuition about <strong>the</strong> nature <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> noun classes<br />

revealed by classifiers - suspect though that may be. A third, more<br />

performance-oriented method, is to <strong>in</strong>troduce new words <strong>and</strong> objects to a<br />

number <strong>of</strong> native speakers <strong>and</strong> see what classifiers <strong>the</strong>y use with <strong>the</strong>m. To<br />

<strong>the</strong>se three I would add a fourth: To observe speech errors <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g<br />

classifiers <strong>and</strong> see to what extent <strong>the</strong> slips reveal systematic<br />

correspondences with category boundaries. For example:


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 1991)<br />

Ex. 105. "R - Bmsm<br />

8 - # @<br />

"wG y6u y'i pi2n ... w6 y6u y'i fGng $n ..." ("I have a letter...")<br />

If this example is <strong>in</strong>deed a slip (<strong>and</strong> not merely due to <strong>the</strong> speaker's<br />

<strong>in</strong>decision as to whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> document <strong>in</strong> question was an article or a<br />

letter), <strong>the</strong>n it constitutes evidence for <strong>the</strong> non-arbitrary <strong>and</strong><br />

psychologically salient nature <strong>of</strong> classifiers.<br />

Ano<strong>the</strong>r related question about classifiers is: To what extent are <strong>the</strong>y<br />

productive? (This is related to Allen's third method above.) To what<br />

extent do Ch<strong>in</strong>ese speakers creatively apply <strong>and</strong> extend classifiers <strong>in</strong> novel<br />

situations, jokes, science-fiction tales, counterfactual scenarios, etc.? (At an<br />

ice cream store, I once playfully referred to a scoop <strong>of</strong> ice cream <strong>in</strong> a cone<br />

as "- % 8 ?& ?#" - <strong>the</strong> character (du6) be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> measure for such<br />

th<strong>in</strong>gs as clouds <strong>and</strong> flowers - <strong>and</strong> got a bemused smile out <strong>of</strong> my Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

friend.) As at least one set <strong>of</strong> researchers (H<strong>of</strong>stadter & Moser, 1989) has<br />

noted, <strong>the</strong> dist<strong>in</strong>ction between an error <strong>and</strong> a creative extension <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

language is <strong>of</strong>ten a f<strong>in</strong>e one, <strong>and</strong> both errors <strong>and</strong> creative usages are most<br />

likely to occur <strong>in</strong> those l<strong>in</strong>guistic situations where <strong>the</strong> capabilities <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

language <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> cognitive resources <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> speaker are pushed somewhat.<br />

4.4. The k<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong> speech pathologies seen <strong>in</strong> aphasics <strong>and</strong> patients with<br />

o<strong>the</strong>r types <strong>of</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> damage have long been used <strong>in</strong> conjunction with<br />

evidence from errors committed by normal people <strong>in</strong> order to piece<br />

toge<strong>the</strong>r a coherent view <strong>of</strong> lexical retrieval <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r phenomena<br />

(Aitchison, 1987). In addition, l<strong>in</strong>guistic pathologies <strong>of</strong> Japanese aphasics<br />

<strong>and</strong> stroke victims have been <strong>of</strong> great <strong>in</strong>terest to researchers <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> process<strong>in</strong>g differences between phonetic scripts like English <strong>and</strong><br />

logographic scripts such as Ch<strong>in</strong>ese. (See, for example, Paradis et al, 1985.)<br />

The reason Japanese is considered a good test<strong>in</strong>g ground for such issues is<br />

that <strong>the</strong> language uses both a phonetic syllabary (<strong>the</strong> h iragana <strong>and</strong><br />

katakana characters) <strong>and</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese characters (called kanji), <strong>and</strong> thus shares<br />

features <strong>of</strong> both English <strong>and</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese. Among <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g results <strong>of</strong><br />

cl<strong>in</strong>ical studies <strong>in</strong> this area is that most <strong>of</strong>ten <strong>the</strong> ability to read <strong>the</strong><br />

phonetic katakana <strong>and</strong> hiragana script is more severely affected than <strong>the</strong><br />

ability to read kanji characters, <strong>and</strong> this is thought to have significant<br />

implications for <strong>the</strong> study <strong>of</strong> process<strong>in</strong>g differences between different k<strong>in</strong>ds<br />

<strong>of</strong> scripts (Paradis et al, 1985, <strong>and</strong> Hung & Tzeng, 1981).<br />

Many studies have been done on <strong>the</strong> extent <strong>of</strong> phonetic speech<br />

recod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>and</strong> English (e.g. Treiman, Baron, & Luk, 1981, <strong>and</strong><br />

Chu-Chang & Loritz, 1977). There is much fasc<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g evidence on this<br />

issue, some <strong>of</strong> it contradictory <strong>and</strong> confus<strong>in</strong>g, but <strong>the</strong>re is general<br />

mmm"


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1 )<br />

agreement that Ch<strong>in</strong>ese readers do employ a speech recod<strong>in</strong>g strategy <strong>in</strong><br />

read<strong>in</strong>g, though not to <strong>the</strong> extent that readers <strong>of</strong> English do. (See Hung &<br />

Tzeng, 1981, for an excellent overview <strong>of</strong> research <strong>in</strong> this area.)<br />

Errors <strong>in</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g out loud might perhaps be used to<br />

exp<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> supplement <strong>the</strong> corpus <strong>of</strong> evidence <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>se two doma<strong>in</strong>s.<br />

There is already some work <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> field <strong>of</strong> error-mak<strong>in</strong>g that addresses<br />

low-level neurol<strong>in</strong>guistic aspects <strong>of</strong> speech errors (e.g. Laver, 1980), <strong>and</strong><br />

such work can no doubt be <strong>in</strong>corporated <strong>in</strong>to work on aphasia <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

speech-related bra<strong>in</strong> disorders. The comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> error research with <strong>the</strong>se<br />

two areas - cl<strong>in</strong>ical research on speech pathology <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigations <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

read<strong>in</strong>g processes <strong>of</strong> normal language users - might prove very fruitful,<br />

s<strong>in</strong>ce behavior observed <strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistic errors has <strong>the</strong> advantage <strong>of</strong> be<strong>in</strong>g<br />

poised somewhere <strong>in</strong> between "bus<strong>in</strong>ess-as-usual" l<strong>in</strong>guistic performance<br />

<strong>and</strong> pathological function<strong>in</strong>g.


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 1991)<br />

Words <strong>of</strong> Thanks<br />

I wish to thank Liu Haorn<strong>in</strong>g (Xu 8% Efl) <strong>of</strong> Indiana University for his<br />

help <strong>in</strong> collect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> analyz<strong>in</strong>g various materials. Most <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

error-category terms are due to him. I also wish to thank Yan Yong (F<br />

) at Stanford University for several error examples <strong>and</strong> much advice.<br />

Thanks to Zi M<strong>in</strong>yun (B $$) <strong>of</strong> Pek<strong>in</strong>g University, who sent me many<br />

error examples <strong>and</strong> helpful comments. I am grateful to Victor Mair at <strong>the</strong><br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania for very cogent comments <strong>and</strong> suggestions on<br />

<strong>the</strong> first draft <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> paper. As usual, special thanks to Douglas H<strong>of</strong>stadter<br />

for a constant supply <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>spiration <strong>and</strong> guidance, <strong>and</strong> for provid<strong>in</strong>g an<br />

ideal work<strong>in</strong>g environment.


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1)<br />

Bibliography<br />

Abd-El-Jawad, Hassan, <strong>and</strong> Abu-Salim, Issam, (1987) "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>in</strong> Arabic <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

Theoretical Implications", Language Sciences, vol. 9, no. 2, p. 145.<br />

Aitchison, Jean (1987) Words <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> M<strong>in</strong>d, New York: Basil Blackwell.<br />

Aitchison, Jean & Straf, Miron, (1982) "Lexical Storage <strong>and</strong> Retrieval: a Develop<strong>in</strong>g Skill?" <strong>in</strong><br />

Cutler, A. (ed.) <strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> Language Production, Berl<strong>in</strong>: Mouton.<br />

Allen, Keith (1977) "Classifiers", Language vol. 53, no. 2, pp. 285-312.<br />

Bol<strong>in</strong>ger, Dwight (1986) Intonation <strong>and</strong> Its Parts: Melody <strong>in</strong> Spoken English, Stanford: Stanford<br />

University Press.<br />

Butterworth, B. (ed.) (1983) Language Production vol. 2, Development, Writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> O<strong>the</strong>r<br />

Language Processes. London: Academic Press.<br />

Chu-Chang, Mae & Loritz, Donald J. (1977) "Phonological Encod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Ideographs <strong>in</strong><br />

Short-Term Memory", Language Learn<strong>in</strong>g vol. 27, no. 2, pp. 341-352.<br />

Clark, E. V. (1977) "Universal Categories <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Semantics <strong>of</strong> Classifiers <strong>and</strong> Children's Early<br />

Word Mean<strong>in</strong>gs", <strong>in</strong> A. Juill<strong>and</strong> (ed. ), L<strong>in</strong>guistic Studies Offered to Joseph Greenberg, vol.<br />

III, pp. 449-462.<br />

Cohen, Gerald (1987) Syntactic Blends <strong>in</strong> English Parole, Frankfurt-am-Ma<strong>in</strong>: Peter Lang Verlag.<br />

Cutler, Anne (ed.) (1982) <strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> Language Production, Berl<strong>in</strong>: Mouton.<br />

Cutler, Anne (1980) "Errors <strong>of</strong> Stress <strong>and</strong> Intonation", <strong>in</strong> Fromk<strong>in</strong> (ed.) Speech Errors as<br />

L<strong>in</strong>guistic Evidence, The Hague: Mouton.<br />

DeFrancis, John (1984) The Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Language: Fact <strong>and</strong> Fantasy, Honolulu: University <strong>of</strong> Hawaii<br />

Press.<br />

DeFrancis John (1989) Visible Speech: The Diverse Oneness <strong>of</strong> Writ<strong>in</strong>g Systems, Honolulu:<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Hawaii Press.<br />

Dell, Gary S. (1984) "Representation <strong>of</strong> Serial Order <strong>in</strong> Speech: Evidence From <strong>the</strong> Repeated<br />

Phoneme Effect <strong>in</strong> Speech Errors", Journal <strong>of</strong> Experimental Psychology: Learn<strong>in</strong>g, Memory,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Cognition, 1984, vol. 10, no. 2, p. 222-233.<br />

Dell, Gary S. (1986) "A Spread<strong>in</strong>g Activation Theory <strong>of</strong> Retrieval <strong>in</strong> Sentence Production",<br />

Psychological Review, vol. 93, no. 3.<br />

Dell, Gary S. <strong>and</strong> Reich, P. A. (1980) "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong>: The Facts <strong>and</strong> a Stratificational<br />

Model", <strong>in</strong> J. E. Copel<strong>and</strong> & P. W. Davis (eds.), <strong>Papers</strong> <strong>in</strong> Cognitive-Stratificational<br />

L<strong>in</strong>guistics vol. 66, Houston: Rice University Studies, pp. 6 1 1-629.<br />

Dell, Gary S. <strong>and</strong> Reich, P. A. (1981) "Stages <strong>in</strong> Speech Production: an Analysis <strong>of</strong> Speech Error<br />

Data", Journal <strong>of</strong> Verbal Learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Verbal Behaviour 20, pp. 61 1-629.<br />

Ellis, Andrew (1979) "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>", Visible Language XI11 3, pp. 265-282.<br />

I<br />

-


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" Sim-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 1991)<br />

Fay, David (1977) "Prefix errors". Paper presented at <strong>the</strong> 4th Salzburg International L<strong>in</strong>guistics<br />

Meet<strong>in</strong>g, August 1977, <strong>in</strong> Aitchison, Words <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> M<strong>in</strong>d, New York: Basil Blackwell.<br />

Fay, David & Cutler, Anne, (1977) "Malapropisms <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> Structure <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Mental Lexicon",<br />

L<strong>in</strong>guistic Inquiry, vol. 8, no. 3, pp. 505-520.<br />

Fromk<strong>in</strong>, Victoria, (ed.) (1980) Errors <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistic Performance: <strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong>, Ear, <strong>Pen</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> H<strong>and</strong>, New York: Academic Press.<br />

Fromk<strong>in</strong>, Victoria, (ed.) (1973) Speech Errors as L<strong>in</strong>guistic Evidence, The Hague: Mouton & Co.<br />

Frornk<strong>in</strong>, Victoria, (1973) "The Non-Anomalous Nature <strong>of</strong> Anomalous Utterances", <strong>in</strong> Fromk<strong>in</strong><br />

(d) Speech Errors as L<strong>in</strong>guistic Evidence, The Hague: Mouton.<br />

G<strong>and</strong>our, Jack (1977) "Counterfeit Tones <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Speech <strong>of</strong> Sou<strong>the</strong>rn Thai Bidialecticals", L<strong>in</strong>gua<br />

41, pp. 125-143.<br />

Garrett, Memll F. (1975) "The Analysis <strong>of</strong> Sentence Production", <strong>in</strong> G. H. Bower (ed.), The<br />

Psychology <strong>of</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Motivation, vol. 9, New York: Academic Press, pp. 133-177.<br />

Garrett, Merrill F. (1980) "The Limits <strong>of</strong> Accommodation: Arguments for Independent Process<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Levels <strong>in</strong> Sentence Production", <strong>in</strong> Fromk<strong>in</strong> (ed.), Errors <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistic Performance: <strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong>, Ear, <strong>Pen</strong>, <strong>and</strong> H<strong>and</strong>, New York: Academic Press, pp. 263-27 1.<br />

Gibbs, Raymond W. & N<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>i P. Nayak (1989) "Psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic Studies on <strong>the</strong> Syntactic<br />

Behavior <strong>of</strong> Idioms", Cognitive Psychology, vol. 2 1, pp. 100- 138.<br />

Hockett, Charles F. (1967) "Where <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>Slips</strong>, There Slip I", To Honor Roman Jakobson,<br />

The Hague: Mouton.<br />

H<strong>of</strong>stadter, Douglas R. (1985) Metamagical Themas, New York: Basic Books, chapter twelve.<br />

H<strong>of</strong>stadter, D. R., & Moser, D. (1989) "To Err is Human: To Study Error-mak<strong>in</strong>g is Cognitive<br />

Science, Michigan Quarterly Review, vol. 28, no. 2, Spr<strong>in</strong>g 1989, pp. 185-215.<br />

Hotopf, W. H. N. (1980) "Semantic Similarity as a Factor <strong>in</strong> Whole-word <strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong>", <strong>in</strong><br />

Fromk<strong>in</strong> (ed.), Errors <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistic Per$orm.ance: <strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong>, Ear, <strong>Pen</strong>, <strong>and</strong> H<strong>and</strong>,<br />

New York: Academic Press, pp. 97-109.<br />

Hotopf, W. H. N. (1983) "Lexical <strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong>: What They Tell Us About<br />

Language Production", <strong>in</strong> Butterworth, B. (ed) Language Production vol. 2, Development,<br />

Writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> O<strong>the</strong>r Language Processes. London: Academic Press.<br />

Hung, Daisy L. & Tzeng, Ovid J. L. (1981) "Orthographic Variations <strong>and</strong> Visual Information<br />

Process<strong>in</strong>g", Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong>, vol. 90, No. 3, pp. 377-414.<br />

Kao, H. S. R. & Hoosa<strong>in</strong>, R. (1986) L<strong>in</strong>guistics, Psychology, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Language, Hong<br />

Kong: Centre <strong>of</strong> Asian Studies, University <strong>of</strong> Hong Kong.<br />

Kiril<strong>of</strong>f, C. (1969) "On <strong>the</strong> Auditory Perception <strong>of</strong> Tones <strong>in</strong> M<strong>and</strong>ar<strong>in</strong>", Phonetica, no. 20, pp.<br />

2-4.<br />

Lak<strong>of</strong>f, George (1987) Women, Fire, <strong>and</strong> Dangerous Th<strong>in</strong>gs: What Categories Reveal About <strong>the</strong><br />

M<strong>in</strong>d, Chicago: University <strong>of</strong> Chicago Press.


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 199 1)<br />

Laver, John (1980) "Monitor<strong>in</strong>g Systems <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Neurol<strong>in</strong>guistic Control <strong>of</strong> Speech Production", <strong>in</strong> I<br />

Fromk<strong>in</strong> (ed.), Errors <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistic Performance: <strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong>, Ear, <strong>Pen</strong>, <strong>and</strong> H<strong>and</strong>,<br />

New York: Academic Press, pp. 287-305.<br />

Li, C. N. & Thompson, S<strong>and</strong>ra (1977) "The Acquisition <strong>of</strong> Tone <strong>in</strong> M<strong>and</strong>ar<strong>in</strong>-speak<strong>in</strong>g Children",<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Child Language, no. 4, pp. 185-199.<br />

Libennan, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, A. M., Fowler, C. & Fischer, F. W., (1977)<br />

"Phonetic Segmentation <strong>and</strong> Recod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g Reader", <strong>in</strong> A. S. Reber & D.<br />

Scarborough (eds.), Toward a Psychology <strong>of</strong> Read<strong>in</strong>g; The Proceed<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> CUNY<br />

Conference, Hillsdale: Erlbaum.<br />

Loke, Kit Ken & Harrison, Godfrey (1986) "Young Children's Use <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese (Cantonese <strong>and</strong><br />

M<strong>and</strong>ar<strong>in</strong>) Sortal Classifiers" L<strong>in</strong>guistics, Psychology <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Language, Hong<br />

Kong: Centre <strong>of</strong> Asian Studies, University <strong>of</strong> Hong Kong, pp. 125- 146.<br />

Mann, Virg<strong>in</strong>ia (1 98 6) "Phonological Awareness : The Role <strong>of</strong> Read<strong>in</strong>g Experience", Cognition,<br />

vol. 24, pp. 65-92.<br />

Moser, David (1988) "If This Paper Were <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, Would Ch<strong>in</strong>ese People Underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Title?: An Exploration <strong>of</strong> Whorfian Claims About <strong>the</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Language", CRCC Tech<br />

Report no. 21, Center for Research on Concepts <strong>and</strong> Cognition, Indiana University.<br />

Moser, David & Douglas H<strong>of</strong>stadter (1991) "Speech Errors: <strong>the</strong> Royal Road to <strong>the</strong> M<strong>in</strong>d", <strong>in</strong><br />

Douglas H<strong>of</strong>stadter et al, Fluid Concepts <strong>and</strong> Creative Analogies, New York: Basic Books,<br />

forthcom<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Motley, M. T. & Baars, B. J. (1975) "Toward a Model <strong>of</strong> Integrated Edit<strong>in</strong>g Processes <strong>in</strong><br />

Prearticulatory Encod<strong>in</strong>g: Evidence from Laboratory Generated Verbal <strong>Slips</strong>", Paper<br />

presented to <strong>the</strong> Speech Communication Association, Houston. .<br />

Nihei, Yoshiaki (1986) "Experimentally Induced <strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>", <strong>in</strong> Hoosa<strong>in</strong>, R. & Kao, H. S.<br />

R. (eds.) L<strong>in</strong>guistics, Psychology <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Language, Hong Kong: Centre <strong>of</strong> Asian<br />

Studies, University <strong>of</strong> Hong Kong, pp. 309-315.<br />

Nooteboom, Sieb G. (1973) "The <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>Slips</strong> <strong>in</strong>to Patterns", <strong>in</strong> Fromk<strong>in</strong> (ed.), Speech Errors as<br />

L<strong>in</strong>guistic Evidence, The Hague: Mouton.<br />

Nooteboom, Sieb G. (1980) "Speak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Unspeak<strong>in</strong>g: Detection <strong>and</strong> Correction <strong>of</strong> Phonological<br />

<strong>and</strong> Lexical Errors <strong>in</strong> Spontaneous Speech", <strong>in</strong> Fromk<strong>in</strong>, (ed.) Errors <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistic<br />

Performance: <strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong>, Ear, <strong>Pen</strong>, <strong>and</strong> H<strong>and</strong>, New York: Academic Press.<br />

Norman, Donald (1981) "Categorization <strong>of</strong> Action <strong>Slips</strong>", Psychological Review, vol. 88, no. 1,<br />

January, pp. 1-15.<br />

Norman, Donald (1988) The Psychology <strong>of</strong> Everyday Th<strong>in</strong>gs, New York: Basic Books.<br />

Paradis, Michel, Hagiwara, Hiroko, & Hildebr<strong>and</strong>t, Nancy (1985) Neurol<strong>in</strong>guistic Aspects <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Japanese Writ<strong>in</strong>g System, Orl<strong>and</strong>o: Academic Press.<br />

Rosch, Eleanor (1 973) "Natural Categories", Cognitive Psychology, no. 7, pp. 328-350.<br />

Stemberger, Joseph Paul (1984) "Speech Error Collection <strong>and</strong> Fieldwork: Some Choctaw Speech<br />

Errors", International Journal <strong>of</strong> American L<strong>in</strong>guistics, vol. 15, no. 3, July 1984, pp. 345-9.<br />

L<br />

-


David Moser, "<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong>, no. 22 (March 1991)<br />

Tai, James & Wang, Lianq<strong>in</strong>g (1990) "A Cognition-based Semantic Study <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Classifier Tim",<br />

The Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Longuage Teacher's Association, forthcom<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Treiman, Rebecca A., Baron, Jonathan & Luk, Kenneth (1977) "S~eech Recod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Silent<br />

re d<strong>in</strong>^: a comparison <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>and</strong> ~nglish", ~ourial <strong>of</strong> ~hiiese ~<strong>in</strong>~uistics,"vol. 9, pp.<br />

116-125.<br />

Tzeng, 0. J., (1977) "Speech recod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g Ch<strong>in</strong>ese characters" Journal <strong>of</strong> Experimental<br />

Psychology: Human Learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Memory, 3, p. 621.<br />

Tzeng, Ovid & S<strong>in</strong>ger, Harry (eds.), (1981) Perception <strong>of</strong> Pr<strong>in</strong>t: Read<strong>in</strong>g Research <strong>in</strong> Experimental<br />

Psychology. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.<br />

Tzeng, Ovid & Hung, Daisy (198 1) "L<strong>in</strong>guistic Determ<strong>in</strong>ism: A Written Language Perspective", <strong>in</strong><br />

0. J. L. Tzeng & H. S<strong>in</strong>ger (eds.), Perception <strong>of</strong> Pr<strong>in</strong>t: Read<strong>in</strong>g Research <strong>in</strong> Experimental<br />

Psychology. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.<br />

Tzeng, Ovid, Hung, Daisy & Wang, W. S-Y. (1977) "Speech recod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Read<strong>in</strong>g Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

Characters", Journal <strong>of</strong> Experimental Psychology; Human Learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Memory 3, pp.<br />

621 -630.<br />

Tzeng, Ovid & Wang, W. S.-Y. (1983) "The First Two R's", American Scientist, no. 71, pp.<br />

238-243.<br />

van den Broecke, Marcel P. R., & Goldste<strong>in</strong>, Louis (1980) "Consonant Features <strong>in</strong> Speech<br />

Errors", <strong>in</strong> Frornk<strong>in</strong> (ed.), Speech Errors as L<strong>in</strong>guistic Evidence, The Hague: Mouton.<br />

Wells, Rulon (195 I), "Predict<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong>", Yale Scientific Magaz<strong>in</strong>e, December, pp.<br />

9-12,<br />

Zhao Yuanren (1968) A Grammar <strong>of</strong> Spoken Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, Berkeley: University <strong>of</strong> California Press.


Previous Issues<br />

Number Date Author Title Pages<br />

1 Nov.<br />

1986<br />

2 Dec.<br />

1986<br />

3 March<br />

1987<br />

4 Nov.<br />

1987<br />

5 Dec.<br />

1987<br />

6 Jan.<br />

1988<br />

7 Jan.<br />

1988<br />

8 Feb.<br />

1988<br />

9 Dec.<br />

1988<br />

10 June<br />

1989<br />

11 July<br />

1989<br />

Victor H. Mair<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

Andrew Jones<br />

Hiroshima<br />

Victor H. Mair<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

Robert M. S<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Hawaii<br />

Eric A. Havelock<br />

Vassar College<br />

J. Marshall Unger<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Hawaii<br />

Chang Tsung-tung<br />

Goe<strong>the</strong>-Universität<br />

The Need for an Alphabetically<br />

Arranged General Usage<br />

Dictionary <strong>of</strong> M<strong>and</strong>ar<strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese: A<br />

Review Article <strong>of</strong> Some Recent<br />

Dictionaries <strong>and</strong> Current<br />

Lexicographical Projects<br />

The Poetics <strong>of</strong> Uncerta<strong>in</strong>ty <strong>in</strong> Early<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Literature<br />

A Partial Bibliography for <strong>the</strong><br />

Study <strong>of</strong> Indian Influence on<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Popular Literature<br />

The Four Languages <strong>of</strong><br />

“M<strong>and</strong>ar<strong>in</strong>”<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Characters <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> Greek<br />

Alphabet<br />

Computers <strong>and</strong> Japanese Literacy:<br />

Nihonz<strong>in</strong> no Yomikaki Nôryoku to<br />

Konpyuta<br />

Indo-European Vocabulary <strong>in</strong> Old<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

31<br />

45<br />

iv, 214<br />

14<br />

4<br />

13<br />

i, 56<br />

various Reviews (I) ii, 39<br />

Soho Machida<br />

Daitoku-ji, Kyoto<br />

Pratoom Angurarohita<br />

Chulalongkorn University<br />

Bangkok<br />

Edward Shaughnessy<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Chicago<br />

Life <strong>and</strong> Light, <strong>the</strong> Inf<strong>in</strong>ite: A<br />

Historical <strong>and</strong> Philological<br />

Analysis <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Amida Cult<br />

Buddhist Influence on <strong>the</strong><br />

Neo-Confucian Concept <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Sage<br />

Western Cultural Innovations <strong>in</strong><br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>a, 1200 BC<br />

46<br />

31<br />

8


Previous Issues, cont.<br />

Number Date Author Title Pages<br />

12 Aug.<br />

1989<br />

13 Oct.<br />

1989<br />

14 Dec.<br />

1989<br />

15 Jan.<br />

1990<br />

16 March<br />

1990<br />

17 April<br />

1990<br />

18 May<br />

1990<br />

19 June<br />

1990<br />

20 Oct.<br />

1990<br />

Victor H. Mair<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

Jiaosheng Wang<br />

Shanghai<br />

The Contributions <strong>of</strong> T’ang <strong>and</strong><br />

Five Dynasties Transformation<br />

Texts (pien-wen) to Later Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

Popular Literature<br />

The Complete Ci-Poems <strong>of</strong> Li<br />

Q<strong>in</strong>gzhao: A New English<br />

Translation<br />

71<br />

xii,<br />

122<br />

various Reviews (II) 69<br />

George Cardona<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

Victor H. Mair<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

Hea<strong>the</strong>r Peters<br />

University Museum <strong>of</strong><br />

Philadelphia<br />

Victor H. Mair<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

Bosat Man<br />

Nal<strong>and</strong>a<br />

Victor H. Mair<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

On Attitudes Toward Language <strong>in</strong><br />

Ancient India<br />

Three Brief Essays Concern<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Tocharistan<br />

Tattooed Faces <strong>and</strong> Stilt Houses:<br />

Who Were <strong>the</strong> Ancient Yue?<br />

Two Non-Tetragraphic Nor<strong>the</strong>rn<br />

S<strong>in</strong>itic Languages<br />

a. Implications <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Soviet Dungan Script for<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Language<br />

Reform<br />

b. Who Were <strong>the</strong> Gyámi?<br />

Backhill/Pek<strong>in</strong>g/Beij<strong>in</strong>g 6<br />

Introduction <strong>and</strong> Notes for a<br />

Translation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Ma-wang-tui<br />

MSS <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Lao Tzu<br />

19<br />

16<br />

28<br />

28<br />

68


Previous Issues, cont.<br />

Number Date Author Title Pages<br />

21 Dec.<br />

1990<br />

22 March<br />

1991<br />

23 April<br />

1991<br />

24 Aug.<br />

1991<br />

25 Aug.<br />

1991<br />

26 Sept.<br />

1991<br />

27 Aug.<br />

1991<br />

28 Sept.<br />

1991<br />

29 Sept.<br />

1991<br />

30 Oct.<br />

1991<br />

Philippa Jane Benson<br />

Carnegie Mellon<br />

University<br />

David Moser<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Michigan<br />

Victor H. Mair<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

David A. Utz<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

Jean DeBernardi<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Alberta<br />

JAO Tsung-i<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese University <strong>of</strong><br />

Hong Kong<br />

Victor H. Mair, ed.<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

ZHOU Youguang<br />

State Language<br />

Commission, Pek<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Victor H. Mair<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

M. V. S<strong>of</strong>ronov<br />

Institute <strong>of</strong> Far Eastern<br />

Studies, Academy <strong>of</strong><br />

Sciences, Moscow<br />

Two Cross-Cultural Studies on<br />

Read<strong>in</strong>g Theory<br />

<strong>Slips</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Tongue</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Pen</strong> <strong>in</strong><br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

Tracks <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Tao, Semantics <strong>of</strong><br />

Zen<br />

Language, Writ<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> Tradition<br />

<strong>in</strong> Iran<br />

L<strong>in</strong>guistic Nationalism: The Case<br />

<strong>of</strong> Sou<strong>the</strong>rn M<strong>in</strong><br />

Questions on <strong>the</strong> Orig<strong>in</strong>s <strong>of</strong> Writ<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Raised by <strong>the</strong> Silk Road<br />

Schriftfestschrift: Essays <strong>in</strong> Honor<br />

<strong>of</strong> John DeFrancis on His Eightieth<br />

Birthday<br />

The Family <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

Character-Type Scripts (Twenty<br />

Members <strong>and</strong> Four Stages <strong>of</strong><br />

Development)<br />

What Is a Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

“Dialect/Topolect”? Reflections on<br />

Some Key S<strong>in</strong>o-English L<strong>in</strong>guistic<br />

Terms<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Philology <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> Scripts<br />

<strong>of</strong> Central Asia<br />

9, 13<br />

45<br />

10<br />

24<br />

22 + 3<br />

figs.<br />

10<br />

ix, 245<br />

11<br />

31<br />

10


Previous Issues, cont.<br />

Number Date Author Title Pages<br />

31 Oct.<br />

1991<br />

32 Aug.<br />

1992<br />

33 Sept.<br />

1992<br />

34 Oct.<br />

1992<br />

35 Nov.<br />

1992<br />

36 Feb.<br />

1993<br />

37 March<br />

1993<br />

38 April<br />

1993<br />

various Reviews (III) 68<br />

David McCraw<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Hawaii<br />

FENG Lide <strong>and</strong> Kev<strong>in</strong><br />

Stuart<br />

Chuankou No. 1 Middle<br />

School <strong>and</strong> Q<strong>in</strong>ghai<br />

Education College<br />

Victor H. Mair<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

Victor H. Mair<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

with an added note by<br />

Edw<strong>in</strong> G. Pulleyblank<br />

XU Wenkan<br />

Hanyu Da Cidian editorial<br />

<strong>of</strong>fices, Shanghai<br />

Tanya Storch<br />

University <strong>of</strong> New Mexico<br />

Victor H. Mair<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

How <strong>the</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>awoman Lost Her<br />

Voice<br />

Interethnic Contact on <strong>the</strong> Inner<br />

Asian Frontier: The Gangou People<br />

<strong>of</strong> M<strong>in</strong>he County, Q<strong>in</strong>ghai<br />

Two <strong>Papers</strong> on S<strong>in</strong>ol<strong>in</strong>guistics<br />

1. A Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis<br />

Concern<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Orig<strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Term fanqie<br />

(“Countertomy”)<br />

2. East Asian Round-Trip<br />

Words<br />

27<br />

34<br />

13<br />

Reviews (IV) 37<br />

Hanyu Wailaici de Yuyuan<br />

Kaozheng he Cidian Bianzuan<br />

(Philological Research on <strong>the</strong><br />

Etymology <strong>of</strong> Loanwords <strong>in</strong> S<strong>in</strong>itic<br />

<strong>and</strong> Dictionary Compilation)<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Buddhist Historiography<br />

<strong>and</strong> Orality<br />

The L<strong>in</strong>guistic <strong>and</strong> Textual<br />

Antecedents <strong>of</strong> The Sutra <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Wise <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> Foolish<br />

13<br />

16<br />

95


Previous Issues, cont.<br />

Number Date Author Title Pages<br />

39 Aug.<br />

1993<br />

40 Sept.<br />

1993<br />

41 Oct.<br />

1993<br />

42 Nov.<br />

1993<br />

43 Dec.<br />

1993<br />

44 Jan.<br />

1994<br />

45 May<br />

1994<br />

46 July<br />

1994<br />

Jordan Paper<br />

York University<br />

Michael Carr<br />

Center for Language<br />

Studies, Otaru University<br />

<strong>of</strong> Commerce<br />

Paul Gold<strong>in</strong><br />

Harvard University<br />

Rench<strong>in</strong>-Jashe Yulshul<br />

Tibetan Autonomous<br />

Prefecture, Kokonor<br />

(Q<strong>in</strong>ghai)<br />

<strong>and</strong> Kev<strong>in</strong> Stuart<br />

Institute <strong>of</strong> Foreign<br />

Languages, Ulaanbaatar,<br />

Mongolia<br />

MA Quanl<strong>in</strong>, MA<br />

Wanxiang, <strong>and</strong> MA<br />

Zhicheng<br />

X<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<br />

Edited by Kev<strong>in</strong> Stuart<br />

Kokonor<br />

Dolkun Kamberi<br />

Columbia University<br />

Mark Hansell<br />

Carleton College<br />

A Material Case for a Late Ber<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Strait Cross<strong>in</strong>g Co<strong>in</strong>cident with<br />

Pre-Columbian Trans-Pacific<br />

Cross<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

Tiao-Fish through Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

Dictionaries<br />

Mich<strong>in</strong>g Mallecho: The Zhanguo<br />

ce <strong>and</strong> Classical Rhetoric<br />

17<br />

68<br />

27<br />

Kham Tibetan Language Materials 39<br />

Salar Language Materials 72<br />

The Three Thous<strong>and</strong> Year Old<br />

Charchan Man Preserved at<br />

Zaghunluq<br />

The S<strong>in</strong>o-Alphabet: The<br />

Assimilation <strong>of</strong> Roman Letters <strong>in</strong>to<br />

<strong>the</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Writ<strong>in</strong>g System<br />

various Reviews (V) 2, 155<br />

15<br />

28


Previous Issues, cont.<br />

Number Date Author Title Pages<br />

47 Aug.<br />

1994<br />

48 Sept.<br />

1994<br />

49 Oct.<br />

1994<br />

50 Nov.<br />

1994<br />

51 Nov.<br />

1994<br />

52 Nov.<br />

1994<br />

53 Nov.<br />

1994<br />

54 Nov.<br />

1994<br />

55 Nov.<br />

1994<br />

Robert S. Bauer<br />

Mahidol University Salaya<br />

Nakornpathom, Thail<strong>and</strong><br />

Victor H. Mair<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

Ludo Rocher<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

YIN B<strong>in</strong>yong<br />

State Language<br />

Commission <strong>and</strong> Institute<br />

for Applied L<strong>in</strong>guistics<br />

(Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Academy <strong>of</strong><br />

Social Sciences)<br />

HAN Kangx<strong>in</strong><br />

Institute <strong>of</strong> Archeology<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Academy <strong>of</strong><br />

Social Sciences<br />

Warren A. Shibles<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Wiscons<strong>in</strong><br />

Whitewater<br />

XU Wenkan<br />

Editorial Offices <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Hanyu Da Cidian<br />

Shanghai<br />

Üjiyedi<strong>in</strong> Chuluu<br />

(Chaolu Wu)<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Toronto<br />

Üjiyedi<strong>in</strong> Chuluu<br />

(Chaolu Wu)<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Toronto<br />

S<strong>in</strong>o-Tibetan *kolo “Wheel” 11<br />

Introduction <strong>and</strong> Notes for a<br />

Complete Translation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Chuang Tzu<br />

Orality <strong>and</strong> Textuality <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Indian<br />

Context<br />

Diyi ge Lad<strong>in</strong>g Zimu de Hanyu<br />

P<strong>in</strong>y<strong>in</strong> Fang’an Shi Zenyang<br />

Chansheng de? [How Was <strong>the</strong> First<br />

Romanized Spell<strong>in</strong>g System for<br />

S<strong>in</strong>itic Produced?]<br />

The Study <strong>of</strong> Ancient Human<br />

Skeletons from X<strong>in</strong>jiang, Ch<strong>in</strong>a<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Romanization Systems:<br />

IPA Transliteration<br />

Guanyu Tuhuoluoren de Qiyuan he<br />

Qianxi Wenti [On <strong>the</strong> Problem <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> Orig<strong>in</strong>s <strong>and</strong> Migrations <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Tocharians]<br />

Introduction, Grammar, <strong>and</strong><br />

Sample Sentences for Jegün Yogur<br />

Introduction, Grammar, <strong>and</strong><br />

Sample Sentences for Dongxiang<br />

xxxiv,<br />

110<br />

28<br />

7<br />

9 + 4<br />

figs.<br />

20<br />

11<br />

34<br />

34


Previous Issues, cont.<br />

Number Date Author Title Pages<br />

56 Nov.<br />

1994<br />

57 Nov.<br />

1994<br />

58 Nov.<br />

1994<br />

59 Dec.<br />

1994<br />

60 Dec.<br />

1994<br />

61 Dec.<br />

1994<br />

62 Dec.<br />

1994<br />

63 Dec.<br />

1994<br />

Üjiyedi<strong>in</strong> Chuluu<br />

(Chaolu Wu)<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Toronto<br />

Üjiyedi<strong>in</strong> Chuluu<br />

(Chaolu Wu)<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Toronto<br />

Üjiyedi<strong>in</strong> Chuluu<br />

(Chaolu Wu)<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Toronto<br />

Kev<strong>in</strong> Stuart<br />

Q<strong>in</strong>ghai Junior Teachers<br />

College;<br />

Limusishiden<br />

Q<strong>in</strong>ghai Medical College<br />

Attached Hospital, X<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g,<br />

Kokonor (Q<strong>in</strong>ghai)<br />

Kev<strong>in</strong> Stuart, Li<br />

Xuewei, <strong>and</strong> Shelear<br />

Q<strong>in</strong>ghai Junior Teachers<br />

College, X<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, Kokonor<br />

(Q<strong>in</strong>ghai)<br />

Kev<strong>in</strong> Stuart <strong>and</strong> Li<br />

Xuewei<br />

Q<strong>in</strong>ghai Junior Teachers<br />

College, X<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, Kokonor<br />

(Q<strong>in</strong>ghai)<br />

William C. Hannas<br />

Georgetown University<br />

Sarah M. Nelson<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Denver<br />

Introduction, Grammar, <strong>and</strong><br />

Sample Sentences for Dagur<br />

Introduction, Grammar, <strong>and</strong><br />

Sample Sentences for Monguor<br />

Introduction, Grammar, <strong>and</strong><br />

Sample Sentences for Baoan<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>a’s Monguor M<strong>in</strong>ority:<br />

Ethnography <strong>and</strong> Folktales<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>a’s Dagur M<strong>in</strong>ority: Society,<br />

Shamanism, <strong>and</strong> Folklore<br />

Tales from Ch<strong>in</strong>a’s Forest Hunters:<br />

Oroqen Folktales<br />

Reflections on <strong>the</strong> “Unity” <strong>of</strong><br />

Spoken <strong>and</strong> Written Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>and</strong><br />

Academic Learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>a<br />

The Development <strong>of</strong> Complexity <strong>in</strong><br />

Prehistoric North Ch<strong>in</strong>a<br />

36<br />

31<br />

28<br />

i, I,<br />

193<br />

vii,<br />

167<br />

iv, 59<br />

5<br />

17


Previous Issues, cont.<br />

Number Date Author Title Pages<br />

64 Jan.<br />

1995<br />

65 Feb.<br />

1995<br />

66 March<br />

1995<br />

67 April<br />

1995<br />

68 May<br />

1995<br />

69 Jan.<br />

1996<br />

70 Feb.<br />

1996<br />

71 March<br />

1996<br />

72 May<br />

1996<br />

Arne Østmoe<br />

Bangkok, Thail<strong>and</strong>, <strong>and</strong><br />

Drøbak, Norway<br />

<strong>Pen</strong>gl<strong>in</strong> Wang<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese University <strong>of</strong><br />

Hong Kong<br />

ZHU Q<strong>in</strong>gzhi<br />

Sichuan University <strong>and</strong><br />

Pek<strong>in</strong>g University<br />

David McCraw<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Hawaii<br />

Ke <strong>Pen</strong>g, Yanshi Zhu<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Chicago <strong>and</strong><br />

Tokyo, Japan<br />

Dpal-ldan-bkra-shis,<br />

Keith Slater, et al.<br />

Q<strong>in</strong>ghai, Santa Barbara,<br />

etc.<br />

David Utz, X<strong>in</strong>ru Liu,<br />

Taylor Carman, Bryan Van<br />

Norden, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> Editor<br />

Philadelphia, Vassar, etc.<br />

Erik Zürcher<br />

Leiden University<br />

Seishi Karashima<br />

Soka University<br />

Huanm<strong>in</strong>g Q<strong>in</strong><br />

Tang Studies Hotl<strong>in</strong>e<br />

E. Bruce Brooks<br />

University <strong>of</strong><br />

Massachusetts<br />

A Germanic-Tai L<strong>in</strong>guistic Puzzle 81, 6<br />

Indo-European Loanwords <strong>in</strong><br />

Altaic<br />

Some L<strong>in</strong>guistic Evidence for<br />

Early Cultural Exchange Between<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>a <strong>and</strong> India<br />

Pursu<strong>in</strong>g Zhuangzi as a<br />

Rhymemaster: A Snark-Hunt <strong>in</strong><br />

Eight Fits<br />

New Research on <strong>the</strong> Orig<strong>in</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />

Cowries Used <strong>in</strong> Ancient Ch<strong>in</strong>a<br />

Language Materials <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>a’s<br />

Monguor M<strong>in</strong>ority: Huzhu<br />

Mongghul <strong>and</strong> M<strong>in</strong>he Mangghuer<br />

28<br />

7<br />

38<br />

i, 26<br />

xi, 266<br />

Reviews VI 93<br />

Vernacularisms <strong>in</strong> Medieval<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Texts<br />

The Life <strong>and</strong> Mentorship <strong>of</strong><br />

Confucius<br />

31 +<br />

11 + 8<br />

44


Previous Issues, cont.<br />

Number Date Author Title Pages<br />

73 June<br />

1996<br />

74 Jan.<br />

1997<br />

75 Feb.<br />

1997<br />

76 Feb.<br />

1997<br />

77 Jan.<br />

1998<br />

78 Feb.<br />

1998<br />

79 March<br />

1998<br />

80 July<br />

1998<br />

81 Sept.<br />

1998<br />

ZHANG Juan, et al.,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Kev<strong>in</strong> Stuart<br />

Q<strong>in</strong>ghai, Inner Mongolia,<br />

Shanxi, Henan, Liaon<strong>in</strong>g<br />

David Moser<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Michigan &<br />

Beij<strong>in</strong>g Foreign Studies<br />

University<br />

Haun Saussy<br />

Stanford University<br />

Patricia Eichenbaum<br />

Karetzky<br />

Bard College<br />

Daniel Hsieh<br />

Purdue University<br />

Narsu<br />

Inner Mongolia College <strong>of</strong><br />

Agriculture & Animal<br />

Husb<strong>and</strong>ry<br />

Kev<strong>in</strong> Stuart<br />

Q<strong>in</strong>ghai Junior Teachers’<br />

College<br />

Dennis Graffl<strong>in</strong><br />

Bates College<br />

Taishan Yu<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Academy <strong>of</strong><br />

Social Sciences<br />

Hera S. Walker<br />

Urs<strong>in</strong>us College<br />

(Philadelphia)<br />

Blue Cloth <strong>and</strong> Pearl Deer; Yogur<br />

Folklore<br />

Covert Sexism <strong>in</strong> M<strong>and</strong>ar<strong>in</strong><br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

The Prestige <strong>of</strong> Writ<strong>in</strong>g: Wen 2 ,<br />

Letter, Picture, Image, Ideography<br />

The Evolution <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Symbolism <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> Paradise <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Buddha <strong>of</strong><br />

Inf<strong>in</strong>ite Life <strong>and</strong> Its Western<br />

Orig<strong>in</strong>s<br />

The Orig<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> Nature <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

“N<strong>in</strong>eteen Old Poems”<br />

Practical Mongolian Sentences<br />

(With English Translation)<br />

A Sou<strong>the</strong>ast Asian Voice <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Daodej<strong>in</strong>g?<br />

iii, 76<br />

23<br />

40<br />

28<br />

49<br />

iii +<br />

49 + ii<br />

+ 66<br />

A Study <strong>of</strong> Saka History ii +<br />

225<br />

Indigenous or Foreign?: A Look at<br />

<strong>the</strong> Orig<strong>in</strong>s <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Monkey Hero<br />

Sun Wukong<br />

8<br />

iv +<br />

110


Previous Issues, cont.<br />

Number Date Author Title Pages<br />

82 Sept.<br />

1998<br />

83 Oct.<br />

1998<br />

84 Oct.<br />

1998<br />

85 Oct.<br />

1998<br />

86 Oct.<br />

1998<br />

87 Nov.<br />

1998<br />

88 Dec.<br />

1998<br />

89 Jan.<br />

1999<br />

90 Jan.<br />

1999<br />

91 Jan.<br />

1999<br />

I. S. Gurevich<br />

Russian Academy <strong>of</strong><br />

Sciences<br />

M<strong>in</strong>glang Zhou<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Colorado at<br />

Boulder<br />

Ulf Jäger<br />

Gronau/Westfalen,<br />

Germany<br />

Mariko Namba Walter<br />

University <strong>of</strong> New<br />

Engl<strong>and</strong><br />

Jidong Yang<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

Victor H. Mair<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

Saroj Kumar Chaudhuri<br />

Aichi Gakusen University<br />

Alv<strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong><br />

Yale University<br />

A Fragment <strong>of</strong> a pien-wen(?)<br />

Related to <strong>the</strong> Cycle “On Buddha’s<br />

Life”<br />

Tense/Aspect markers <strong>in</strong> M<strong>and</strong>ar<strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Xiang dialects, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

contact<br />

The New Old Mummies from<br />

Eastern Central Asia: Ancestors <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> Tocharian Knights Depicted on<br />

<strong>the</strong> Buddhist Wallpa<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>of</strong><br />

Kucha <strong>and</strong> Turfan? Some<br />

Circumstantial Evidence<br />

Tokharian Buddhism <strong>in</strong> Kucha:<br />

Buddhism <strong>of</strong> Indo-European<br />

Centum Speakers <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

Turkestan before <strong>the</strong> 10th Century<br />

C.E.<br />

Siba: Bronze Age Culture <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Gansu Corridor<br />

Can<strong>in</strong>e Conundrums: Eurasian Dog<br />

Ancestor Myths <strong>in</strong> Historical <strong>and</strong><br />

Ethnic Perspective<br />

15<br />

20<br />

9<br />

30<br />

18<br />

74<br />

Siddham <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>a <strong>and</strong> Japan 9, 124<br />

Writ<strong>in</strong>g Taiwanese: The<br />

Development <strong>of</strong> Modern Written<br />

Taiwanese<br />

Victor H. Mair et al Reviews VII [<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g review <strong>of</strong><br />

The Orig<strong>in</strong>al Analects]<br />

Victor H. Mair<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

Phonosymbolism or Etymology:<br />

The Case <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Verb “Cop”<br />

4 + 41<br />

+ 4<br />

2, 38<br />

28


Previous Issues, cont.<br />

Number Date Author Title Pages<br />

92 Jan.<br />

1999<br />

93 Jan.<br />

1999<br />

94 March<br />

1999<br />

95 May<br />

1999<br />

96 June<br />

1999<br />

97 Dec.<br />

1999<br />

98 Jan.<br />

2000<br />

99 Feb.<br />

2000<br />

100 Feb.<br />

2000<br />

101 March<br />

2000<br />

Christ<strong>in</strong>e Louise L<strong>in</strong><br />

Dartmouth College<br />

David S. Nivison<br />

Stanford University<br />

Julie Lee Wei<br />

Hoover Institute<br />

Victor H. Mair<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

E. Bruce Brooks<br />

University <strong>of</strong><br />

Massachusetts<br />

LI Shuicheng<br />

Pek<strong>in</strong>g University<br />

Peter Daniels, Daniel<br />

Boucher, <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

authors<br />

Anthony Barbieri-Low<br />

Pr<strong>in</strong>ceton University<br />

Wayne Alt<br />

Community College <strong>of</strong><br />

Baltimore County (Essex)<br />

C. Michele Thompson<br />

South Connecticut State<br />

University<br />

The Presbyterian Church <strong>in</strong> Taiwan<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> Advocacy <strong>of</strong> Local<br />

Autonomy<br />

The Key to <strong>the</strong> Chronology <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Three Dynasties: The “Modern<br />

Text” Bamboo Annals<br />

Correspondence Between <strong>the</strong><br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Calendar Signs <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Phoenician Alphabet<br />

A Medieval, Central Asian<br />

Buddhist Theme <strong>in</strong> a Late M<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Taoist Tale by Feng Meng-lung<br />

Alex<strong>and</strong>rian Motifs <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

Texts<br />

S<strong>in</strong>o-Western Contact <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Second Millennium BC<br />

xiii +<br />

136<br />

iv + 68<br />

65 + 6<br />

27<br />

14<br />

iv, 29<br />

Reviews VIII 108<br />

Wheeled Vehicles <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

Bronze Age (c. 2000-741 BC)<br />

Zhuangzi, Mysticism, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Rejection <strong>of</strong> Dist<strong>in</strong>ctions<br />

The Viêt Peoples <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> Orig<strong>in</strong>s <strong>of</strong><br />

Nom<br />

v, 98 +<br />

5 color<br />

plates<br />

29<br />

71, 1


Previous Issues, cont.<br />

Number Date Author Title Pages<br />

102 March<br />

2000<br />

103 June<br />

2000<br />

104 July<br />

2000<br />

105 Aug.<br />

2000<br />

106 Sept.<br />

2000<br />

107 Sept.<br />

2000<br />

108 Sept.<br />

2000<br />

109 Oct.<br />

2000<br />

110 Oct.<br />

2000<br />

111 Nov.<br />

2000<br />

112 July<br />

2001<br />

Theresa Jen<br />

Bryn Mawr College<br />

P<strong>in</strong>g Xu<br />

Baruch College<br />

Carrie E. Reid<br />

Middlebury College<br />

David W. Pankenier<br />

Lehigh University<br />

Anne Birrell<br />

Cambridge University<br />

Yu Taishan<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Academy <strong>of</strong><br />

Social Sciences<br />

Jacques deLisle,<br />

Adelheid E. Krohne,<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> editor<br />

Ruth H. Chang<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

Conán Dean Carey<br />

Stanford University<br />

Toh Hoong Teik<br />

Harvard University<br />

Victor H. Mair<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

Victor H. Mair<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

<strong>Pen</strong>less Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Character<br />

Reproduction<br />

15<br />

Early Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Tattoo 52<br />

Popular Astrology <strong>and</strong> Border<br />

Affairs <strong>in</strong> Early Ch<strong>in</strong>a<br />

Postmodernist Theory <strong>in</strong> Recent<br />

Studies <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Literature<br />

A Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis about <strong>the</strong> Sources <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> Sai Tribes<br />

19 + 1<br />

color<br />

plate<br />

31<br />

i, 3,<br />

200<br />

Reviews IX 148 +<br />

map<br />

Underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g Di <strong>and</strong> Tian: Deity<br />

<strong>and</strong> Heaven From Shang to Tang<br />

vii, 54<br />

In Hell <strong>the</strong> One without S<strong>in</strong> is Lord ii, 60<br />

Shaykh 'Alam: The Emperor <strong>of</strong><br />

Early Sixteenth-Century Ch<strong>in</strong>a<br />

20<br />

The Need for a New Era 10<br />

Notes on <strong>the</strong> Anau Inscription xi, 93


Previous Issues, cont.<br />

Number Date Author Title Pages<br />

113 Aug.<br />

2001<br />

114 March<br />

2002<br />

115 April<br />

2002<br />

116 May<br />

2002<br />

117 May<br />

2002<br />

118 June<br />

2002<br />

119 July<br />

2002<br />

120 July<br />

2002<br />

121 July<br />

2002<br />

Ray Coll<strong>in</strong>s<br />

Chepachet, RI<br />

David Kerr<br />

Melbourne, FL<br />

Ramnath Subbaraman<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Chicago<br />

ZHOU Jixu<br />

Sichuan Normal<br />

University<br />

LIU Yongquan<br />

Institute <strong>of</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics,<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Academy <strong>of</strong><br />

Social Sciences<br />

SHANG Wei<br />

Columbia University<br />

Just<strong>in</strong>e T. Snow<br />

Port Townsend, WA<br />

WU Zhen<br />

X<strong>in</strong>jiang Museum,<br />

Ürümchi<br />

Anne Birrell<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Cambridge,<br />

Clare Hall<br />

Mark Edward Lewis<br />

Stanford University<br />

Etymology <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Word<br />

“Macrobiotic:s” <strong>and</strong> Its Use <strong>in</strong><br />

Modern Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Scholarship<br />

Beyond <strong>the</strong> Question <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Monkey Imposter: Indian Influence<br />

on <strong>the</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Novel, The Journey<br />

to <strong>the</strong> West<br />

Correspondences <strong>of</strong> Basic Words<br />

Between Old Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>and</strong><br />

Proto-Indo-European<br />

On <strong>the</strong> Problem <strong>of</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

Lettered Words<br />

Baihua, Guanhua, Fangyan <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

May Fourth Read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> Rul<strong>in</strong><br />

Waishi<br />

Evidence for <strong>the</strong> Indo-European<br />

Orig<strong>in</strong> <strong>of</strong> Two Ancient Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

Deities<br />

“Hu” Non-Ch<strong>in</strong>ese as They Appear<br />

<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Materials from <strong>the</strong> Astana<br />

Graveyard at Turfan<br />

Female-Gendered Myth <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Classic <strong>of</strong> Mounta<strong>in</strong>s <strong>and</strong> Seas<br />

Dic<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Div<strong>in</strong>ation <strong>in</strong> Early<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>a<br />

18<br />

35<br />

8<br />

13<br />

10<br />

ii, 75,<br />

1<br />

color,<br />

1 b-w<br />

pr<strong>in</strong>t<br />

21, 5<br />

figs.<br />

47<br />

22, 7<br />

figs.


Previous Issues, cont.<br />

Number Date Author Title Pages<br />

122 July<br />

2002<br />

123 Aug.<br />

2002<br />

124 August<br />

2002<br />

125 July<br />

2003<br />

126 Aug.<br />

2003<br />

127 Oct.<br />

2003<br />

128 Nov.<br />

2003<br />

129 Dec.<br />

2003<br />

130 Feb.<br />

2004<br />

Julie Wilensky<br />

Yale Univesity<br />

Paul R. Gold<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

editor<br />

Fredrik T. Hiebert<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

John Colarusso<br />

McMaster University<br />

ZHOU Jixu<br />

Sichuan Normal<br />

University<br />

Shanghai Normal<br />

University<br />

Tim Miller<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Wash<strong>in</strong>gton<br />

Sundeep S. Jhutti<br />

Petaluma, California<br />

Y<strong>in</strong>po Tschang<br />

New York City<br />

Michael Witzel<br />

Harvard University<br />

Bede Fahey<br />

Fort St. John, British<br />

Columbia<br />

The Magical Kunlun <strong>and</strong> “Devil<br />

Slaves”: Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Perceptions <strong>of</strong><br />

Dark-sk<strong>in</strong>ned People <strong>and</strong> Africa<br />

before 1500<br />

51, 3<br />

figs.<br />

Reviews X 30<br />

The Context <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Anau Seal<br />

Remarks on <strong>the</strong> Anau <strong>and</strong> Niyä<br />

Seals<br />

Correspondences <strong>of</strong> Cultural Words<br />

between Old Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>and</strong><br />

Proto-Indo-European<br />

A Sou<strong>the</strong>rn M<strong>in</strong> Word <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Tsu-t’ang chi<br />

1-34<br />

35-47<br />

19<br />

14<br />

The Getes 125, 8<br />

color<br />

plates<br />

On Proto-Shang 18<br />

L<strong>in</strong>guistic Evidence for Cultural<br />

Exchange <strong>in</strong> Prehistoric Western<br />

Central Asia<br />

Mayan: A S<strong>in</strong>o-Tibetan Language?<br />

A Comparative Study<br />

70<br />

61


Previous Issues, cont.<br />

Number Date Author Title Pages<br />

131 March<br />

2004<br />

132 April<br />

2004<br />

133 April<br />

2004<br />

134 May<br />

2004<br />

135 May<br />

2004<br />

136 May<br />

2004<br />

137 June<br />

2004<br />

138 June<br />

2004<br />

139 June<br />

2004<br />

140 June<br />

2004<br />

141 July<br />

2004<br />

Taishan Yu<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Academy <strong>of</strong><br />

Social Sciences<br />

Kim Hayes<br />

Sydney<br />

John L. Sorenson<br />

Brigham Young University<br />

Carl L. Johannessen<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Oregon<br />

Xieyan H<strong>in</strong>cha<br />

Neumädewitz, Germany<br />

John J. Emerson<br />

Portl<strong>and</strong>, Oregon<br />

Serge Papillon<br />

Mouvaux, France <strong>and</strong><br />

Ulaanbaatar, Mongolia<br />

Hoong Teik Toh<br />

Harvard University<br />

Julie Lee Wei<br />

San Jose <strong>and</strong> London<br />

Taishan Yu<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Academy <strong>of</strong><br />

Social Sciences<br />

Y<strong>in</strong>po Tschang<br />

New York City<br />

Y<strong>in</strong>po Tschang<br />

New York City<br />

A History <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Relationship<br />

between <strong>the</strong> Western <strong>and</strong> Eastern<br />

Han, Wei, J<strong>in</strong>, Nor<strong>the</strong>rn <strong>and</strong><br />

Sou<strong>the</strong>rn Dynasties <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Western Regions<br />

On <strong>the</strong> Presence <strong>of</strong> Non-Ch<strong>in</strong>ese at<br />

Anyang<br />

Scientific Evidence for<br />

Pre-Columbian Transoceanic<br />

Voyages CD-ROM<br />

Two Steps Toward Digraphia <strong>in</strong><br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>a<br />

The Secret History <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Mongols<br />

<strong>and</strong> Western Literature<br />

Influences tokhariennes sur la<br />

mythologie ch<strong>in</strong>oise<br />

Some Classical Malay Materials<br />

for <strong>the</strong> Study <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Novel<br />

Journey to <strong>the</strong> West<br />

Dogs <strong>and</strong> Cats: Lessons from<br />

Learn<strong>in</strong>g Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

A Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis on <strong>the</strong> Orig<strong>in</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Yu State<br />

Shih <strong>and</strong> Zong: Social Organization<br />

<strong>in</strong> Bronze Age Ch<strong>in</strong>a<br />

Chaos <strong>in</strong> Heaven: On <strong>the</strong> Calendars<br />

<strong>of</strong> Preclassical Ch<strong>in</strong>a<br />

1, 3,<br />

352<br />

11<br />

48,<br />

166,<br />

19, 15<br />

plates<br />

i, 22<br />

21<br />

47<br />

64<br />

17<br />

20<br />

28<br />

30


Previous Issues, cont.<br />

Number Date Author Title Pages<br />

142 July<br />

2004<br />

143 July<br />

2004<br />

144 July<br />

2004<br />

145 Aug.<br />

2004<br />

146 Feb.<br />

2005<br />

147 March<br />

2005<br />

148 April<br />

2005<br />

149 May<br />

2005<br />

150 May<br />

2005<br />

151 June<br />

2005<br />

Ka<strong>the</strong>ryn L<strong>in</strong>duff, ed.<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Pittsburgh<br />

Victor H. Mair<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

RONG X<strong>in</strong>jiang<br />

Pek<strong>in</strong>g University<br />

<strong>the</strong> editor<br />

Hoong Teik Toh<br />

Academia S<strong>in</strong>ica<br />

Hoong Teik Toh<br />

Academia S<strong>in</strong>ica<br />

Lucas Christopoulos<br />

Beij<strong>in</strong>g Sports University<br />

Kimberly S. Te W<strong>in</strong>kle<br />

University College,<br />

London<br />

Dolkun Kamberi<br />

Wash<strong>in</strong>gton, DC<br />

Jane Jia SI<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

Silk Road Exchange <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>a 64<br />

Sleep <strong>in</strong> Dream: Soporific<br />

Responses to Depression <strong>in</strong> Story<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Stone<br />

L<strong>and</strong> Route or Sea Route?<br />

Commentary on <strong>the</strong> Study <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Paths <strong>of</strong> Transmission <strong>and</strong> Areas <strong>in</strong><br />

which Buddhism Was<br />

Dissem<strong>in</strong>ated dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> Han<br />

Period<br />

99<br />

32<br />

Reviews XI 2, 41<br />

The -yu End<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Xiongnu,<br />

Xianbei, <strong>and</strong> Gaoju Onomastica<br />

Ch. Qiong ~ Tib. Khyung; Taoism<br />

~ Bonpo -- Some Questions<br />

Related to Early Ethno-Religious<br />

History <strong>in</strong> Sichuan<br />

Le gréco-bouddhisme et l’art du<br />

po<strong>in</strong>g en Ch<strong>in</strong>e<br />

A Sacred Tr<strong>in</strong>ity: God, Mounta<strong>in</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Bird: Cultic Practices <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Bronze Age Chengdu Pla<strong>in</strong><br />

24<br />

18<br />

52<br />

ii, 103<br />

(41 <strong>in</strong><br />

color)<br />

Uyghurs <strong>and</strong> Uyghur Identity 44<br />

The Genealogy <strong>of</strong> Dictionaries:<br />

Producers, Literary Audience, <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> Circulation <strong>of</strong> English Texts <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> Treaty Port <strong>of</strong> Shanghai<br />

44, 4<br />

tables


Previous Issues, cont.<br />

Number Date Author Title Pages<br />

152 June<br />

2005<br />

153 July<br />

2005<br />

154 July<br />

2005<br />

155 July<br />

2005<br />

156 July<br />

2005<br />

157 Aug.<br />

2005<br />

158 Aug.<br />

2005<br />

159 Aug.<br />

2005<br />

160 Sept.<br />

2005<br />

161 Sept.<br />

2005<br />

162 Sept.<br />

2005<br />

Denis Mair<br />

Seattle<br />

Alan Piper<br />

London (UK)<br />

Serge Papillon<br />

Belfort, France<br />

Denis Mair<br />

Seattle<br />

Abolqasem Esmailpour<br />

Shahid Beheshti<br />

University, Tehran<br />

Ralph D. Sawyer<br />

Independent Scholar<br />

Mark Edward Lewis<br />

Stanford University<br />

Jens Østergaard<br />

Petersen<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Copenhagen<br />

Matteo Compareti<br />

Venice<br />

Julie Lee Wei<br />

London<br />

Julie Lee Wei<br />

London<br />

The Dance <strong>of</strong> Qian <strong>and</strong> Kun <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Zhouyi<br />

The Mysterious Orig<strong>in</strong>s <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Word “Marihuana”<br />

13, 2<br />

figs.<br />

17<br />

Mythologie s<strong>in</strong>o-européenne 174, 1<br />

plate<br />

Janus-Like Concepts <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Li <strong>and</strong><br />

Kun Trigrams<br />

Manichean Gnosis <strong>and</strong> Creation 157<br />

Paradoxical Coexistence <strong>of</strong><br />

Prognostication <strong>and</strong> Warfare<br />

Writ<strong>in</strong>gs on Warfare Found <strong>in</strong><br />

Ancient Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Tombs<br />

The Zuozhuan Account <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Death <strong>of</strong> K<strong>in</strong>g Zhao <strong>of</strong> Chu <strong>and</strong> Its<br />

Sources<br />

Literary Evidence for <strong>the</strong><br />

Identification <strong>of</strong> Some Common<br />

Scenes <strong>in</strong> Han Funerary Art<br />

The Names <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Yi J<strong>in</strong>g Trigrams:<br />

An Inquiry <strong>in</strong>to Their L<strong>in</strong>guistic<br />

Orig<strong>in</strong>s<br />

Count<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Knott<strong>in</strong>g:<br />

Correspondences between Old<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>and</strong> Indo-European<br />

8<br />

13<br />

15<br />

47<br />

14<br />

18<br />

71,<br />

map


Previous Issues, cont.<br />

Number Date Author Title Pages<br />

163 Oct.<br />

2005<br />

164 Oct.<br />

2005<br />

165 Oct.<br />

2005<br />

166 Nov.<br />

2005<br />

167 Dec.<br />

2005<br />

168 Dec.<br />

2005<br />

169 Jan.<br />

2006<br />

170 Feb.<br />

2006<br />

Julie Lee Wei<br />

London<br />

Julie Lee Wei<br />

London<br />

Julie Lee Wei<br />

London<br />

Julie Lee Wei<br />

London<br />

Hodong Kim<br />

Seoul National University<br />

<strong>and</strong> David Selvia <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> Editor<br />

both <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> University <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

ZHOU Jixu<br />

Sichuan Normal<br />

University<br />

Judith A. Lerner<br />

New York City<br />

Victor H. Mair<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

Amber R. Woodward<br />

University <strong>of</strong> <strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania<br />

Huangdi <strong>and</strong> Huntun (<strong>the</strong> Yellow<br />

Emperor <strong>and</strong> Wonton): A New<br />

Hypo<strong>the</strong>sis on Some Figures <strong>in</strong><br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Mythology<br />

Shang <strong>and</strong> Zhou: An Inquiry <strong>in</strong>to<br />

<strong>the</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistic Orig<strong>in</strong>s <strong>of</strong> Two<br />

Dynastic Names<br />

DAO <strong>and</strong> DE: An Inquiry <strong>in</strong>to <strong>the</strong><br />

L<strong>in</strong>guistic Orig<strong>in</strong>s <strong>of</strong> Some Terms<br />

<strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Philosophy <strong>and</strong><br />

Morality<br />

44<br />

62<br />

51<br />

Reviews XII i, 63<br />

Old Ch<strong>in</strong>ese '帝*tees' <strong>and</strong><br />

Proto-Indo-European “*deus”:<br />

Similarity <strong>in</strong> Religious Ideas <strong>and</strong> a<br />

Common Source <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistics<br />

Aspects <strong>of</strong> Assimilation: <strong>the</strong><br />

Funerary Practices <strong>and</strong> Furnish<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

<strong>of</strong> Central Asians <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>a<br />

Conversion Tables for <strong>the</strong><br />

Three-Volume Edition <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Hanyu Da Cidian<br />

Learn<strong>in</strong>g English, Los<strong>in</strong>g Face, <strong>and</strong><br />

Tak<strong>in</strong>g Over: The Method (or<br />

Madness) <strong>of</strong> Li Yang <strong>and</strong> His Crazy<br />

English<br />

17<br />

51, v,<br />

9<br />

plates<br />

i, 284<br />

18


Previous Issues, cont.<br />

Number Date Author Title Pages<br />

Beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with issue no. 171, S<strong>in</strong>o-<strong>Platonic</strong> <strong>Papers</strong> will be published electronically on <strong>the</strong> Web.<br />

Issues from no. 1 to no. 170, however, will cont<strong>in</strong>ue to be sold as paper copies until our stock runs out,<br />

after which <strong>the</strong>y too will be made available on <strong>the</strong> Web. For prices <strong>of</strong> paper copies, see <strong>the</strong> catalog at<br />

www.s<strong>in</strong>o-platonic.org<br />

171 June<br />

2006<br />

172 Aug.<br />

2006<br />

173 Oct.<br />

2006<br />

174 Nov.<br />

2006<br />

175 Dec.<br />

2006<br />

176 May<br />

2007<br />

John DeFrancis<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Hawaii<br />

The Prospects for Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Writ<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Reform<br />

Deborah Beaser The Outlook for Taiwanese<br />

Language Preservation<br />

Taishan Yu<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Academy <strong>of</strong><br />

Social Sciences<br />

A Study <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> History <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Relationship Between <strong>the</strong> Western<br />

<strong>and</strong> Eastern Han, Wei, J<strong>in</strong>,<br />

Nor<strong>the</strong>rn <strong>and</strong> Sou<strong>the</strong>rn Dynasties<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> Western Regions<br />

26, 3<br />

figs.<br />

Mariko Namba Walter Sogdians <strong>and</strong> Buddhism 65<br />

Zhou Jixu<br />

Center for East Asian<br />

Studies, University <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>Pen</strong>nsylvania; Ch<strong>in</strong>ese<br />

Department, Sichuan<br />

Normal University<br />

Eric Henry<br />

University <strong>of</strong> North<br />

Carol<strong>in</strong>a<br />

The Rise <strong>of</strong> Agricultural<br />

Civilization <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>a: The<br />

Disparity between Archeological<br />

Discovery <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> Documentary<br />

Record <strong>and</strong> Its Explanation<br />

18<br />

167<br />

38<br />

The Submerged History <strong>of</strong> Yuè 36

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!