02.05.2015 Views

State v. Henderson and the New Model Jury Charges - New Jersey ...

State v. Henderson and the New Model Jury Charges - New Jersey ...

State v. Henderson and the New Model Jury Charges - New Jersey ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

a-8-08.opn.html<br />

experiments, Dr. Penrod testified that 64% of eyewitnesses made no<br />

identification, but 36% picked a foil. See Krafka & Penrod, supra, at 64; Pigott et<br />

al., supra, at 86. Those field experiments suggest that when <strong>the</strong> true perpetrator<br />

is not in <strong>the</strong> lineup, eyewitnesses may none<strong>the</strong>less select an innocent suspect<br />

more than one-third of <strong>the</strong> time.<br />

Any one of <strong>the</strong> above studies, st<strong>and</strong>ing alone, reveals a troubling lack of<br />

reliability in eyewitness identifications.<br />

We accept that eyewitnesses generally act in good faith. Most<br />

misidentifications stem from <strong>the</strong> fact that human memory is malleable; <strong>the</strong>y are<br />

not <strong>the</strong> result of malice. As discussed below, an array of variables can affect <strong>and</strong><br />

dilute eyewitness memory.<br />

Along with those variables, a concept called relative judgment, which<br />

<strong>the</strong> Special Master <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> experts discussed, helps explain how people make<br />

identifications <strong>and</strong> raises concerns about reliability. Under typical lineup<br />

conditions, eyewitnesses are asked to identify a suspect from a group of<br />

similar-looking people. “[R]elative judgment refers to <strong>the</strong> fact that <strong>the</strong> witness<br />

seems to be choosing <strong>the</strong> lineup member who most resembles <strong>the</strong> witnesses’<br />

memory relative to o<strong>the</strong>r lineup members.” Gary L. Wells, The Psychology of<br />

Lineup Identifications, 14 J. Applied Soc. Psychol. 89, 92 (1984) (emphasis in<br />

original). As a result, if <strong>the</strong> actual perpetrator is not in a lineup, people may be<br />

inclined to choose <strong>the</strong> best look-alike. Id. at 93. Psychologists have noted that<br />

“[t]his is not a surprising proposition.” Gary L. Wells, What Do We Know About<br />

Eyewitness Identification?, 48 Am. Psychologist 553, 560 (1993). Also not<br />

surprising is that it enhances <strong>the</strong> risk of misidentification. Ibid.<br />

In one relative-judgment experiment, 200 witnesses were shown a staged<br />

crime. Id. at 561. Half of <strong>the</strong> witnesses were <strong>the</strong>n shown a lineup that included<br />

<strong>the</strong> perpetrator <strong>and</strong> five fillers; <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r half looked at a lineup with fillers<br />

only. Ibid. All of <strong>the</strong> witnesses were warned that <strong>the</strong> culprit might not be in <strong>the</strong><br />

array <strong>and</strong> were given <strong>the</strong> option to choose no one. Ibid. From <strong>the</strong> first group,<br />

54% made a correct identification <strong>and</strong> 21% believed, incorrectly, that <strong>the</strong><br />

perpetrator was not in <strong>the</strong> array. Ibid. If witnesses rely on pure memory instead<br />

of relative judgment, <strong>the</strong> accurate identifications from <strong>the</strong> first group should<br />

have translated roughly into 54% making no choice in <strong>the</strong> second, target-absent<br />

http://njlaw.rutgers.edu/collections/courts/supreme/a-8-08.opn.html[4/15/2013 6:04:23 PM]

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!