19.12.2012 Views

1 Chapter 1. Introduction: status and definition of compounding ...

1 Chapter 1. Introduction: status and definition of compounding ...

1 Chapter 1. Introduction: status and definition of compounding ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

One problem hinges on how we distinguish bound roots from derivational affixes. One<br />

criterion that we might use is semantic: roots in some sense have more semantic substance than<br />

affixes. But there are languages in which items that have been formally identified as affixes<br />

have as much, or nearly as much, semantic substance as items that might be identified as roots in<br />

other languages. Mithun (1999:48-50) argues, for example, for what she calls ‗lexical affixes‘ in<br />

many Native American languages. Bearing meanings like ‗clothes‘, ‗floor‘, ‗nape‘, ‗knob‘ (in<br />

Spokane) or ‗eat‘, ‗say‘, ‗fetch‘, ‗hit‘ (in Yup‘ik), they might look semantically like roots, but<br />

their distribution is different from that <strong>of</strong> roots, <strong>and</strong> they serve a discourse function rather<br />

different from that <strong>of</strong> roots (they serve to background information that has already been<br />

introduced in a discourse). So distinguishing lexemes from non-lexemes might not be possible<br />

in semantic terms.<br />

Another criterion must therefore be formal: we might say that bound roots can be<br />

distinguished from affixes only by virtue <strong>of</strong> also occurring as free forms (inflected, <strong>of</strong> course, in<br />

languages that require inflection). But that means that words like overfly <strong>and</strong> outrun in English<br />

must be considered compounds, rather than prefixed forms (as March<strong>and</strong> would like). There are<br />

two problems with this conclusion. First, the <strong>status</strong> <strong>of</strong> verbal compounds in English is highly<br />

disputed, <strong>and</strong> these items are clearly verbal. 1 Second, even though over <strong>and</strong> out also occur as<br />

free morphemes in English, the form that attaches to the verbs fly <strong>and</strong> run behaves rather<br />

differently than the first element <strong>of</strong> a compound. Specifically, the first element <strong>of</strong> a compound in<br />

English is typically syntactically inert: it does not affect the syntactic distribution <strong>of</strong> the complex<br />

word. Yet over- <strong>and</strong> out- have clear effects on verbal diathesis:<br />

(1) a. *The plane flew the field ~ The plane overflew the field.<br />

4

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!