23.12.2012 Views

Using a Music Therapy Collaborative Consultative Approach - World ...

Using a Music Therapy Collaborative Consultative Approach - World ...

Using a Music Therapy Collaborative Consultative Approach - World ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Institut für Musiktherapie, Lehrstuhl für Qualitative Forschung in der Medizin<br />

<strong>Using</strong> a <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> <strong>Collaborative</strong> <strong>Consultative</strong><br />

<strong>Approach</strong> for the Inclusion of Young Children with<br />

Autism in a Childcare Program<br />

vorgelegt von Petra Kern<br />

aus Aalen<br />

2004<br />

Inaugural-Dissertation<br />

zur<br />

Erlangung des Grades eines Doktor rerum medicinalium<br />

der<br />

Universität Witten/Herdecke<br />

im<br />

Bereich der Medizin


“One sees clearly only with the heart.<br />

Anything essential is invisible to the eyes.”<br />

‘The little prince’<br />

by<br />

Antoine de Saint-Exupéry, 1943<br />

Mentor: Prof. David Aldridge, Ph.D.<br />

1. Gutachter: Prof. Dr. rer. medic. Lutz Neugebauer<br />

2. Gutachter: Prof. Mark Wolery, Ph.D.<br />

Akademiereferent: Prof. Dr. rer. medic. Lutz Neugebauer<br />

Tag der Disputation: 19. April, 2004


iii


ABSTRACT<br />

<strong>Using</strong> a <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> <strong>Collaborative</strong> <strong>Consultative</strong> <strong>Approach</strong> for the Inclusion of<br />

Young Children with Autism in a Childcare Program<br />

BACKGROUND. Young children diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder<br />

(ASD) are increasingly being included in childcare programs and receiving their therapeutic<br />

services in the context of ongoing class activities and routines. The rationale for this<br />

approach is at least threefold: (a) to minimize stigma and isolation; (b) to capitalize on<br />

children’s naturally occurring learning opportunities; and (c) to increase the number of<br />

experiences that promote learning. Despite widespread studies of preschool inclusion, thus<br />

far there exists no scholarly research specifically evaluating the effectiveness of music<br />

therapy interventions – particularly the collaborative consultative model of service delivery –<br />

for the inclusion of young children with autism in childcare programs.<br />

PURPOSE. The purpose of this cumulative study is to examine the effects of<br />

embedded music therapy interventions on the functioning of young children with autism<br />

during challenging classroom routines within an inclusive childcare program. The key<br />

research questions are: (1) Will individualized music therapy interventions increase target<br />

children’s independent performance during specific routines; and (2) Can classroom teachers<br />

implement interventions based on music therapy principles in ongoing classroom activities<br />

and routines, when music therapy collaborative consultation is provided?<br />

INTERVENTIONS. Each of three interventions addresses a key difficulty targeted<br />

children face. These correspond to (a) the morning greeting routine (Experiment I); (b)<br />

multiple-step tasks within classroom routines (Experiment II); and (c) peer interactions on<br />

the childcare playground (Experiment III), respectively. To increase target children’s independent<br />

performance, seven uniquely composed songs and two pre-composed songs are used<br />

and an outdoor music center is added to the childcare playground. Children’s individual<br />

educational goals, coupled with the strategies commonly used with children with autism, are<br />

taken into consideration in the design of the intervention and song development.<br />

<strong>Collaborative</strong> consultation, including staff development activities, are provided prior to and<br />

during each intervention.<br />

METHOD. The effects of the song interventions are evaluated by using single-case<br />

experimental designs. These are cumulated as a “case study design.”<br />

Experiment I: Participants in this study are two three-year old children, diagnosed<br />

with ASD (n=2), who exhibit difficulties with the classroom’s morning greeting routine, and<br />

their peers with and without special needs (n=13), classroom teachers (n=5), and the target<br />

children’s respective caregivers (n=2). The aim of the intervention is to increase the<br />

independent performance of target children during the morning greeting routine by way of<br />

iv


v<br />

unique greeting songs implemented by classroom teachers and the inclusion of peers and<br />

caregivers. <strong>Using</strong> an A-B-A-B withdrawal design for subject 1, and a modified version of<br />

this design (A-B-C-A-C withdrawal design) for subject 2, the effectiveness of the song<br />

interventions is evaluated. Categories of behaviors are coded through direct observation<br />

using an event recording system. The experimental design involves two conditions for<br />

subject 1 (Baseline-Intervention) and three conditions (Baseline-Intervention-Modified<br />

Intervention) for subject 2.<br />

Results: Results of this study indicate that during baseline (Condition A) subject 1<br />

has a low level of independent performance (M=33.3%). With the introduction of the song<br />

intervention (Condition B), he steadily becomes more independent (M=54%). The<br />

withdrawal of the song intervention immediately decreases his performance (M=46.7%),<br />

while re-introducing the song increases his independence once again, this time on an even<br />

higher level (M=77.8%). During baseline (Condition A), subject 2 also exhibits a low level<br />

of independent performance (M=23.3%). The initial implementation of the song intervention<br />

(Condition B) does not change his performance significantly (M=30%), while the modified<br />

song intervention (Condition C) increases subject 2’s independent actions significantly and<br />

immediately (M=60%). After withdrawing the song, subject 2’s independent behavior<br />

decreases (M=40%), re-introducing the modified song intervention produces a high level of<br />

independent performance (M=80%). As evaluated in subject 2, the song intervention<br />

changes the classmates’ greeting behavior toward the target child and increases peer<br />

interaction. As for the positive outcomes of the interventions, it can be concluded that<br />

classroom teachers successfully implement interventions based on music therapy principles<br />

into the ongoing classroom routine.<br />

Experiment II: A three-year old boy, diagnosed with ASD (n=1), who has difficulties<br />

managing the sequences required for the multiple-step tasks (i.e., hand-washing, toileting and<br />

cleaning-up independently) is the subject of this study along with his classroom teacher<br />

(n=1). The intentions of this investigation are to evaluate the effectiveness of songs<br />

embedded by the classroom teacher as structural prompts in increasing the independent<br />

performance for the target child during these classroom routines, and to evaluate whether the<br />

musical presentation or the verbal presentation of the sequencing is more effective. <strong>Using</strong> an<br />

alternating treatment design replicated across these three tasks, the effectiveness of the song<br />

intervention (Condition A) versus lyric intervention (Condition B) is compared. Six<br />

categories of behaviors for each multiple-step task are coded through direct observation using<br />

event recording.<br />

Results: Results of this study indicate that the implementation of either form of the<br />

intervention (song intervention - lyric intervention) is successful in increasing the target<br />

child’s independent performance for each multiple-step task. However, the song intervention<br />

is more effective than the lyric intervention for the hand-washing (song intervention M=66%<br />

versus lyric intervention M=57.1%) and cleaning-up procedures (song intervention M=66.6%<br />

versus lyric intervention M=36.7%), whereas for toileting the lyric intervention (M=38.2%)<br />

is slightly more effective than the song intervention (M=32%). As for the child’s positive<br />

learning progress, it can be affirmed that the teacher embeds both forms of the presented<br />

sequencing in the ongoing classroom routines effectively.


Experiment III: This investigation involves four boys, ages three to five, diagnosed<br />

with ASD (n=4), who display a lack of peer interaction on the playground, typically<br />

developing children as well as children with other disabilities (n=32), and their classroom<br />

teachers (n=6). Two peers for each target child are trained as formal peer helpers. The goal<br />

is to improve the target children’s interactions with peers and play and engagement on the<br />

childcare playground. <strong>Using</strong> a multiple baseline design across four subjects, the effectiveness<br />

of the intervention is evaluated. Categories of interaction behaviors are coded through direct<br />

observation using a 15-second momentary time sampling recording procedure. Procedural<br />

fidelity data on the teachers and peers task behaviors are recorded as well. Four sequential<br />

conditions (Baseline [Condition A]; Adaptation of the playground [Condition B]; Teachermediated<br />

intervention [Condition C]; Peer-mediated intervention [Condition D]) are<br />

implemented for all subjects, except condition D, where only three subjects participated.<br />

Results: Results of this study indicate that prior to the musical adaptation of the<br />

playground, the targeted children have few positive peer interactions on the playground<br />

(M=3.2%). The musical adaptation of the playground enhances positive peer interactions<br />

slightly but not significantly (M=7.1%). The teacher-mediated intervention results in an<br />

immediate and significant increase in positive peer interactions (M=66.2%). With only one<br />

exception, a high level of the teacher’s ability to implement the intervention is observed<br />

(M=84%). Peers participate and model the tasks on a high level (M=85.3%). Peer<br />

interaction, meanwhile, decreases during the peer-mediated intervention (M=21.1%) but<br />

significantly improves compared to both the playground adaptation and especially the<br />

baseline. Play and engagement increase significantly for three subjects and remain the same<br />

over conditions for one subject.<br />

CONCLUSION. These studies demonstrate the potential benefits of a cumulative<br />

case study design for music therapists working in early intervention. Single-case<br />

experimental designs enable professionals to evaluate clinical practice and consultation with<br />

other professionals and to ask important questions about the practices and principles of music<br />

therapy in a quantitative and experimental way. The results of each study shed light upon the<br />

broader project while the individual results can be cumulated in a singular discussion.<br />

This cumulative case study supports the contemporary model of service delivery in<br />

early intervention/early childhood special education, and shows that music therapy<br />

interventions can be meaningfully embedded in ongoing classroom activities and routines.<br />

The music therapy collaborative consultative approach was effective in enabling teachers to<br />

implement the interventions successfully. Through individualized song interventions, target<br />

children acquired new skills and/or improved in key deficit areas of autism, such as<br />

transitioning, following multiple-step tasks, and social interactions with peers in the natural<br />

environment.<br />

<strong>Collaborative</strong> consultation, widely employed elsewhere in early intervention/early<br />

childhood special education to promote program sustainability, is an appropriate and<br />

effective way of providing music therapy treatment as well; indeed, it allows for the<br />

expansion of music therapy services more broadly. Overall, music therapy enhances services<br />

for young children with autism, and should be considered as a routine treatment option.<br />

However, training for music therapists in collaborative consultative methods of service<br />

delivery, along with continued research into the effects of embedded music therapy<br />

interventions in inclusive childcare programs, continues to be warranted.<br />

vi


vii


ZUSAMMENFASSUNG<br />

Anwendung eines musiktherapeutischen kollaborativen Beratungsansatzes zur<br />

Integration von Kleinkindern mit Autismus in einer Kindertageseinrichtung<br />

HINTERGRUND. Kleinkinder mit der Diagnose Autistisches Syndrom werden<br />

zunehmend in Kindertageseinrichtungen integriert und erhalten ihre therapeutischen<br />

Massnahmen im Kontext der alltäglichen Gruppenaktivitäten und routinemäßigen Tagesabläufe.<br />

Dafür gibt es mindestens drei Gründe: (a) um Stigmatisierung und Isolation zu<br />

minimieren, (b) um die im kindlichen Umfeld natürlich vorhandenen Lernmöglichkeiten zu<br />

nutzen, und (c) um die Anzahl an lernfördernden Erfahrungen zu erhöhen. Trotz weitverbreiteter<br />

Studien zur Kindergartenintegration hat sich noch keine Forschungsarbeit mit der<br />

Wirkungsweise von musiktherapeutischen Interventionen auf die integrationsfördernde<br />

Behandlung von Kleinkindern mit frühkindlichem Autismus in Kindertageseinrichtungen<br />

unter Anwendung eines kollaborativen Beratungsmodells beschäftigt.<br />

ZIELSETZUNG. Zielsetzung dieser kumulativen Studie ist, die Wirkung zu<br />

überprüfen, die in das Alltagsleben eingebettete musiktherapeutische Interventionen auf die<br />

Handlungsfähigkeit von Kleinkindern mit frühkindlichem Autismus während für sie<br />

schwieriger Gruppen- und Tagesabläufe in einer integrativen Kindertageseinrichtung haben<br />

können. Die zentralen Forschungsfragen sind: (1) Verbessern individualisierte musiktherapeutische<br />

Interventionen das selbständige Handeln der Zielgruppe während spezifischer<br />

Routinen?, und (2): Können Erzieherinnen Interventionen, die auf musiktherapeutischen<br />

Grundprinzipien beruhen, innerhalb der normalen Gruppenaktivitäten und -abläufe ausführen,<br />

sofern musiktherapeutische kollaborative Beratung angeboten wird?<br />

INTERVENTIONEN. Die Interventionen fokussieren Schwierigkeiten, denen die<br />

ausgewählten Kinder gegenüberstehen, und zwar (a) während der morgendlichen<br />

Begrüßungsroutine (Experiment I), (b) bei Aufgaben innerhalb des üblichen Tagesablaufs,<br />

die mehrere Schritte einer komplexen Handlung umfassen (Experiment II), und (c) bei der<br />

Peerinteraktion auf dem Spielplatz der Kindertageseinrichtung (Experiment III). Um das<br />

selbständige Handeln der ausgewählten Kinder zu verbessern, werden sieben Lieder<br />

komponiert und zwei den Kindern bereits bekannte Lieder verwendet; darüber hinaus wird<br />

der Kinderspielplatz um ein Musikpavillon ergänzt. Beim Design der Interventionen und der<br />

Liedentwicklung, werden individuelle pädagogische Ziele der Kinder sowie Strategien, die<br />

generell bei Kindern mit frühkindlichem Autismus angewendet werden berücksichtigt.<br />

Kollaborative Beratung, einschließlich Mitarbeitertraining, werden vor und während jeder<br />

Intervention gewährleistet.<br />

METHODE. Die Wirkung der Liedinterventionen wird durch die Anwendung von<br />

experimentellen Einzelfallstudiendesigns ausgewertet, die zu einem kumulativen Fallstudiendesign<br />

zusammengefasst werden.<br />

viii


ix<br />

Experiment I: Teilnehmer dieser Studie sind zwei 3-jährige Kinder mit der Diagnose<br />

Autistisches Syndrom (n=2), die Schwierigkeiten bei der morgendlichen Begrüßungsroutine<br />

zeigen, sowie ihre Peers mit und ohne Behinderungen (n=13), Erzieherinnen (n=5) und die<br />

Fürsorgenden der ausgewählten Kinder (n=2). Ziel der Intervention ist, das selbständige<br />

Handeln der Kinder während der morgendlichen Begrüßungsroutine zu verbessern, indem<br />

speziell für diese Kinder komponierte Begrüßungslieder verwendet werden, die von den<br />

Erzieherinnen implementiert werden und Peers sowie die Fürsorgenden einbezieht. Mit<br />

einem „A-B-A-B-Withdrawal Design“ für Kind 1 und einer modifizierten Version dieses<br />

Designs („A-B-C-A-C-Withdrawal Design“) für Kind 2 wird die Wirksamkeit der Liedinterventionen<br />

evaluiert. Verhaltenskategorien werden durch unmittelbare Beobachtung mit<br />

einem „event recording system“ codiert. Das experimentelle Design umfasst für Kind 1 zwei<br />

Konditionen (Baseline; Intervention) und für Kind 2 drei Konditionen (Baseline;<br />

Intervention; modifizierte Intervention).<br />

Ergebnisse: Ergebnisse dieser Studie zeigen, dass während der Baseline (Kondition<br />

A) Kind 1 ein niedriges Niveau des selbständigen Handelns (M=33.3%) hat. Mit der<br />

Einführung der Liedintervention (Kondition B) wird es stetig selbständiger (M=54%). Die<br />

Wegnahme der Liedintervention verringert sofort sein selbständiges Handeln (M=46.7%),<br />

während die Wiedereinführung des Liedes seine Selbständigkeit noch einmal erhöht, dieses<br />

mal sogar auf einem höheren Niveau (M=77.8%). Während der Baseline (Kondition A) hat<br />

auch Kind 2 ein niedriges Niveau des selbständigen Handelns (M=23.3%). Die erstmalige<br />

Einführung der Liedintervention (Kondition B) ändert sein selbständiges Handeln geringfügig<br />

(M=30%), während die modifizierte Liedintervention (Kondition C) das selbständige<br />

Handeln sofort erheblich erhöht (M=60%). Nachdem das Lied entzogen wird, verringerte<br />

sich das selbständige Handeln des Kindes 2 (M=40%), die Wiedereinführung der<br />

modifizierten Liedintervention führt zu einem hohen Niveau des selbständigen Handelns<br />

(M=80%). Wie für Kind 2 ausgewertet wird, ändert die Liedintervention das Grußverhalten<br />

der Peers in bezug auf das ausgewählte Kind und erhöht die Peerinteraktion. Aufgrund der<br />

positiven Ergebnisse der Intervention kann gefolgert werden, dass die Erzieherinnen<br />

Interventionen, die auf musiktherapeutischen Grundprinzipien basieren, in die normalen<br />

Gruppenaktivitäten und routinemäßigen Tagesabläufe erfolgreich einführen.<br />

Experiment II: Ein 3-jähriger Junge diagnostiziert mit Autistischem Syndrom (n=1),<br />

der Schwierigkeiten hat, komplexe Handlungsabläufe, wie selbständiges Händewaschen,<br />

Zur-Toilette-Gehen und Aufräumen, zu bewältigen, sowie seine Erzieherin (n=1) nehmen an<br />

dieser Studie teil. Die Intention dieser Untersuchung ist auszuwerten, ob Lieder, die von der<br />

Erzieherin als strukturierte Anleitung in den Handlungsablauf eingebettet werden, das<br />

selbständige Handeln des ausgewählten Kindes während dieser routinemäßigen Gruppenaktiviäten<br />

und -abläufe verbessern und ob die musikalische oder aber die verbale Darbietung<br />

der Sequenz wirkungsvoller ist. Anhand eines über diese drei Handlungsabläufe replizierten<br />

„alternating treatment design“ wird die Wirksamkeit der Liedintervention (Kondition A) mit<br />

der verbalen Intervention (Kondition B) erforscht. Sechs Verhaltenskategorien werden für<br />

jede komplexe Handlung durch direkte Beobachtung der Handlungsabläufe codiert.<br />

Ergebnisse: Ergebnisse dieser Studie zeigen, dass die Implementierung beider<br />

Interventionsformen (Liedintervention, verbale Intervention) erfolgreich ist, um das selbständige<br />

Tun des Kindes innerhalb der jeweiligen komplexen Handlung zu verbessern. Die<br />

Liedintervention ist jedoch wirkungsvoller als die verbale Intervention für das Hände-


waschen (Liedintervention M=66% gegenüber verbaler Intervention M=57.1%) und die<br />

Aufräumaktionen (Liedintervention M=66.6% gegenüber verbaler Intervention M=36.7%),<br />

während für den Toilettengang die verbale Intervention (M=38.2%) gerinfügig wirkungsvoller<br />

als die Liedintervention (M=32%) ist. Aufgrund des positiven Lernfortschritts des<br />

Kindes kann bestätigt werden, dass die Erzieherin beide Formen der dargestellten Sequenzen<br />

erfolgreich in die fortwährenden Gruppenaktivitäten und -abläufe einbettet.<br />

Experiment III: Dieses Experiment involviert vier Jungen mit der Diagnose<br />

Autistisches Syndrom (n=4) im Alter von 3 bis 5 Jahren, die einen Mangel an Peerinteraktion<br />

auf dem Spielplatz aufweisen, normal entwickelte Kinder sowie Kinder mit Behinderungen<br />

(n=32) und ihre Erzieherinnen (n=6). Für jedes ausgewähltes Kind werden zwei formale<br />

Peerhelfer ausgebildet. Ziel ist es Interaktionen mit anderen Kindern sowie das Spiel und die<br />

Tätigkeit der ausgewählten Kinder auf dem Spielplatz der Kindertageseinrichtung zu<br />

verbessern. Die Wirksamkeit der Intervention wird mit einem „Multiple Baseline Design“<br />

ausgewertet, das über die vier Kinder repliziert wird. Interaktionskategorien werden durch<br />

direkte Beobachtung mit einem „15-second momentary time sampling recording procedure“<br />

kodiert. Verfahrensqualitätsdaten bezüglich des Erzieherinnen- und des Peerverhaltens<br />

werden ebenfalls festgehalten. Vier aufeinanderfolgende Konditionen [Baseline (Kondition<br />

A); Modifizierung des Spielplatzes (Kondition B); Erziehervermittelte Intervention<br />

(Kondition C); Peervermittelte Intervention (Intervention D)], ausgenommen Kondition D,<br />

bei der nur drei Kinder teilnehmen, werden für alle Kinder durchgeführt.<br />

Ergebnisse: Ergebnisse dieser Studie zeigen, dass vor der musikalischen<br />

Modifizierung des Spielplatzes die Zielgruppe wenig positive Interaktionen mit anderen<br />

Kindern auf dem Spielplatz (M=3.2%) zutage treten. Die musikalische Modifizierung des<br />

Spielplatzes erhöht die positiven Interaktionen mit anderen Kindern geringfügig, aber nicht<br />

signifikant (M=7.1%). Die erziehervermittelte Intervention resultiert in eine sofortige und<br />

signifikante Zunahme der positiven Peerinteraktionen (M=66.2%). Mit nur einer Ausnahme<br />

wird bei den Erzieherinnen ein hohes Niveau der Fähigkeit die Intervention einzuführen<br />

beobachtet (M=84%). Peers machen mit und bewältigten die Aufgabe auf einem hohen<br />

Niveau (M=85.3%). Die Peerinteraktion nimmt während der peervermittelten Intervention<br />

jedoch ab (M=21.1%), ist aber signifikant höher als vor und mit der Spielplatzmodifikation.<br />

Das Spielen und die Tätigkeiten erhöhen sich signifikant für drei ausgewählten Kinder und<br />

bleiben für ein ausgewählten Kinder über die Konditionen hinweg gleich.<br />

SCHLUSSFOLGERUNG. Die Studien zeigen den potentiellen Vorteil eines<br />

kumulativen Fallstudiendesigns für Musiktherapeuten, die in der Frühförderung tätig sind.<br />

Experimentelle Einzelfallstudiendesigns ermöglichen Fachleuten, klinische Praxis und<br />

Beratung mit anderen Berufsgruppen auszuwerten und wichtige Fragen über die Praxis und<br />

Grundsätze der Musiktherapie in quantitativer und experimenteller Weise zu stellen. Die<br />

Ergebnisse der Studien geben Einblick in das umfangreiche Projekt, während die einzelnen<br />

Ergebnisse in einer Diskussion zusammengefasst werden können.<br />

Diese kumulative Fallstudie unterstützt die derzeit aktuelle Behandlungspraxis in der<br />

Frühförderung und in der Sonderpädagogik und zeigt, dass musiktherapeutische Interventionen<br />

in normale Gruppenaktivitäten und Tagesabläufe eingebettet werden können. Der<br />

musiktherapeutische kollaborative Beratungsansatz ermöglicht es Erzieherinnen, Inter-<br />

x


xi<br />

ventionen erfolgreich einzusetzen. Durch individualisierte Liedinterventionen erwerben die<br />

ausgewählten Kinder neue Fähigkeiten und/oder verbessern zentrale Defizitbereiche des<br />

Syndroms, wie Übergänge, Befolgung von Schritten einer komplexen Handlung oder soziale<br />

Interaktionen mit Peers in ihrer gewohnten Umgebung.<br />

Kollaborative Beratung, die vielfach in der Frühförderung und Sonderpädagogik<br />

angewendet wird, um Nachhaltigkeit zu unterstützen, ist eine angemessene und<br />

wirkungsvolle Art und Weise Behandlungen anzubieten und erweitert zudem das<br />

musiktherapeutische Angebot. Insgesamt verbessert Musiktherapie die therapeutischen<br />

Maßnahmen für Kleinkinder mit frühkindlichem Autismus und sollte routinemäßig als<br />

Behandlungsoption betrachtet werden. Es ist jedoch notwendig, Musiktherapeuten in<br />

kollaborativen Beratungsmethoden auszubilden und die Effekte von eingebetteten<br />

musiktherapeutischen Interventionen in integrativen Kindertageseinrichtungen weiter zu<br />

erforschen.


ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS<br />

I would like to take this opportunity to thank those whose expertise and<br />

companionship had a major impact on my academic and personal growth during the progress<br />

toward my doctorate.<br />

Very special thanks goes to my mentor, Prof. Dr. David Aldridge, for sharing his<br />

immeasurable knowledge and wisdom, generosity and British humor, as well as for his gentle<br />

pushes and challenges, open conversations and vision. Thank you for giving me space and<br />

guidance for my academic and personal growth. Many thanks to Prof. Dr. Lutz Neugebauer<br />

for being an inspiration, and for his open conversations, “Geselligkeit,” and profound ideas.<br />

My deepest appreciation goes to Prof. Dr. Mark Wolery for introducing me to the world of<br />

research. Thank you for your contributions, support, honesty, open heart and belief in me.<br />

I would also like to thank the children and families, teachers and colleagues at the<br />

Frank Porter Graham Childcare program at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.<br />

Without their dedication, collaboration and support, this dissertation would never have<br />

happened. Many thanks to Dr. Debby Cryer, Dr. P. J. McWilliam, Dr. Ann Garfinkle, Mary<br />

Jenne, Maggie Connolly, and Prof. Dr. Don Bailey at the FPG Child Development Institute<br />

for making this research project and my stay in the U.S. possible. Thanks to Prof. Dr. Lee<br />

Marcus and Prof. Dr. Gary Mesibov from Division TEACCH for inviting me to the students’<br />

research meetings.<br />

xii


xiii<br />

Extraordinary thanks to Linn Wakeford, Maureen Furlong, and Rob McCouch for<br />

patiently proof-reading this thesis, and for their encouragement and valuable feedback.<br />

Thanks to Don Trull, Thomas Turanchik, Michael Schuschk and Rainer Dörrer for media and<br />

technical support. Thank you to my colleagues at the University Witten-Herdecke, especially<br />

Dr. Jörg Fachner, for their hospitality, open minds, incredible musicianship and many laughs.<br />

Thanks to my American music therapy colleagues, particularly Laura DeLoye and Yasmine<br />

White, for sharing their expertise, inspiring moments and friendly collaboration.<br />

Thanks to Jon Metzger, my jazz teacher and friend, for keeping me swinging and<br />

nurturing my spirit and soul through music. Deep gratitude goes to Kathleen Hannan, Darby<br />

Ottom, Sonja Steward, and many others thanks for the endless pleasure of jamming and<br />

musicking. Thanks to Friderike Gerling, Susanne Hitschold, Volker Gronwald, Christine<br />

Arnsberger, Nives Ebert, and Tonia Morrison for their friendship, sharing of life experiences<br />

and encouragement. Thanks to my godchildren, Tamara and Fiona, and to Amelie,<br />

Constantin, and Leonie, Jana, Ronya, Anne and Elias, Niklas, Elea, Owen and Anna for<br />

sharing their beautiful children’s spirit, curiosity, wisdom and music.<br />

Thanks to my parents, Alois and Angela Kern, for being my roots and Swabian home,<br />

as well as to Gertraud and Helmut Schmid for their belief in education and being my second<br />

family. My sisters Christine and Sarah Kern, my thanks for sharing family stories,<br />

adventures and love. Last but not least, thanks to Joachim Schmid for his unconditional love,<br />

countless hours of talking and listening, his generosity and belief in me.


TABLE OF CONTENTS<br />

Chapter Page<br />

List of Tables ……………………………………………………………………………. xix<br />

List of Figures .…………………………………………………………………………... xxi<br />

List of Video Excerpts ………………………………….………………………...…… xxiv<br />

List of Audio Excerpts ………………………………………………………………… xxvi<br />

1. Introduction …………………………………………………………………………… 1<br />

2. Literature Review …….……………………………………………………………….. 9<br />

2.1 Early Intervention/Early Childhood Special Education ………………..…. 9<br />

2.1.1 Definitions and Rationale ………………………………………….…... 9<br />

2.1.2 Inclusion …………………………………………………….………… 10<br />

2.1.2.1 Definitions, Philosophy and Rationale ...……………..…..….…… 10<br />

2.1.2.2 Developmentally Appropriate Practice ……………………...…… 12<br />

2.1.3 Early Intervention/Early Childhood Special Education Services …….. 13<br />

2.1.3.1 Embedded Model of Service Delivery …………………………… 13<br />

2.1.3.2 Interdisciplinary Team <strong>Approach</strong> and Individualized Family<br />

xiv<br />

Service Plan/Individualized Education Program …………………. 14<br />

2.1.3.3 Integrated <strong>Therapy</strong> ……………………………………………….. 15<br />

2.1.3.4 Integrated versus Segregated Model of Service Delivery …...…… 18<br />

2.1.4 <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> in Early Intervention/Early Childhood Special<br />

Education ………………………………………………………...…… 19


xv<br />

2.1.4.1 <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> and Children with Special Needs ……………...… 19<br />

2.1.4.2 <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> in Inclusive Childcare Programs ……………….... 20<br />

2.1.4.3 Models of Service Delivery in <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> ……………...…… 25<br />

2.1.4.4 <strong>Collaborative</strong> and <strong>Consultative</strong> Models of Service Delivery in<br />

<strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> …………………………………………………… 26<br />

2.2 Autism ……………………………………………………………………. 29<br />

2.2.1 Definition, Prevalence, Causes and Diagnosis ……………………….. 29<br />

2.2.2 Characteristics of Autism ……………………………………………... 31<br />

2.2.2.1 Social Interaction and Relationships ……………………………... 32<br />

2.2.2.2 Language and Communication Development ………………….… 33<br />

2.2.2.3 Repetitive, Restricted and Stereotypic Behaviors ………………... 33<br />

2.2.2.4 Other Characteristics ……………………………………………... 34<br />

2.2.3 Contemporary Education and Treatments for Young Children with<br />

Autism ……………………………………………………………….... 35<br />

2.2.3.1 Medical Treatments ……………………………………………… 36<br />

2.2.3.2 Educational and Behavioral <strong>Approach</strong>es and Programs …………. 37<br />

2.2.3.3 Complementary Therapies ……………………………………….. 38<br />

2.2.4 Including Children with Autism in Preschool Education Program ...… 39<br />

2.2.4.1 Rationale for Inclusion ………………………………………….... 39<br />

2.2.4.2 Theoretical Framework for Teaching Children with Autism ……. 40<br />

2.2.4.3 Early Intervention Programs for Children with Autism …………. 43<br />

2.2.4.4 Teaching Strategies ………………………………………………. 45<br />

2.2.5 <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> for Children with Autism …………………………..… 51


2.2.5.1 Benefits of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> for Children with Autism …………... 51<br />

2.2.5.2 <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> and Autism: Clinical Practice and Research ….… 52<br />

2.2.5.3 <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> in Inclusive Preschool Education Programs for<br />

xvi<br />

Children with Autism …………………………………………..… 59<br />

2.2.5.4 Inside-Out Reports Related to <strong>Music</strong> ………………………...…. 53<br />

2.2.5.4.1 From Adults’ Perspectives ………………………………... 63<br />

2.2.5.4.2 Interview with a Ten-Year-Old Jazz Pianist …………….... 65<br />

3. Methods………………………………………………………………………………... 71<br />

4. Experiment I: Increasing Independent Performance of Children with Autism<br />

during the Morning Greeting Routine ……………………………………………... 77<br />

4.1 Introduction ……………………………………………………………….… 77<br />

4.2 Research Questions ……………………………………………………….… 79<br />

4.3 Method …………………………………………………………………….… 80<br />

4.3.1 Participants ………………………………………………………….… 80<br />

4.3.2 Setting ………………………………………………………………… 86<br />

4.3.3 Materials …………………………………………………………….... 87<br />

4.3.4 Procedure …………………………………………………………...… 93<br />

4.3.5 Experimental Design ………………………………………………….. 96<br />

4.3.6 Measurements ……………………………………………………….... 98<br />

4.3.7 Reliability ………………………………………………………….… 100<br />

4.4 Results ……………………………………………………………………… 101<br />

4.4.1 Interobserver Agreement …………………………………………….. 101<br />

4.4.2 Outcome Data ……………………………………………………….. 102


xvii<br />

4.5 Discussion ………………………………………………………………..…. 108<br />

5. Experiment II: Increasing Independent Performance of a Child with Autism<br />

during Multiple-Step Tasks ………………………………………………………... 121<br />

5.1 Introduction ……………………………………………………………….. 121<br />

5.2 Research Questions ……………………………………………………..…. 130<br />

5.3 Method ……………………………………………………………………... 130<br />

5.3.1 Participants …………………………………………………………... 130<br />

5.3.2 Setting …………………………………………………………….…. 135<br />

5.3.3 Materials …………………………………………………………….. 137<br />

5.3.4 Procedure ……………………………………………………………. 141<br />

5.3.5 Experimental Design ………………………………………………… 144<br />

5.3.6 Measurements ……………………………………………………..… 145<br />

5.3.7 Reliability ……………………………………………………………. 148<br />

5.4 Results ……………………………………………………………………… 148<br />

5.4.1 Interobserver Agreement ………………………………………….… 148<br />

5.4.2 Outcome Data ……………………………………………………..… 149<br />

5.5 Discussion ………………………………………………………………..… 153<br />

6. Experiment III: Increasing Peer Interaction of Children with Autism on<br />

Childcare Playground through <strong>Music</strong> ……………………………………………. 161<br />

6.1 Introduction ………………………………………………………………... 161<br />

6.2 Research Questions ……………………………………………………...… 165<br />

6.3 Method ………………………………………………………………...…… 165<br />

6.3.1 Participants …………………………………………………………... 165


xviii<br />

6.3.2 Setting ……………………………………………………………..… 177<br />

6.3.3 Materials …………………………………………………………..…. 180<br />

6.3.4 Procedure ……………………………………………………………. 199<br />

6.3.5 Experimental Design ………………………………………………… 204<br />

6.3.6 Measurements …………………………………………………..…… 206<br />

6.3.7 Reliability ………………………………………………………….… 210<br />

6.4 Results ……………………………………………………………………… 211<br />

6.4.1 Interobserver Agreement ……………………………………………. 211<br />

6.4.2 Outcome Data ………………………………………………..……… 212<br />

6.5 Discussion …………………………………………………………..……… 241<br />

7. General Discussion……………………………………………………………...…… 263<br />

8. Conclusions ……………………………………………………………………..…… 273<br />

References……………………………………………………………………………...… 277<br />

Appendices …………………………………………………………………………...…. 315<br />

Appendix A: Data Sheet Experiment I ……………………………………… 315<br />

Appendix B: Data Sheet Experiment II ……………………………………... 317<br />

Appendix C: Data Sheets Experiment III …………………………………… 318


xix<br />

LIST OF TABLES<br />

Table 3.1 Participant composition of experimental groups ……………………… 74<br />

Table 4.1 Overview of Phillip’s and Ben’s IEP goals incorporated in “Song<br />

for Phillip” and “Song for Ben” ………………………………...….… 89<br />

Table 4.2 Interobserver agreement in Conditions A, B, and C …………..…...…. 102<br />

Table 5.1 Interobserver agreement over Task #1, #2, and #3 ………………..…. 149<br />

Table 6.1 Summary of characteristics for each subject …………………..….…. 172<br />

Table 6.2 Overview of Eric’s IEP goals incorporated in the song<br />

“Hey You” …………………………………………………………..... 188<br />

Table 6.3 Overview of Ben’s IEP goals incorporated in the song<br />

“You and I”………………………………………………………….... 190<br />

Table 6.4 Overview of Phillip’s IEP goals incorporated in the song “Phillip’s<br />

Groove” ……………………………………………………………… 193<br />

Table 6.5 Overview of Lucas’ IEP goals incorporated in the song “Lucas’<br />

Dance” …………………………………………..…………………… 196<br />

Table 6.6 Interobserver agreement for each target child in conditions<br />

A, B, C, and D ……………………………………..………………… 212<br />

Table 6.7 Range (Min% and Max%), Mean (M%), and Standard Deviation (SD) of<br />

positive peer interaction for each target child, by each condition of<br />

Experiment III …………………………………………………..…… 213<br />

Table 6.8 Range (Min% and Max%), Mean (M), and Standard Deviation (SD)<br />

of teacher task behavior for each target child in Condition C …….… 223<br />

Table 6.9. Range (Min % and Max %), and Standard Deviation (SD) of peer


task behavior for each target child in Condition D …..……………… 228<br />

Table 6.10 Range (Min% and Max%), Mean (M%), and Standard Deviation<br />

(SD) of stays in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut for each target child, by Conditions<br />

B, C, and D of Experiment III ...……………………………………… 232<br />

Table 6.11 Range (Min% and Max%), Mean (M%), and Standard Deviation<br />

(SD) of play and engagement for each target child, by each<br />

condition of Experiment III ………………………………………… 237<br />

xx


xxi<br />

LIST OF FIGURES<br />

Figure 4.1 “Hello” picture symbol employed to greet classroom teachers and<br />

peers during the morning greeting routine ……..…………………… 87<br />

Figure 4.2. Transcript of ”Song for Phillip,” written to address Phillip’s<br />

demands of the morning greeting routine, social interaction and<br />

specific IEP goals ……………………………………………….…….. 91<br />

Figure 4.3 Transcript of ”Song for Ben,” written to address Ben’s demands of<br />

the morning greeting routine, social interaction and specific IEP<br />

goals …………………………………………………………………... 92<br />

Figure 4.4 Number of independent responses performed by Phillip during the<br />

morning greeting routine in baseline and intervention sessions …..… 104<br />

Figure 4.5 Number of independent responses performed by Ben during the<br />

morning greeting routine in baseline, intervention, and modified<br />

intervention sessions ……………………………...……………….… 106<br />

Figure 4.6 Number of peers greeting Ben during the morning greeting routine<br />

in baseline, intervention, and modified intervention sessions …….… 107<br />

Figure 4.7 Transcript of the song ”Wait,” written for Phillip addressing the<br />

ability to wait ………………………………………………...……… 112<br />

Figure 5.1 Transcript of the song ”Let’s go potty,” written for Andy,<br />

Addressing the demands of his toileting routine while in the<br />

classroom …………………………………………………….……… 140<br />

Figure 5.2 Number of independent steps correctly performed by Andy for<br />

hand-washing, toilet training, and cleaning-up …….………………… 151


Figure 6.1 Playground equipment, including the Sound Path on the FPG<br />

xxii<br />

childcare program’s playground ………………………………..…… 179<br />

Figure 6.2 Panorama view of the outdoor music center (<strong>Music</strong> Hut), showing<br />

the instruments and construction …….……………………………… 181<br />

Figure 6.3 Transcript of the medley: “Everybody in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut,” written<br />

for the Grand Opening of the <strong>Music</strong> Hut ……………………………. 187<br />

Figure 6.4 Transcript of the song ”Hey You,” written for Ben addressing<br />

social interaction and specific IEP goals …………………………..… 189<br />

Figure 6.5 Transcript of the song ”You and I,” written for Ben addressing<br />

social interaction and specific IEP goals ……………………………. 192<br />

Figure 6.6 Transcript of the song ”Phillip’s Groove,” written for Phillip<br />

addressing social interaction and specific IEP goals ……………...… 194<br />

Figure 6.7 Transcript of the song ”Lucas’ Dance,” written for Lucas<br />

addressing social interaction and specific IEP goals ...………………. 198<br />

Figure 6.8 Percentage of positive peer interactions for each target child, by<br />

each condition of Experiment III ……………………………….…… 214<br />

Figure 6.9 Percentage of positive peer interactions for Eric during Conditions<br />

A, B, and C ……………………………………………...…………… 216<br />

Figure 6.10 Percentage of positive peer interactions for Ben during Conditions<br />

A, B, C, and D …………. …………………………………………… 218<br />

Figure 6.11 Percentage of positive peer interactions for Phillip during<br />

Conditions A, B, C, and D …………………………………………… 220


xxiii<br />

Figure 6.12 Percentage of positive peer interactions for Lucas during<br />

Conditions A, B, C, and D …………………………………………… 222<br />

Figure 6.13 Teacher’s Task behaviors and positive peer interactions in<br />

Condition C for Eric (top panel), Ben (second panel), Phillip<br />

(third panel), and Lucas (bottom panel) ……………………………… 224<br />

Figure 6.14 Peer’s task behaviors and positive peer interactions in<br />

Condition D for Ben (top panel), Phillip (middle panel), and Lucas<br />

(bottom panel) ……………………………………………………….. 229<br />

Figure 6.15 Percentage of positive peer interactions and stays in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut<br />

for Eric (top panel), Ben (second panel), Phillip (third panel), and<br />

Lucas (bottom panel), in all conditions ….….……………………….. 233<br />

Figure 6.16 Percentage of play and engagement and stays in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut for<br />

Eric (top panel), Ben (second panel), Phillip (third panel), and Lucas<br />

(bottom panel), in all conditions …………………………………….. 238


LIST OF VIDEO EXCERPTS<br />

Video Excerpt 4.1 Phillip’s morning greeting time behaviors during baseline<br />

xxiv<br />

condition ………………………………………………………… 83<br />

Video Excerpt 4.2 Ben’s morning greeting time behaviors during baseline<br />

condition ……………………………………………...………… 86<br />

Video Excerpt 4.3 Phillip, his classroom peers, parents and teachers during a<br />

song intervention session (Condition B) of the morning<br />

greeting routine …………………………………………………. 111<br />

Video Excerpt 4.4 Ben, his classroom peers, and teachers during a modified<br />

intervention session (Condition C) of the morning greeting<br />

routine …………………………….…………………………… 115<br />

Video Excerpt 5.1 Andy performing the multiple steps included in cleaning-up<br />

during lyric intervention and song intervention ………..……… 157<br />

Video Excerpt 6.1 Video collage of Eric’s typical playground behaviors<br />

during baseline condition ………….…………………………… 167<br />

Video Excerpt 6.2 Video collage of Ben’s typical playground behaviors during<br />

baseline condition ………….…………………………..……… 169<br />

Video Excerpt 6.3 Video collage of Phillip’s typical playground behaviors during<br />

baseline condition ………….………………………………..… 170<br />

Video Excerpt 6.4 Video collage of Lucas’ typical playground behaviors during<br />

baseline condition ………….………………………………..… 172<br />

Video Excerpt 6.5 Eric’s performance during the teacher-mediated intervention<br />

phase of the playground interaction study ……………..………. 245


xxv<br />

Video Excerpt 6.6 Ben’s performance during the teacher and peer-mediated<br />

intervention phase of the playground interaction study ……….. 248<br />

Video Excerpt 6.7 Phillip’s performance during the teacher and peer-mediated<br />

intervention phase of the playground interaction study …..…… 251<br />

Video Excerpt 6.8 Lucas’ performance during the teacher and peer-mediated<br />

intervention phase of the playground interaction study ……...… 254


LIST OF AUDIO EXCERPTS<br />

Audio Excerpt 5.1 Recording of the first part of the hand-washing song, sung<br />

xxvi<br />

by Andy’s classroom teacher …….……………….………….… 138<br />

Audio Excerpt 5.2 Recording of the song “Let’s go potty,” featuring the<br />

classroom teacher on vocals …………….……………………… 139<br />

Audio Excerpt 5.3 Recording of the first part of the cleaning-up song, sung by<br />

the children of the FPG childcare program …………….…….… 141


xxvii


1. Introduction<br />

“Children are the touchstone of a healthy and sustainable society. How a culture<br />

treats its youngest members has a significant influence on how it will grow, prosper, and be<br />

viewed by others.”<br />

(Meisels & Shonkoff, 2000, p. 3)<br />

Providing support and assistance to young children with special needs, and to their<br />

families, is an important act for both the individual and the society. Early education and<br />

treatment of children with special needs, including those with autism, significantly enhances<br />

a child’s ability to participate meaningfully in family and community life, and to play a vital<br />

role in society (Autism Society of America, 2003; TEACCH, 1999; Wolery et al., 2001).<br />

Because the socio-cultural environment is an integral force in a child’s learning and<br />

development (DEC, 2000; Vygotsky, 1978, Wolery & Wilbers, 1994), full inclusion of<br />

children with special needs in their natural environments – such as early childhood programs<br />

– has been supported on ethical, legal and educational grounds (Wilson, 2002; Wolery &<br />

Wilbers, 1994). For young children with autism, whose social interactions and relationships,<br />

language and communication, and behaviors are significantly affected, inclusion greatly<br />

enhances the child’s development and quality of life in numerous ways (American<br />

Psychiatric Association, 2000; Autism Society of America, 2003; National Research Council,<br />

2001). Early experiments on inclusion give children with autism the opportunity to imitate<br />

peer models, interact and communicate with competent partners, provide realistic learning<br />

experiences in a natural environment, and foster friendships with typically developing peers.<br />

Typically developing peers involved in such experiments, for their part, might likewise<br />

1


2<br />

improve their social skills and attitudes toward individuals with disabilities (Buysse, 1993;<br />

Jellison, 1985; Peck, Odom, & Bricker, 1993; Wolery & Wilbers, 1994).<br />

This knowledge has led to a trend toward more widespread inclusion of young<br />

children with autism in community-based preschools or childcare programs (Dawson &<br />

Osterling, 1997; Handleman & Harris, 2001; National Research Council, 2001). But to<br />

ensure successful inclusion, and to meet individual learning and developmental needs, the<br />

application of specific, individualized interventions and strategies is imperative (Sandall,<br />

McLean, & Smith, 2000; Wolery & Wilbers, 1994). Several educational and therapeutic<br />

interventions and strategies have been suggested for effectively including and educating<br />

children with autism (Koegel & Koegel, 1995; National Research Council, 2001). Towards<br />

this end, the Division of Early Childhood (DEC) (Sandall et al., 2000), as well as the<br />

Committee on Educational Interventions for Children with Autism (National Research<br />

Council, 2001), recommend that interventions be embedded in ongoing classroom routines.<br />

The rationale for this procedure is at least threefold: (a) to minimize stigma and isolation; (b)<br />

to capitalize on children’s naturally occurring learning opportunities; and (c) to increase the<br />

number of experiences that promote learning (McWilliam, 1996; Wolery & Wilbers, 1994).<br />

Contemporary service delivery, therefore, is based on an integrated therapy model – that is,<br />

that specialized therapies are delivered in the context of the child’s naturally occurring<br />

environments, routines and activities (McWilliam, 1996). To ensure normalization,<br />

continuity, maintenance and generalization, therapists work with the individual child or a<br />

group of children within the ongoing classroom routines, or as consultants to the classroom<br />

staff and families to embed therapeutic goals (National Research Council, 2001; McWilliam,


1996). Occupational therapists (Dunn, 1996; Sandler, 1997), speech/language pathologists<br />

(Wilcox & Shannon, 1996), physical therapists (Rainforth & Roberts, 1996; Sandler, 1997)<br />

and special educators (Garfinkel & Schwartz, 2002; Venn et al., 1993; Wolery et al., 2002)<br />

have evaluated and successfully applied this approach in inclusive childcare settings. But<br />

this approach has not been widely applied, nor has it yet been evaluated by music therapists.<br />

<strong>Music</strong> therapy has a long tradition of serving young children with special needs,<br />

especially those with autism (Alvin & Warwick, 1991; Nordoff & Robbins, 1977). Studies<br />

on interest in music and relative strength of musical abilities in children with autism<br />

(Applebaum et al., 1979; Blackstock, 1978; Thaut, 1987, 1988) and anecdotal reports on the<br />

quality of music as a means of nonverbal communication, social contact and self-expression<br />

(Alvin & Warwick, 1991; Nelson, 1984; Nordoff & Robbins, 1968a; Schuhmacher, 1994),<br />

suggests that music therapy is a suitable treatment option for individuals with autism. <strong>Music</strong><br />

therapy interventions address the challenges associated with autism in an intentional and<br />

developmentally appropriate manner, and might be effective in facilitating development of<br />

core skills and personal growth. Key strategies applied for educating children with autism –<br />

such as individualization, structure and predictability, and emphasis on the child’s strengths<br />

and individual needs – are incorporated in music therapy treatments or are part of the nature<br />

of music itself (AMTA, 2003c). <strong>Music</strong> therapy also strongly supports and facilitates<br />

inclusion of children with special needs in various educational settings (Wilson, 2002). That<br />

said, the effectiveness of music therapy interventions for the inclusion and improvement of<br />

core skills in young children with autism enrolled in inclusive preschool settings is<br />

3


4<br />

documented by few (Furman, 2001, 2002; Humpal & Wolf 2003; Snell, 2002); and<br />

controlled studies of any kind are missing alltogether.<br />

Although music therapy services can either be provided directly to clients or through<br />

consultation with professionals and others directly involved with the client (AMTA 2003a),<br />

only 13% of the music therapists in the U.S. provide consultative services (Smith &<br />

Harrison, 1999). Some attention has been paid to embedded treatments and the collaborative<br />

and consultative models of music therapy service delivery as effective strategies for<br />

including students with special needs within public school settings (Furman, 2002; Humpal,<br />

2002; Johnson, 2002; Snell, 2002). But the literature that has ensued – program descriptions,<br />

guidelines, strategies, and benefits of this model of service delivery – is not the result of<br />

formal scholarly research investigations, but rather of formative best practices research.<br />

Additionally, no information is currently available to describe whether or not the integrated<br />

models of service delivery, especially collaborative and consultative strategies, are common<br />

clinical practice in music therapy for serving young children with special needs in inclusive<br />

preschool settings.<br />

The purpose of the following series of studies was to understand if individually<br />

designed music therapy interventions will increase the performance of young children with<br />

autism during challenging routines in an inclusive childcare program, and to learn if the<br />

interventions can be embedded in the ongoing classroom routine by teachers. The<br />

interventions addressed difficulties the targeted children faced during (a) the morning


greeting routine, (b) multiple-step tasks within classroom routines, and (c) peer interactions<br />

on the childcare playground.<br />

For many children – those with and without disabilities alike – one of the most crucial<br />

transitions is that from home to preschool (Alger, 1984). Making transitions is particularly<br />

difficult for children with autism. The preferences for sameness and sensitivity to changes,<br />

as well as the lack of understanding of symbolic gestures (such as waving for greeting or<br />

good-bye), are among the defining characteristics of autism (Dawson & Osterling, 1997;<br />

Mesibov et al., 1997).<br />

The childcare day is not only filled with transitions, but also with many routines, such<br />

as hand-washing, toileting and cleaning-up, which are repeated on a daily time schedule.<br />

These routines require children to follow a sequence of steps that typically cause significant<br />

difficulty for children with autism (Boswell & Gray, 2003). In order to function<br />

independently in daily life, children must learn to understand, remember and perform the<br />

sequence of these tasks.<br />

During the childcare day, children spend large blocks of time in outdoor play. The<br />

large undefined space, unstructured playtime, and play styles on the playground is highly<br />

challenging for children with autism as measured by the ability to engage in meaningful play<br />

and interact with peers. The severe delay in understanding social relationships and<br />

communication as a defining characteristic of autism often results in a lack of peer<br />

interaction (Danko & Buysse, 2002; Quill, 2001). To ensure the time spent on childcare<br />

5


6<br />

playgrounds promotes the development of children with autism, predictable play routines and<br />

different play activities that support these children’s interests and strengths need to be<br />

identified and established.<br />

Diverse cultures’ use of song to accompany daily life activities and to heal (Hart,<br />

1990; Morgan, 1994; Silver, 2003); as well as the wide range of professionals using songs<br />

with preschoolers to address academic, social, language and communication and motor skills<br />

and to express emotions (Enoch, 2002; Furman, 2001; Humpal, 1998), led to the design of<br />

song interventions. The playground used in this study has been modified by adding an<br />

outdoor music center, so as to structure and create a musical environment that enhances the<br />

social and play experiences of young children with autism, together with their peers, on the<br />

playground.<br />

The music therapy interventions were designed and implemented by using a<br />

collaborative consultative model of service delivery. Staff development activities on the use<br />

of music therapy techniques to implement the intervention were provided prior to the studies.<br />

In addition to the teacher-mediated interventions, peer-mediated strategies were applied to<br />

increase peer interactions on the childcare playground. Parents and caregivers were fully<br />

included in the morning greeting routine intervention and supported through frequent<br />

communication prior to, during and after the experiments.


The effects of the music therapy interventions were evaluated by using single-case<br />

experimental designs (Aldridge, 1996; Kazdin, 1982; Tawney & Gast, 1984; Wolery, Bailey,<br />

& Sugai, 1988).<br />

The following chapters will provide a detailed review of related literature and a<br />

separate description of each Experiment, including a specific Introduction, Method, Results<br />

and Discussion followed by a General Discussion and Conclusions of this thesis.<br />

7


2. Literature Review<br />

2.1 Early Intervention/Early Childhood Special Education<br />

2.1.1 Definitions and Rationale<br />

Early Intervention/Early Childhood Special Education provides support to young<br />

children with special needs, their families and community to promote the child’s<br />

development and inclusion. Children with special needs are those who are gifted and<br />

talented, as well as those who have disabilities or developmental delays or are at risk for<br />

future morbidity (DEC, 1998). U.S. legislation distinguishes early childhood intervention<br />

services (children from birth through age three) from early childhood special education<br />

services (children ages three through eight) (DEC, 2000; IDEA, 1997; Mellard, 2000). Both<br />

Early Intervention and Early Childhood Special Education services include therapy,<br />

education, health and medical services, assisting technology support, transportation,<br />

integration support in early childhood services, formal and informal family support, and the<br />

provision of information and support for transition into school systems (IDEA, 1997;<br />

Mellard, 2000). While these services are available for all children identified with special<br />

needs, the remainder of this thesis will focus on services for children with disabilities and<br />

developmental delays.<br />

Recognizing and respecting the importance and influence of family in a child’s life,<br />

Early Intervention / Early Childhood Special Education services are delivered by using a<br />

family-centered approach. This means services are delivered within the context of the family<br />

and as a response to the concerns and priorities established by the family (Bailey, 1994;<br />

9


10<br />

Dunst et al., 1996; McWilliam, Tocci, & Harbin, 1998). Services may be delivered in the<br />

family’s home, in early childhood intervention centers, in preschools or daycare centers, or in<br />

hospitals or community health centers. The reason for providing services as early as possible<br />

are at least threefold: (1) to enhance the child’s development during her/his state of<br />

maximum readiness for learning; (2) to support the family’s well-being which might be<br />

affected by the child’s condition and affect the child’s development; and (3) to maximize<br />

long-term social and economic benefits to society by decreasing dependency upon social<br />

institutions and increasing the child’s independent performance in society (Wolery &<br />

Wilbers, 1994). The positive consequences and lasting effects of early intervention are<br />

evident in a number of research reports (e.g. Barnett, 1998; Caine & Caine, 1991; Casto &<br />

Mastropieri, 1986; Jonsdottir, 2002; Stern, 1985; Lazar & Darlington, 1982; Schweinhart &<br />

Weikart, 1996).<br />

2.1.2. Inclusion<br />

2.1.2.1 Definitions, Philosophy and Rationale<br />

One aspect of Early Intervention / Early Childhood Special Education is the inclusion<br />

of children with special needs in their natural environments. Natural environments are<br />

generally thought of as those in which the child would typically spend time, regardless of any<br />

special needs (DEC, 1998; McWilliam, 2000). Given the structure of U.S. society, many<br />

families require out of home care for their young children so that local preschools or fullday<br />

childcare programs are often a natural environment (Cryer, Hurwitz, & Wolery, 1999; Scar,<br />

1998; Willer et al., 1991). Serving young children with special needs in community-based<br />

childcare programs together with their typically developing peers, rather than in special


programs, is called inclusion (Buysse et al., 2000; DEC, 2000). Inclusion begins with the<br />

underlying assumption that children with special needs have more in common with typically<br />

developing children than what separates them by their disability; they can participate actively<br />

in natural environments within their community; and the socio-cultural environment has a<br />

positive influence on the child’s learning and development (DEC, 1998; Gold et al., 1999,<br />

DEC, 2000, Salisbury, 1991; Wolery & Wilbers, 1994).<br />

“Development appears to be a result of the ongoing interactions and transactions<br />

between a biologically maturing organism with a specific genetic endowment and the social<br />

and physical dimensions of the environment embedded within a complex ecological system”<br />

(Wolery, 2000, p. 190). The influence and importance of the child’s experiences with the<br />

social and physical environment on her/his learning and development is discussed in most<br />

prominent theories of child development. Piaget (Piaget, 1951; in Berk, 1997) described four<br />

aspects influencing the child’s cognitive development: (1) interaction with the social world;<br />

(2) interaction with the physical world; (3) equilibration; and (4) biological maturation.<br />

Vygotsky (1978; Berk & Winsler, 2002) recognized in his sociocultural theory that all higher<br />

cognitive processes develop out of the child’s social interactions with members of her/his<br />

society. He stresses the influence of joint activities with more mature members of the society<br />

(adults and peers) on the child’s mastery of activities and thinking, in ways that have<br />

meaning in her or his culture. And Stern (1985) maintained that the infant’s core self is in a<br />

relationship with the core self of the caregiver and that of others of significance. This<br />

relationship forms a crucial axis of development.<br />

11


12<br />

The importance of the natural environment, alongside inclusion and free appropriate<br />

public education, is also recognized and manifested in the U.S. legislation with the<br />

Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA) of 1997. An extensive body of literature<br />

finds that providing inclusive services for children with special needs has positive influences<br />

and benefits for all children and their families, whether with or without special needs. For<br />

example, children with special needs have peer models for learning and development,<br />

competent partners with whom to interact and communicate, realistic learning experiences in<br />

a natural environment, and opportunities of finding friends without disabilities. At the same<br />

time, their typically developing peers may improve their social skills, learn altruistic actions,<br />

and develop positive attitudes toward individuals with disabilities. The families of children<br />

with disabilities might feel more integrated in the community and be spared negative effects<br />

such as labeling or feeling different and not accepted (e.g. Bricker, 1978; Buysse, 1993;<br />

Davis, 1990; Hundert et al., 1998; Humpal, 1991; Jellison, 1985; Peck, Carlson, &<br />

Helmstetter, 1992; Peck, Odom, & Bricker, 1993; Odom & Diamond, 1998; Wolery &<br />

Wilbers, 1994).<br />

2.1.2.2 Developmentally Appropriate Practice<br />

Contemporary models of inclusion in high-quality childcare programs are based on<br />

the concept of developmentally appropriate practice (Bredekamp & Copple, 2002; Carta,<br />

1994; DEC, 2000; Gold et al., 1999; NAEYC, 1996). Developmentally appropriate practice<br />

is built on two dimensions: age-appropriateness and individual-appropriateness. Age-<br />

appropriateness refers to relatively predictable age-related sequences of the physical,<br />

emotional, social and cognitive development in young children. Individual-appropriateness


acknowledges that each child is unique in her/his development and allows practice to<br />

accommodate individual differences in personality, learning styles, growth, strength,<br />

interests, experiences, and family background. To be most beneficial to children’s<br />

development, an appropriate learning environment is provided and appropriate experiences<br />

are planned by the childcare program based on this knowledge in response to individual<br />

needs, interest and abilities (Bredekamp, 1991). These guidelines, developed for general<br />

early childhood education, can be the baseline for Early Intervention/Early Childhood<br />

Special Education, but might need modifications and adaptations to ensure that the individual<br />

needs of children with special needs are met (Carta, 1995; Sandall, McLean, & Smith, 2000;<br />

Wolery, Strain, & Bailey, 1992; Wolery & Bredekamp, 1994). Substantial modifications and<br />

adaptations for special early childhood education practice are set forth in several sources,<br />

among them the Division of Early Childhood (DEC) and National Association for the<br />

Educating Young Children (NAEYC) documents (Bredekamp & Copple, 2002; DEC, 1993;<br />

Sandall, McLean, & Smith, 2000).<br />

2.1.3 Early Intervention / Early Childhood Special Education Services<br />

2.1.3.1 Embedded Model of Service Delivery<br />

Many studies conducted in inclusive childcare programs indicated that, prior to the<br />

application of specific interventions, children’s needs for learning and development were not<br />

being met (Venn et al., 1993; 1997; Kaiser, Yoder, & Keetz, 1992). The inclusion of<br />

children with special needs in community-based childcare programs is only sufficient and<br />

successful if the children’s learning and developmental needs are also addressed. Therefore,<br />

the application of specific individualized interventions might be required (Lamorey &<br />

13


14<br />

Bricker, 1993; Wolery & Wilbers, 1994). It is recommended practice that young children<br />

with special needs receive their special and related services embedded in ongoing classroom<br />

routines and activities (Buysse & Bailey, 1993; Lamorey & Bricker, 1993; Sandall, McLean,<br />

& Smith, 2000). Bricker, Pretti-Frontczak, and McComas (1998) define embedded as “a<br />

procedure in which children are given opportunities to practice individual goals and<br />

objectives that are included within an activity or even that expands, modifies or adapts the<br />

activity/event while remaining meaningful and interesting to children” (p. 73). The<br />

rationales for inclusive service delivery are manifold: (a) to minimize stigma and isolation,<br />

by having the child remain in class with her/his peers; (b) to capitalize on the child’s<br />

naturally occurring learning opportunities by providing support in context; (c) to increase the<br />

number of experiences that promote learning, by addressing the problem whenever it occurs;<br />

(d) to promote social competence by keeping the child involved in activities with classroom<br />

peers; (e) to increase generalization by practicing the skills needed in the place they are<br />

needed; and (f) to ensure consistency by having all the adults working with a child be aware<br />

of why and how to provide intervention (McWilliam, 1996; Wolery & Wilbers, 1994).<br />

2.1.3.2 Interdisciplinary Team <strong>Approach</strong> and Individualized Family Service<br />

Plan/Individualized Education Program<br />

In order to understand and address the complex needs of young children with<br />

disabilities and their families, a team (including family members and members across<br />

professional disciplines) is needed to ensure the availability of adequate support and services.<br />

In Early Intervention/Early Childhood Special Education, it is recommended that<br />

professionals work in interdisciplinary teams. Each specialist is responsible for his area of


expertise, but may work on the same goals as other team member, and receives support from<br />

other team members in achieving the goal (transdisciplinary) (Johnson, 2002; Mackey, &<br />

McQueen, 1998; McWilliam, 1996; Wolery, & Wilbers, 1994). The philosophical<br />

foundation of service delivery is the collaboration of these individuals to ensure the child’s<br />

learning and developmental needs are addressed. Both planning and implementing services<br />

require a collaborative team process (Bruder, 1994, 1996; DEC, 1998, McWilliam, 1996;<br />

Wolery & Wilbers, 1994); this process is documented in writing, establishing an<br />

Individualized Family Service Plan (IFSP) for children under three years or an Individualized<br />

Education Program (IEP), for children over three years of age. The IFSP includes the child’s<br />

present level of development, family resources, priorities, and concerns, child and family<br />

goals, procedure, intensity, location and duration of early intervention services, identification<br />

of a service coordinator, and a transition plan to further services. The IEP identifies and<br />

describes each child’s present individual performance, short-term and long-term goals for<br />

service delivery, frequency, duration, and location of services, as well as strategies and<br />

examples how to achieve those goals (Bailey, 1994; Mellard, 2000; Humpal, 2002).<br />

2.1.3.3 Integrated <strong>Therapy</strong><br />

One innovative model of service delivery in high-quality center-based childcare<br />

programs is integrated therapy. In an integrated therapy approach, therapists provide services<br />

to a child with special needs within the context of the child’s natural environment, routines<br />

and activities. <strong>Using</strong> the integrated therapy model in inclusive classrooms, therapists work<br />

with the individual child or a group of children within the ongoing classroom routines or as a<br />

consultant to the classroom teachers (Mackey & McQueen, 1997; McWilliam, 1996, 2000).<br />

15


16<br />

The following continuum of six models of service delivery within a childcare setting, from<br />

most segregated to the most integrated, was defined by McWilliam and colleagues<br />

(McWilliam, 1995, 1996; Scott, McWilliam, & Mayhew, 1999):<br />

• Individual pull-out. The focal child is removed from the classroom to a separate<br />

location and the therapy focuses directly on the child’s functioning. Teachers provide<br />

information before the session and receive information afterwards.<br />

• Small-group pull-out. In addition to the focal child, one to six peers with and/or<br />

without disabilities are taken out of the classroom for a group session. The therapy<br />

focuses directly on the functioning of the focal child or children with special needs.<br />

Teachers decide which peers will participate, and provide information before the<br />

session and receive information afterwards.<br />

• One-to-one in classroom. <strong>Therapy</strong> is provided to the focal child in the classroom, but<br />

separated from her/his peers and ongoing classroom activities. The therapy focuses<br />

directly on the child’s functioning. This approach is used primarily for assessment or<br />

experimentation with new interventions or equipment, with the expectation that it<br />

might later be incorporated into classroom routines and activities. It is distinct from<br />

individual within routines, because the child is actively removed from ongoing<br />

activities. Peers might be present, but not involved in, the therapy. The teachers<br />

involve the other children and keep them from disrupting the therapy. Teachers<br />

watch the therapy session, providing and receiving information after the session.<br />

• Group Activity. The group therapy approach includes the focal child and peers with<br />

and without disabilities within the classroom routine. The therapy focuses on social<br />

skills of all children and emphasizes focal child’s needs. The teachers participate in


group activities and planning. Teachers’ role can be different (leading/assisting) or<br />

the same (co-therapist/co-teaching), can switch back and forth during the activities, or<br />

from day to day.<br />

• Individual within routines. The therapist works with the child within the context of<br />

ongoing classroom routines and activities. The focus of therapy is directly, but not<br />

exclusively on the target child. If possible, the teacher is actively involved. If not,<br />

therapist and teacher consult with each other during allocated therapy time.<br />

• Consultation. The therapist works primarily with the teacher, rather than with the<br />

child. The purpose is for the therapist and teachers to collaborate on the best<br />

approach the teaching staff should use in order to meet the child’s identified goals.<br />

Consultation can range from “expert consultation,” where the therapist provides<br />

guidance, information, and training on the use of therapeutic techniques, to<br />

“collaborative consultation,” where the therapist (consultant) and teacher engage<br />

equally in defining the problem; identifying the goals of intervention; planning the<br />

intervention; and engaging in follow-ups. The therapist provides training for teachers<br />

on the use of therapeutic strategies and supports the teachers during implementation<br />

(Achilles, Yates, & Freese, 1991; Buysse et al., 1994; Bruder, 1996).<br />

When using the integrative model of service delivery, developmental and functional<br />

goals (i.e. those that are useful for a child’s life circumstances) for the child’s IFSP/IEP<br />

should be developed through routine-based assessment rather than domain-based assessment.<br />

Routine-based assessment is the process of identifying skills a child needs to perform<br />

successfully and independently in the place where she/he spends time. This is accomplished<br />

17


18<br />

by family interviews and observation of the child in the context, where the problems arise.<br />

Skills that are immediately useful for the child are identified, rather than applying a priori<br />

developmental prerequisites or foundations for future needs. Routine-based assessment<br />

considers the whole child and increases the chance that goals and strategies are relevant and<br />

realistic (McWilliam, 1992, 1996; Wolery, 1996).<br />

2.1.3.4. Integrated versus Segregated Model of Service Delivery<br />

Research addressing the effectiveness of integrated services versus segregated<br />

therapy shows that there is no evidence for the superiority of one model over the other.<br />

Integrated therapy has similar effects as segregated therapy on the child’s acquisition,<br />

generalization, and goal-related progress (Cole at al., 1989; Davies & Gavin, 1994; Hundert<br />

et al., 1998; McWilliam & Bailey, 1994; Wilcox, Kouri, & Caswell, 1991). Additionally,<br />

Cole, Harris, Eland, and Mills (1989) find that classroom staff prefers an integrated versus a<br />

segregated model of therapy. Dunn (1990) reports that teachers contributed more to the<br />

accomplishment of children’s IEP goals when engaging in collaborative consultation with<br />

occupational therapists and had better comments on an attitude scale compared to teachers<br />

whose children received segregated therapy. Palsha and Wesley (1998) provide evidence<br />

that on-site consultation and collaboration in childcare programs improved the physical<br />

environment, service delivery, and teachers’ comfort and satisfaction. In addition, a study by<br />

Davies and Gavin (1994) indicates that the consultation method resulted in a significant<br />

increase in fine and gross motor skills for preschool children with developmental delays.<br />

Overall, the integrated therapy model of service delivery is more compatible with the current<br />

philosophical trends in Early Intervention/Early Childhood Special Education. The


incorporated principles of normalization, inclusion, developmental appropriate practice,<br />

individualization, and collaboration suggest that integrated services are more preferable to<br />

segregated therapy for preschoolers with special needs and who are enrolled in inclusive<br />

childcare programs (McWilliam, 1996). The most integrated model of service delivery,<br />

collaboration in conjunction with consultation, is desirable because it might enhance services<br />

for children with special needs and result in more comprehensive intervention and higher-<br />

quality treatment. Together, collaboration and consultation provide continuity of service and<br />

a more holistic intervention, because no aspect is completely isolated. Sharing of knowledge<br />

among professionals might have benefits for those involved, particularly children and<br />

families receiving services (Sandler, 1997).<br />

The integrated therapy approach has been evaluated and successfully applied in<br />

inclusive childcare settings by occupational therapists (Dunn, 1996; Sandler, 1997), speech-<br />

and language pathologists (Wilcox & Shannon, 1996), physical therapists (Rainforth &<br />

Roberts, 1996; Sandler, 1997), and special educators (Garfinkel & Schwartz, 2002; Wolery et<br />

al., 2002), but is thus far not widely applied, not has it been evaluated by music therapists.<br />

2.1.4 <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> in Early Intervention / Early Childhood Special Education<br />

2.1.4.1. <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> and Children with Special Needs<br />

<strong>Music</strong> therapy has a long tradition of providing services for young children with<br />

special needs. A wealth of case studies concerning music therapy for the treatment of<br />

children with special needs, including implications and applications (e.g., Gustorff, 1997;<br />

Gustorff & Neugebauer, 1997; Schmid & Janssen, 2001; Brusica, 1996) as well as music<br />

19


20<br />

therapy assessment tools (e.g., Brunk & Coleman, 2000; Wigram, 2002), can be found in the<br />

music therapy literature. A large body of research supports the positive impact of music on<br />

the child’s development and quality of life. <strong>Music</strong> therapy can significantly enhance the<br />

exceptional child’ cognitive/academic development (Campbell, 2000; Standley & Hughes,<br />

1997; Register, 2001, Colwell & Murrless, 2002; Wolfe & Horn, 1993), physical<br />

development (Galloway & Bean, 1974; Neugebauer, 2001), communication and language<br />

development (Harding & Ballard, 1982; Horn, 1988; Hoskins, 1988; Edgerton, 1994),<br />

emotional development (Crocker, 1968; Giles, Cogan & Cox, 1991; Hillard, 2001), and/or<br />

social development (Krout, 1986; Jellison, Brooks, & Huck, 1984; Brownell, 2002). It<br />

should be noted that music therapy never addresses only one area of the child’s development.<br />

<strong>Music</strong> therapy interventions are unique in reaching the whole child in a developmentally<br />

appropriate and holistic manner (Aldridge, Gustorff, & Neugebauer, 1995). An overview of<br />

research publications especially addressing newborns to age three (early intervention) and<br />

toddlers to school entry (early childhood special education) in various settings can be found<br />

in Humpal’s (2001) “Annotated bibliography of music therapy articles related to young<br />

children: from music therapy journals (1990-2000).” The following literature review focuses<br />

on music therapy programs and studies conducted in inclusive preschool settings.<br />

2.1.4.2. <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> in Inclusive Childcare Programs<br />

For more than a decade, music therapists in the U.S. have recognized the potential for<br />

professional involvement in early childhood programs. <strong>Music</strong> activities designed for specific<br />

therapeutic interventions and guidelines for applying and implementing interventions have<br />

been proposed. Inclusion has been facilitated by creating developmentally appropriate


curricula for young children with special needs, and for their typically developing peers.<br />

Some attention has been given to evaluate the effectiveness of music therapy interventions in<br />

Early Intervention / Early Childhood Special Education programs.<br />

In a literature review related to early intervention and music therapy, Humpal (1990)<br />

describes the appropriateness and implications for music therapy among young children with<br />

disabilities, and suggests music therapy techniques to accomplish early intervention goals<br />

within different domains. She also stresses that music therapy might be provided as a related<br />

service to children with special needs and their families. In a more recent publication,<br />

Warlick (2000), Director of the Office of Special Education and Rehabilitative Services in<br />

the United States Department of Education, confirmed in a letter to Andrea H. Farbman<br />

(Executive Director of the American <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Association, AMTA) the legal status of<br />

music therapy under the U.S. legislation. This letter reflects that music therapy is recognized<br />

as a related service under IDEA, but continues to be omitted from the written form of<br />

legislation.<br />

Davis first introduced a model for the integration of music therapy in daily classroom<br />

routines for children with physical disabilities or language delays in an inclusive preschool<br />

setting in 1990. As a contracting music therapist, her role involved consultation to teachers<br />

regarding musical recourses and activities enhancing other therapist’s goals, and direct<br />

services with individuals and small groups within and outside the classroom. In 2001, Davis<br />

gave an extended description of the model design, incorporating a “hierarchical approach to<br />

group music therapy intervention.” Based on child development theories and music therapy<br />

21


22<br />

models, including hierarchies of skills (Briggs, 1991; Steele, 1985), Davis presented three<br />

levels of functioning and related goals “through which children can learn, practice, and<br />

expand their repertoire of skills” (p. 37). The described program incorporates many aspects<br />

of McWilliam’s (1995) proposed model of integrated therapy, and emphasizes the<br />

importance of interdisciplinary collaboration and the possibility of co-treatment with other<br />

specialists. No research has been conducted to evaluate the effectiveness of this music<br />

therapy model.<br />

One intervention technique Davis applied to enhance social interaction within<br />

classroom routines was first introduced and studied by Gunsberg in 1988 and 1991. He used<br />

improvised music and lyrics to facilitate and maintain social play between children with<br />

developmental delays and their typically developing peers age three to five during free time<br />

play in a preschool program. By analyzing ten videotaped episodes, Gunsberg’s<br />

investigation provided evidence that when using this technique (Improvised <strong>Music</strong> Play),<br />

social play episodes lasted more than three times the duration of typical free-time play<br />

episodes among children with developmental delays and their typically developing peers. In<br />

William’s (1995) continuum of six models of service delivery, Gunsberg’s approach can be<br />

seen as a group activity within classroom routines.<br />

Another pilot program conducted by Hughes, Robbins, McKenzie, and Robb (1990)<br />

provided structured music activities and unstructured social playtime activities for one<br />

hundred pre-kindergarten children with special needs and typically developing children<br />

coming from various schools. Although quantitative data were not reported due to


questionable reliability, the following beneficial outcomes of the structured music activities<br />

were observed by live and videotaped observations and statement analysis of teachers and<br />

paraprofessionals: growing willingness to interact with one another, increasing acceptance<br />

and friendships among children with and without disabilities, and positive validation of the<br />

music sessions by participating staff.<br />

Because music might enhance social interactions among children with special needs<br />

and their typically developing peers, Humpal (1991) designed a weekly integrated early<br />

childhood music program to increase social interaction between preschool-aged children with<br />

moderate level of mental retardation and preschoolers from a typical school. The model<br />

included session plans and strategies, in-service training of staff, parent meetings, and a<br />

“handicap awareness session” for the typically developing peers. Based on trend analysis<br />

and a staff questionnaire, Humpal provided evidence that social acceptance and interaction<br />

between preschool-aged children with disabilities and typically developing peers does not<br />

automatically occur, but rather can be taught by structured music activities. This replicated<br />

the results of an earlier study conducted by Jellison, Brooks, and Huck (1984) with school-<br />

aged children. Humpal also reported that staff rated the integrated early childhood music<br />

program as outstanding “in providing opportunities for following group directions, in<br />

fostering positive self-concepts, in increasing acceptance of handicapped conditions, and<br />

teaching social skills” (p. 173).<br />

In another study addressing social interactions among other aspects, Kern and Wolery<br />

(2001) found that a child with visual impairment engaged in more social interactions with his<br />

23


24<br />

peers, both with and without special needs, as well as with adults, after adding musical<br />

instruments to meaningful locations on an inclusive preschool playground and providing staff<br />

development activities. Additional results, gained by using a within single-subject<br />

experimental design, were increased engagement with materials, a significant decrease of<br />

stereotypic behaviors, and more goal-directed movement within the playground environment.<br />

With this study Kern and Wolery (2001) demonstrated that music therapy interventions can<br />

be successfully applied in inclusive childcare settings by using a collaborative consultation<br />

model of service delivery.<br />

As evident in a study by Standley and Hughes (1997), music activities can also<br />

enhance academic skills of preschoolers enrolled in inclusive early intervention programs.<br />

By testing two groups of children with repeated measures and counterbalanced<br />

treatment/control conditions, Standley and Hughes found that music can significantly<br />

enhance their print concepts and prewriting skills. More specifically, “music can help<br />

children practice language patterns, learn print and book conventions, develop<br />

comprehension through song dramatization, and begin decoding written language” (p.85). In<br />

2001, Register replicated this study with a larger sample size (n=50). Results reinforced the<br />

previous outcomes and provided evidence that music therapy sessions specifically designed<br />

to improve academic skills are more efficient for desirable outcomes than those designed for<br />

multiple goals.<br />

Further, Standley and Hughes (1996) demonstrated that music therapy activities can<br />

be applied within the guidelines of developmentally appropriate practice (NAEYC, 1986).


Lesson plans addressing communication, academic, social, emotional and music skills<br />

following the standards of the NAECY were designed and implemented in an inclusive pre-<br />

kindergarten. Results of the study indicated that children’s level of response and on-task<br />

behavior was extremely high and the therapists’ and teachers’ implementation of the lesson<br />

plans followed the guidelines for developmentally appropriate instruction.<br />

Attention to the importance of music for young children’s learning and development<br />

has also come from the National Association for <strong>Music</strong> Education (MENC), the National<br />

Association for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC) and the U.S. Department of<br />

Education. A series of projects and events was conducted to help preschool children in the<br />

U.S. grow through music (Humpal & Wolf, 2003; Neelly, Kenney, & Wolf, 2000; Boston,<br />

2000). In addition, the American Association of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> (AMTA) joined “Support<br />

<strong>Music</strong>,” the largest initiation dedicated to providing music as an integral part of education to<br />

every child in the U.S. (AMTA, 2003a). Finally, the current action to add music therapy to<br />

the list of related services under the reauthorization of the IDEA – known as 1401 (22) and<br />

1432(4) (F) – might also have a huge impact for future music therapy service delivery for<br />

children with special needs in early childhood intervention/special education settings<br />

(Simpson, 2003).<br />

2.1.4.3. Models of Service Delivery in <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong><br />

With the changing landscape of service delivery in Early Intervention / Early<br />

Childhood Special Education, along with the growing awareness of the potential benefits of<br />

music for children’s learning and development, music therapists need to adapt their practice<br />

25


26<br />

to the needs of individuals and their families, the public resources, and trends in Early<br />

Intervention / Early Childhood Special Education. More effective and contemporary models<br />

of service delivery are sought (Chester at al., 1999; Register, 2002; Reuer, 1996). Reuer<br />

(1996) says: “For music therapists who embrace change, unlimited opportunities exist to<br />

actively participate in the redefinition of health care and the transformation of practice<br />

settings” (p. 16). In her article she encouraged music therapists to explore new methods of<br />

service delivery and introduced consultative strategies as a career option for music therapists.<br />

The AMTA’s Standards of Clinical Practice (2003b) states that music therapy can be<br />

provided both as a direct service to clients or consultative services “to other professionals in<br />

music therapy and related disciplines and to others directly involved with the client” (p. 20).<br />

Smith and Hairston (1999) cite outcomes of a national survey investigating the current<br />

practice of music therapy in U.S. school settings showed that 62% of the study participants<br />

engaged in the traditional direct-service delivery model each week, whereas only 13%<br />

provided consultative services.<br />

2.1.4.4. <strong>Collaborative</strong> and <strong>Consultative</strong> Models of Service Delivery in <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong><br />

<strong>Music</strong> therapists have given some attention to collaborative and consultative models<br />

of service delivery as effective strategies for inclusion of students within public school<br />

settings. A revised monograph by Wilson (2002) focuses on the most current information on<br />

models of music therapy service delivery in school settings. In this publication, Coleman<br />

(2002) gives specific information on direct and consultative models of service delivery for<br />

children with severe disabilities. Snell (2002) discusses models for school-aged students<br />

with ASD, Gladfelter (2002) for students with learning disabilities, Darrow and Grohe (2002)


for students with deaf/hard-hearing conditions, Hughes, Rice, DeBedout, and Hightower<br />

(2002) for students in comprehensive schools settings, and Furman (2002) as well as Humpal<br />

(2002) for children with special needs enrolled in community early education settings.<br />

In addition to direct services in self-contained special education and inclusive school<br />

settings, Johnson (2002) described four categories of consultative music therapy services.<br />

According to Johnson, the role of a music therapy consultant is: (1) to facilitate inclusion by<br />

designing and providing appropriate musical activities for students with special needs, and to<br />

support regular classroom teachers and music teachers in implementing these activities, (2) to<br />

facilitate music education by helping music educators understand handicapping conditions of<br />

students, and supporting the teachers to meet all students’ needs and educational goals (3) to<br />

facilitate musical performance by increasing the student’s musical abilities to contribute to<br />

the performance, and (4) to consult with non-music education staff by designing appropriate<br />

musical activities to meet the student’s IEP goals, to train staff to implement basic music<br />

activities, to provide appropriate music resources, and to facilitate in-class demonstration of<br />

appropriate music activities and expectations.<br />

Chester, Holmberg, Lawrence, and Thurmond (1999) also introduce a description of a<br />

“program-based consultative music therapy model” serving preschoolers with disabilities in<br />

self-contained language-based programs among other public elementary school programs.<br />

The following benefits of this model are described as follows: “more students receive<br />

services, teachers use music therapist-generated strategies regularly, collaboration occurs<br />

27


28<br />

across disciplines, and the music therapist’s time serving the classrooms is maximized” (p.<br />

82).<br />

Nevertheless, the description of these programs and service delivery models is not the<br />

result of systematic, scholarly research investigation. Data to support the effectiveness of a<br />

consultative model of music therapy service delivery have thus for eluded the field.<br />

Register (2002) recently conducted a national survey investigating collaboration and<br />

consultation practices of music therapists in the U.S. She reports that 87% of study<br />

participants collaborate with team members or professionals regarding the treatments of their<br />

clients whereas as only 44% music therapists provided consultative services. Consultation<br />

was primarily delivered in educational settings (38.8%) by providing workshops/seminars/in-<br />

services (72.6%) and one-to-one meetings (67.7%). The individuals with whom music<br />

therapists mostly consult were educators (62.0%); and parents, caregiver, and other family<br />

members (59.7%). Twenty-one-point-one percent of participants reported that they provided<br />

consultative services on a weekly or monthly basis. Subject areas most cited for goals and<br />

objectives of consultation were educational (84.0%). Register elucidated that “future<br />

research should seek to define both collaboration and consultation and how each of these<br />

techniques are acquired and implemented by music therapists” (p. 320). The survey did not<br />

specify the educational settings where music therapists administered collaborative and<br />

consultative services. At this point, no statement can be made as to whether or not<br />

collaborative and consultative strategies are common practice for music therapists working in


inclusive preschool settings. A survey investigating collaborative and consultation practice<br />

of music therapy in preschool settings is warranted.<br />

Clearly, collaboration and consultation models of service delivery do play an<br />

important role in the growth and understanding of music therapy as a profession (Register,<br />

2002). Not only does it have potential for professional involvement; it also supports the<br />

contemporary model of service delivery in Early Intervention/Early Childhood Special<br />

Education. Only one study that provides data-based information of the effects of<br />

collaborative and consultative models of music therapy service delivery in inclusive<br />

preschool settings emerged. More research is necessary in order to clarify whether a music<br />

therapy collaborative consultation model of music therapy service delivery is effective.<br />

2.2. Autism<br />

2.2.1 Definition, Prevalence, Causes and Diagnosis<br />

Autism is a complex developmental disorder that significantly affects the individual’s<br />

verbal and nonverbal communication and social interaction, and is associated with<br />

engagement in restricted, repetitive and stereotypic patterns of behavior, usually evident<br />

before age three (APA, 2000; ASA, 2003; WHO, 1993). Child psychiatrist Leo Kanner<br />

originally described the essential features of autism, once considered a rare disorder, in 1943.<br />

Today, it is estimated that autism occurs in as many as 1 to 6 per 1000 births, with an<br />

increasing trend upward, and is four times more prevalent in boys than in girls (APA, 2000;<br />

Gillberg & Wing, 1999; NICHCY, 2003; CDC, 2003; NINDS, 2003). This increasing<br />

incidence is the subject of great controversy. Whether the numbers indicate an increased<br />

29


30<br />

frequency of autism, or simply relate to changes in diagnostic criteria, earlier identification,<br />

and an improved awareness of the condition, remains unclear at present (Dalldorf, 2002a;<br />

Scott, Clark, & Brady, 2000). Although its causes are still unknown, autism is generally<br />

accepted as a neurological disorder present at birth (APA, 2000). Autism is sometimes<br />

observed in association with biological conditions such as Fragile X Syndrome, congenital<br />

Rubella Syndrome, and Tuberous Scleroses (APA, 2000; Dalldorf, 2002a; Gillberg &<br />

Coleman, 1996). Although a specific gene linked to autism has not yet been isolated,<br />

genetics also seem to play a part in the disorder (Lauritsen & Ewald, 2001). Current research<br />

investigates neurological damage and biochemical imbalances in the brain (Dalldorf, 2002a;<br />

National Research Council, 2001; NICHCY, 2003). It is known that there is no racial,<br />

ethnic, or social boundaries, and autism is not caused by parents’ lifestyles, educational level<br />

or psychological factors in the child’s environment (Dalldorf, 2002a; NICHCY, 2003; ASA,<br />

2003, TEACCH, 1999). A series of studies on attachment behaviors in autism refutes the<br />

long-standing assumption that autism represents a failure of the attachment process between<br />

child and caretaker (Capps et al., 1994; Rogers et al., 1993; Shapiro et al., 1987). However,<br />

autism remains a heterogeneous, and indeed puzzling, disability. Currently, there are no<br />

medical or genetic tests to identify autism. The most frequently clinical tool used to diagnose<br />

autism is the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4 th ed.) Text Revision,<br />

(DSM-IV-TR) (APA, 2000). Autism disorder is listed under the broader category of<br />

Pervasive Developmental Disorders (PDD), which include Rett’s Disorder, Childhood<br />

Disintegrative Disorder (CDD), Asperger’s Disorder, and PDD Not Otherwise Specified<br />

(PDD-NOS). Differential diagnosis is based on observation of the child’s individual<br />

developmental and functional level to determine if a specific number of characteristics within


the three key diagnostic areas (i.e., social interaction, communication, and behavior) listed in<br />

the DSM-IV-TR are present. A number of screening tools (e.g., Checklist for Autism and<br />

Toddlers (CHAT) (Baird et al., 2000), Autism Screening Questionnaire (Berument et al.,<br />

1999), Screening Test for Autism in Two-Year Olds (STAT) (Stone et al., 2000) is available<br />

to identify whether an individual is at risk of autism spectrum disorder or not. Various<br />

standardized and documented diagnostic instruments, based on observation, parental<br />

interviews and checklists, have emerged that identify the disorder and other associated<br />

developmental delays (e.g. Psychoeducational Profile-Revised (PEP-R) (Schopler et al.,<br />

1990), Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) (Lord et al., 1999), Vineland<br />

Adaptive Behavior Scales (Sparrow, Balla, & Cichetti, 1984), Childhood Autism Rating<br />

Scale (CARS) (Schopler, Reichler, & Renner, 1988). Currently, children can be definitively<br />

diagnosed as early as 18 months of age (Baird et al., 2000; Stone et al., 1999; Gillberg et al.,<br />

1990; Wolery et al., 2001).<br />

2.2.2 Characteristics of Autism<br />

Autism is now understood as a spectrum of disorders, in which a combination of<br />

some or all of the predetermined characteristics might be observed in varying degrees of<br />

severity, ranging from difficulty with social interactions and relationships to language and<br />

communication difficulties, repetitive, restricted and stereotypic behaviors, and other<br />

characteristics. There is no typical single behavior of autism, even though there are unique<br />

patterns and relative strengths (American Psychiatry Association, 2000; National Research<br />

Council, 2001). The following description is based on the three diagnostic categories listed<br />

in the DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000), and other associated characteristics thoroughly described in<br />

31


32<br />

the literature (e.g. Accardo, Magnusen, & Capute, 2000; ASA, 2003a; Doherty, McNally, &<br />

Sherrard, 2000; Grandin, 2003; NICHCY, 2003; Society for the Autistically Handicapped<br />

2003; TEACCH, 1999; Mesibov, Adams, Klinger, 1997; National Research Council, 2001;<br />

Wolery et al., 2001).<br />

2.2.2.1 Social Interactions and Relationships<br />

The child with autism might have severe difficulty in relating to people. This<br />

problem might be manifested in exhibiting difficulty with, or a lack of understanding of,<br />

conventional nonverbal communication such as eye-gaze, facial expression, body postures,<br />

and gestures necessary to facilitate social interaction. Temple Grandin (1999), diagnosed<br />

with autism at an early age, describes this characteristic as following: “I didn’t know that eye<br />

movements had meaning until I read Mind Blindness by Simon Baron-Cohen [(1995)]. I had<br />

no idea that people communicated feelings with their eyes” (p. 1). Children with autism tend<br />

to spend time alone and seem to “tune out” the outside world. They might have little interest<br />

in peers and be passive in social situations both in initiating and responding. Often children<br />

with autism fail to engage in symbolic and dramatic play or social imitation. Those who<br />

interact socially might exhibit inappropriate, intrusive, or insensitive actions. Children with<br />

autism most frequently don’t spontaneously seek to share enjoyment, interests and<br />

achievements with others. Although they are capable of expressing emotions, a lack of social<br />

and emotional reciprocity is often present. Consequently, many children with autism have<br />

great difficulty developing social relations and friendships.


2.2.2.2 Language and Communication Development<br />

The child with autism might have various difficulties regarding language and<br />

communication, affecting both receptive language and expressive language. These problems<br />

might be manifested in a delayed or total absence of language development. Many children<br />

with autism do not use conventional body language in place of verbal skills. For example,<br />

they often will not show, wave, point, or nod. Consequently, expressing needs and<br />

understanding simple questions, verbal directions or understanding symbolic gestures might<br />

be a challenging and frustrating experience for the child. When speech does develop, it often<br />

includes repetition of words or phrases (echolalia). Children with more advanced language<br />

skills tend to use a small range of topics or words without attachment to their usual meaning,<br />

and they have great difficulty with abstract language. All these make initiating or sustaining<br />

a conversation a major challenge. Those who are able to use language effectively often have<br />

an abnormality in pitch, intonation, rate, rhythm or stress in their voice.<br />

2.2.2.3 Repetitive, Restricted and Stereotypic Behaviors<br />

The child with autism might exhibit restricted interests of abnormal intensity and<br />

focus. These children might insist on specific routines and rituals (e.g. following the same<br />

schedule each day), and can become very distressed when changes occur. People with<br />

autism might perform stereotyped, repetitive motor movements, such as hand flapping, toe<br />

walking, or whole body movements. Persistent preoccupations with parts of objects and<br />

repetitive play skills (e.g., spinning object) are generally evident as well. Donna Williams<br />

(1992), diagnosed with autism in early childhood, offers the following explanation for the<br />

repetitive and restricted behavior patterns: “The constant change of most things never seemed<br />

33


34<br />

to give me any chance to prepare myself for them. Because of this I found pleasure and<br />

comfort in doing the same thing over and over again” (p. 44).<br />

2.2.2.4 Other Characteristics<br />

Children with autism also frequently display an inconsistent response to sensory<br />

information during their toddler or preschool years (Ermer & Dunn, 1998). Often, a<br />

hypersensitivity to sounds, lights, certain texture of fabrics, and taste and smell of food are<br />

observed. At other times, the same individual might respond differently or have a lack of<br />

responsiveness to the same stimuli (Dawson & Watling, 2000). Grandin (in Grandin &<br />

Scariano, 1996) gives the following example of an inconsistent pattern of sensory response<br />

for hearing: “Sometimes I heard and understood, and other times sounds or speech reached<br />

my brain like the unbearable noise of an onrushing freight train” (p.145). Additional, in<br />

infants with autism an insensitivity to pain is more commonly seen than in typical infants or<br />

infants with other special needs (Hoshino et al., 1982).<br />

Children with autism often have cognitive deficits. Their cognitive abilities can vary<br />

widely and often they do have uneven patterns of intellectual functioning. For example,<br />

some aspects of development (such as visual memory and visual-spatial organization) might<br />

be close to normal, while others (such as problem-solving and abstract thinking) might be<br />

seriously delayed. Other cognitive characteristics such as difficulty with attention,<br />

information processing and social-cognition, including focusing on irrelevant details,<br />

difficulty with perceiving or understanding thoughts, feelings, or intentions of others,<br />

inability to generalize, and problems in sequencing and organization, have been described in


the literature (Baron-Cohen, 1995; Quill, 2000). Only a small percentage of individuals with<br />

autism fall in the average or upper range of intelligence. Among them, a few do have “islets<br />

of ability or intelligence,” such as unusual musical or artistic talents or extraordinary<br />

calculation and memorization skills (Marohn, 2002; Mesibov & Shea, 2003). This<br />

phenomenon is described in the literature as Savant-Syndrome (e.g., Dworschak, 2003;<br />

Young & Nettelbeck, 1995). However, the majority of children with autism (about 70%)<br />

have various degrees of mental retardation (National Research Council, 2001).<br />

Professionals and parents of children with autism often report secondary effects, such<br />

as problems with sleeping, eating, and toileting, extreme temper tantrums, aggressions,<br />

impulsivity, obsessions, hyperactivity, and self-injury. Although not specific to the disorder<br />

(or part of its defining diagnosis), these behaviors create major challenges in the daily life of<br />

children with autism (National Research Council, 2001; Wolery et al., 2001).<br />

2.2.3 Contemporary Education and Treatments for Young Children with Autism<br />

While there is at present no cure for autism, research indicates that early diagnosis<br />

followed by early education and treatment efforts can significantly enhance a child’s ability<br />

to participate meaningfully in family and community life and play a vital part in society<br />

(ASA, 2003; TEACCH, 1999; Wolery et al., 2001). A large body of literature and an<br />

abundance of recent information on the Internet describe, evaluate and discuss contemporary<br />

education and treatments for children with autism (e.g., ASA, 2003; Dalldorf, 2002b;<br />

Dempsey & Foreman, 2001; Kraus, 2002; Marohn, 2002; McIntosh, 1999, NICHCY, 1998). 1<br />

1 To be sure, use of the Internet as a source of information in scholarly inquiries – at least as a blanket<br />

source of information – is not without controversy. There is no substitute for scientific, peer-reviewed<br />

35


36<br />

Because each child has unique strengths and weaknesses, there is no one approach that is<br />

suitable for all children. Educational and therapeutic interventions must be individualized to<br />

the child’s individual needs and abilities (ASA, 2003; Kraus, 2002; National Research<br />

Council, 2001; Schopler, 1997). A combination of individualized educational approaches<br />

and treatments is often most beneficial for individuals diagnosed with autism, but should be<br />

carefully selected (ASA, 2003). Based on the literature, treatment options for children with<br />

autism can be placed into three general categories: medical treatments, educational and<br />

behavioral approaches and programs, and complementary therapy. The following sections<br />

review each of these treatments in turn.<br />

2.2.3.1 Medical Treatments<br />

According to recent research, no dietary or biomedical approaches can cure autism<br />

(NINDS, 2003). However, some medical treatments have positive effects on some behaviors<br />

associated with autism (ASA, 2003). Medical treatments for autism currently include (1)<br />

medication; (2) nutrient therapy (vitamins and nutritional supplements such as vitamin B6, A,<br />

C and magnesium zinc, folic acid); (3) hormone therapy (e.g., secretin, melatonin), (4)<br />

Landau Kleffer Syndrome treatment; (5) heavy metal detoxification/chelation therapy (e.g.,<br />

mercury or other substances in food, and other environmental sources); (6) dietary<br />

interventions; (e.g., gluten, casein) to control food intolerances and allergies; (7) anti-yeast<br />

therapy; and (8) immune system therapy (Dalldorf, 2002; Marohn, 2002; Society for the<br />

journals as a source of evidence. That said, for the purposes of this inquiry the Internet serves a<br />

valuable purpose for several reasons. First, it provides accessibility for individuals with ASD and their<br />

families, as well as for professionals. Second, it is well suited to the particularly fast-changing nature<br />

of ASD research. Third, and by extension, much of the scholarly work on ASD is in fact located on the<br />

Internet. Finally, associations use Internet as a primary vehicle for the dissemination of information on<br />

ASD and related disorders.


Autistically Handicapped, 2003b, Kraus, 2002). Some of these treatments have been tested<br />

and proven effective, while others have not. Controversy remains over some outcomes and<br />

methodology, and thus for no statistically significant improvements in the core symptoms<br />

have been found (ASA, 2003; National Research Council, 2001).<br />

2.2.3.2 Educational and Behavioral <strong>Approach</strong>es and Programs<br />

As evident in the autism literature, educational and behavioral interventions are<br />

currently the most effective and most commonly used approaches in the U.S. for treating<br />

children with autism (Cambell et al., 1996; Dalldorf, 2002b; Green 1996). A variety of<br />

intervention practice research (as reviewed in Wolery & Garfinkel, 2002) and the evaluation<br />

of intervention programs (as reviewed in Dawson & Osterling, 1997; Handleman & Harris,<br />

2001; National Research Council, 2001; Smith, 1999) can be found in the literature. A large<br />

body of research has demonstrated significant improvement in the key areas, including social<br />

skills (as reviewed in Hwang & Hughes, 2000; McConell, 2002; Pollard, 1998), language<br />

and communication abilities (e.g., Buffington et al., 1998; Keen, Sigafoos, Woodyatt, 2001;<br />

Koegel, O’Dell & Koegel, 1987) and behaviors (e.g., Koegel et al., 1992; Morrison &<br />

Rosales-Ruiz, 1997) in relative short time periods when appropriate services, training and<br />

information were provided. The committee on educational intervention for children with<br />

autism (National Research Council, 2001) states: “Research is under way to predict<br />

responders and non-responders to medication and to determine which child will benefit from<br />

behavioral treatment alone and what combinations of medication and behavioral treatments<br />

are most effective” (p. 130).<br />

37


38<br />

2.2.3.3 Complementary Therapies<br />

Other interventions might play an important role in improving social and<br />

communication skills and in reducing inappropriate behaviors associated with autism as well.<br />

These therapies might be provided in segregated therapy settings or integrated in educational<br />

programs. When speech, language, and motor deficits hinder progress in school, IDEA<br />

mandates that speech-language therapy, physical therapy, and occupational therapy be<br />

provided (National Research Council, 2001). Although music therapy is not as well known<br />

to the public as a related service under IDEA, its value is well established for the treatment of<br />

individuals with autism (ASA, 2003; Dalldorf, 2002b, Grandin, 1988b; Kraus, 2002; The<br />

National Autistic Society, 2002).<br />

Many other innovative interventions such as Sensory Integration (Ayres & Tickle,<br />

1980; Berger, 2002; Hutch-Rasmussen, 1995), The Squeezing Machine (Grandin, 1992),<br />

Holding <strong>Therapy</strong> (Welch, 1988), Doman/Delacato Method (Delacato, 1974), Flexyx <strong>Therapy</strong><br />

(OchsLabs, 2002), Daily Life <strong>Therapy</strong>: Higashi (The National Autistic Society, 2001);<br />

Cranosacral <strong>Therapy</strong> (Mehl-Madrona, 2003), Facilitated Communication (Calculator et al.,<br />

1995; Shane, 1994), Animal-Assisted <strong>Therapy</strong> (Redefer & Goodman, 1989), the Son-Rise<br />

program (Autism Treatment Center of America, 2003), Vision <strong>Therapy</strong> (Kaplan et al., 1998);<br />

Auditory Integration <strong>Therapy</strong> (Berard, 1993), and Tomatis <strong>Therapy</strong> (The Tomatis Method,<br />

2003) are available. Since there is not much scientific support of the effectiveness of these<br />

widely publicized therapies, their application remains controversial. Turnbull, Wilcox and<br />

Stowe (2002) report “(…) there is great emphasis in the [U.S.] law on using interventions<br />

that are proven and that have some scientific evidence supporting their value” (p. 487).


Research including well-controlled studies demonstrates the effectiveness for these<br />

interventions is strongly suggested (National Research Council, 2001).<br />

2.2.4 Including Young Children with Autism in Preschool Education Programs<br />

2.2.4.1 Rationale for Inclusion<br />

Lovaas (1987) changed autism treatment dramatically by showing the benefits of<br />

early intensive interventions in the form of behavior modification. Nearly half of the young<br />

children with autism in his projects (47%) who underwent an intensive behavior modification<br />

treatment eventually achieved normal intellectual and educational functioning in comparison<br />

to the control group, where only a few children (3%) reached the same functioning level.<br />

McGee, Paradis and Feldman (1993) find that simply close proximity to typically developing<br />

peers in inclusive settings, children with autism significantly decreases levels of autistic<br />

behaviors in contrast to self-contained settings or when they were alone. Strain, Odom and<br />

their colleagues (1985; 1986) report that children with autism make social and educational<br />

progress when exposed to typically developing peers in integrated preschools. Strain (1983)<br />

also demonstrates that maintenance and generalization more likely occurs in integrated<br />

preschool settings than in self-contained classes. Finally, Dawson and Osterling (1997)<br />

report that in four out of six programs, approximately 50% of children receiving early<br />

intervention services are in full inclusion classes later.<br />

The data supporting early intervention and inclusion of children with autism, along<br />

with the provision of special education and related services for children with autism in the<br />

least restricted environment (NICHCY, 2003; Turnbull, Wilcox, & Stowe, 2002) specifically<br />

39


40<br />

manifested under IDEA (U.S.C. 1401 [3] [A]) lead to an increasing trend toward including<br />

children with autism in preschool education programs. The rationale for including children<br />

with autism is similar to those generally described for other children with special needs.<br />

Their severe difficulties with social interactions and relationships especially support<br />

inclusion, which offers naturally occurring learning opportunities. The overall educational<br />

goals for children with autism are the same as those for typically developing children:<br />

“personal independence and social responsibility” (National Research Council, 2001, p. 216).<br />

To assist a child accomplishing these goals, preschool education programs for young children<br />

with autism not only emphasize the acquisition of academic skills; they also foster the<br />

improvement of social, language and communication, and daily living skills and address<br />

behavioral problems associated with autism (ASA, 2003; Dawson & Osterling, 1997; Koegel<br />

& Koegel, 1995; National Research Council, 2001). According to the Committee of<br />

Educational Interventions for Children with Autism (National Research Council, 2001), there<br />

are two theoretical frameworks for teaching children with autism: developmental and<br />

behavioral.<br />

2.2.4.2 Theoretical Framework for Teaching Children with Autism<br />

Whereas<br />

The developmental approach is described by the National Research Council (2001) as<br />

(…) a model of typical development to guide the educational process involving<br />

assessment, goal setting, and teaching. It involves assessing each developmental<br />

area — motor, cognition, communication, and social development, among others —<br />

and using a child’s successes, emerging skills, and failures to determining a child’s<br />

zone of proximal development (Vygosky, 2000). This zone indicates the set of skills<br />

that a child appears to be ready to learn next, based on his or her assessed<br />

performance. Those skills are the targeted for teaching (p. 72).


(…) in a behavioral approach child’s behavioral repertoire is evaluated according to<br />

the presence of behavioral excesses — presence of abnormal behaviors or of an<br />

abnormal frequency of certain behavior – and behavioral deficits – absence or low<br />

frequency of typical skills (Lovaas, 1987). Behavioral strategies are then designed to<br />

increase a child’s performance of deficit skills and decrease the behavioral excesses.<br />

These strategies involve identifying the target of teaching, determining the<br />

appropriate antecedent and consequence for the target behavior and assess students<br />

progress (p. 72).<br />

As described in section 2.1.2.2, contemporary models of inclusion are based on a<br />

developmental approach (e.g., Bredekamp & Copple, 2002; NAEYC, 1996). Among the<br />

many advantages of this approach are its typical application in early childhood education<br />

settings, the described guidelines, familiar assessment tools, and availability of teaching<br />

materials. Also, professionals involved in early education are usually trained in<br />

developmental appropriate teaching practice. Some drawbacks of this approach are that<br />

children with autism do not exhibit usual developmental patterns in domains such as<br />

communication, language and social development in comparison to their typically<br />

developing peers. Traditional developmental teaching strategies, such as verbal instruction,<br />

imitation of teachers and peers, and independent learning, depend largely on a child’s<br />

internal motivation to learn. Because of this, these strategies might not be effective tools for<br />

teaching children with autism (National Research Council, 2001). The developmental<br />

approach needs to be modified or combined with other teaching strategies to ensure that the<br />

educational goals of children with autism are met (e.g., Sandall, McLean, & Smith, 2000;<br />

Wolery & Bredekamp, 1994).<br />

Some advantages of the behavioral approach are that there is some research-based<br />

evidence of generalization and maintenance, that antecedents and consequences are<br />

41


42<br />

incorporated, and systematic strategies are used to teach complex skills. Drawbacks of the<br />

traditional behavioral approach are that the complex data system involved in this approach<br />

might complicate the implementation in inclusive preschool settings, the use of “aversive”<br />

techniques for behavior management is highly controversial, and staff members on early<br />

childhood teams often do not receive enough training for proper implementation of this<br />

approach in classroom routines (Dempsey & Foreman, 2001; National Research Council,<br />

2001).<br />

There are now more contemporary behavioral approaches, such as the natural<br />

language paradigm (e.g., Koegel, O’Dell, & Koegel, 1987), incidental teaching (e.g. McGee<br />

et al., 1999), time delay and milieu intervention (e.g., Halle, Marshall, & Spradlin, 1979),<br />

pivotal response training (e.g., Koegel et al., 1999), and peer-mediated strategies (e.g.,<br />

DiSalvo & Oswald, 2002; Goldstein et al., 1992; McGee et al., 1992) that emphasize<br />

naturalistic delivery in group settings and are more compatible with inclusion. These<br />

behavioral approaches are based on systematic teaching trials focusing on the child’s<br />

initiation, the child’s interest, and natural reinforcers in the child’s environment. Although<br />

not as well known to professionals and parents, these strategies have led to impressive<br />

outcomes in numerous domains (Dempsey & Foreman, 2001; National Research Council,<br />

2001; Schreibman, 2000). A detailed description of the continuum of behavioral approached<br />

has been published in several summary documents (e.g., Anderson & Romanczyk, 1999,<br />

Prizant & Wetherby, 1998, Schreibman, 2000).


To date, more empirical data are available to the behavioral approaches than to the<br />

developmental approach. Because the developmental approach is commonly used in early<br />

childhood education, it would be desirable to have more empirical validation supporting it.<br />

2.2.4.3 Early Intervention Programs for Children with Autism<br />

A number of university-based model programs empirically demonstrate effective<br />

inclusion of young children with ASD. Intervention approaches and philosophical and<br />

theoretical positions encompassed in these model programs vary greatly. These range from<br />

the traditional discrete trial approaches (Lovaas, 1987) to more contemporary applied<br />

behavior analysis programs (Harris et al., 1991; Fenske et al., 1985, McGee, 1999) to<br />

developmentally oriented programs (e.g., Greenspan & Wieder, 1997; Prizant & Wetherby,<br />

1998; Rogers & Lewis, 1989; Schopler et al., 1995). A detailed description of model<br />

programs, including structure and context, diagnostic and assessment procedures, staffing<br />

and administration, intervention components, integration aspects, family involvement,<br />

outcome measures, and discussion on effects on children and families, can be found in<br />

summary documents from Handleman and Harris (2001), Dawson and Osterling (1997), and<br />

the National Research Council (2001). Well-established and reputable programs are the<br />

University of California at Los Angeles (UCLA) Young Autism Project and Treatment and<br />

Education of Autistic and Related Communication Handicapped Children (TEACCH) at the<br />

University of North Carolina, School of Medicine at Chapel Hill.<br />

The UCLA Young Autism Project. This model is based on early research by Ivar<br />

Lovaas and colleagues, as well as subsequent investigations using applied behavior analysis<br />

43


44<br />

(ABA). Application of the behavioral intervention curriculum for young children with<br />

autism started in the seventies. The treatment usually lasts three years, is time-consuming<br />

and involves an intensive training. In the first year, a trained therapist and parents primarily<br />

use a one-to-one discrete trial format in the child’s home. In a discrete trail training, the<br />

therapist-parent team breaks down the tasks into short simple pieces, or ‘trials.” The<br />

appropriate response to cues is followed by consequences that are reinforcing for the<br />

individual. The assumption is that behavior rewarded is more likely to be repeated than<br />

behavior ignored. Intervention goals are responding to requests, imitating, playing with toys,<br />

self-help skills and interaction with family members, followed during the second year by<br />

teaching communicative language, abstract concepts, emotion discrimination, pre-academic<br />

skills, and observational learning. Socialization and integration into school and other<br />

community settings is the focus of the third year. A full-time aide who is familiar to the<br />

child, assists her or him in generalizing previously mastered skills, following the classroom<br />

routine and responding to classroom teacher and peers until the child functions independently<br />

in the new environment (Smith et al., 2001).<br />

Treatment and Education of Autistic and Related Communication Handicapped<br />

Children (TEACCH). Founded in North Carolina in 1972, this program provides statewide,<br />

comprehensive community-based services to people with ASD from preschool to adult life.<br />

There are two preschool demonstration classrooms. In addition, educational services for<br />

preschool children are provided within the child’s natural environment by assessment,<br />

individual program planning, parent and teacher training, and consultation. Based on<br />

research findings indicating that children with autism learn better in structured rather than in


unstructured learning situations (Schopler, Kinsbourne, & Reichler, 1971), a central aspect of<br />

the TEACCH program is structured teaching, in which environments are modified with clear,<br />

concrete, visual information. The physical structure of the environment includes specific<br />

work areas for different activities designed to support children in accomplishing educational<br />

goals. Daily schedules and clear visual cues are used to assist the child to learn directions<br />

and understand the classroom routines. The TEACCH program is designed to address each<br />

child’s individual needs and takes into consideration strengths, interests and learning styles.<br />

Parental involvement is essential in the TEACCH program. Parents are seen as co-therapists,<br />

and trained in individual strategies to work with their child. Support programs, such as a<br />

mothers’ and a fathers’ group, a parent mentor program, a home teaching kit, a parent course<br />

and workshops, are offered to the families (Marcus, Lord, & Schopler, 2001). TEACCH has<br />

also developed a communication curriculum that uses a naturalistic behavioral approach,<br />

along with alternative communication strategies for nonverbal children (Watson et al., 1988).<br />

2.2.4.4 Teaching Strategies<br />

Some preschool educational programs use unilateral approaches and strategies,<br />

whereas others combine several strategies and treatment approaches for the inclusion and<br />

education of young children with autism. A huge body of literature examines educational<br />

practice for promoting social, language and communication development and addressing<br />

problematic behaviors and other difficulties in young children with autism (e.g., Koegel &<br />

Koegel, 1995; National Research Council, 2001; Wolery & Garfinkel, 2002). Both<br />

practitioners and researchers have suggested teaching strategies for successful inclusion of<br />

children with autism in preschool education settings.<br />

45


46<br />

Social development. Interventions focusing on improving social skills, in children<br />

with autism target both adults and peers. The variety of strategies used to improve social<br />

skills in children with autism fall into three categories:<br />

(1) adult-directed instruction of specific goals of social skills, such as joint attentions,<br />

response by gaze, imitation, turn taking, and initiating social interactions;<br />

(2) a child-centered approach, in which adults follow the child’s lead, encourage and sustain<br />

interactions, scaffold to higher levels, and extend the duration of interaction; and<br />

(3) peer strategies, in which typically developing peers prompt and maintain social<br />

engagement (National Research Council, 2001).<br />

According to McGee, Morrier and Daly (2001), true social inclusion only occurs by<br />

teaching both children with autism and their typically developing peers how to interact with<br />

one another. Peer-mediated strategies shown themselves to be the most powerful means of<br />

improving social interactions of children with autism, and of generalizing and maintaining in<br />

inclusive preschool settings (Strain et al., 1979; Goldstein et al., 1992; McGee et al., 1992).<br />

When using peer-mediated strategies, teachers train typically developed peers how to interact<br />

with their classmates with autism to model and role-playing. These strategies are then cued,<br />

reinforced and systematically faded out by teachers (National Research Council, 2001).<br />

Other strategies used to increase social interaction in children with autism are peer tutoring<br />

through incidental teaching (McGee et al., 1992), adult instruction in social games (Goldstein<br />

et al., 1988), script-fading procedures (Krantz & McClannahan, 1998), and social stories<br />

(Gray & Garand, 1993). “(…) Peer-mediated approaches are complex to deliver, require<br />

socially skilled typical peers, precise adult control in training peers, managing and fading


einforcement, and monitoring ongoing child interaction data” (National Research Council,<br />

2001, p. 138). Interactions with peers are also important, providing as they do models of<br />

appropriate language and communication, as well as behavioral skills.<br />

Language and communication development. Strategies used to improve language and<br />

communication in children with autism vary greatly along the continuum of behavioral to<br />

developmental approaches. In general, it is recognized that the lack of motivation to use<br />

language and communication in children with autism is an issue. Therefore, strategies to<br />

increase language and communication in children with autism must encourage language and<br />

communication by capitalizing on the child’s natural desires and preferences within her or<br />

his environment. Often, environments are arranged to provide opportunities for<br />

communication. The smallest attempt of the child to communicate is immediately rewarded,<br />

imitated and expanded upon by adults (Dawson & Osterling, 1997).<br />

If a child with autism demonstrates non-functional language, has limited verbal<br />

communication skills or difficulty with language comprehension, augmentative and<br />

alternative communication (AAC) is likely to be introduced. AAC strategies use assistive<br />

devices, such as visual communication symbols (pictures, written words, objects), visual<br />

schedules, displays on communication boards, voice output devices with digitized speech,<br />

and sign language in place of or in combination with verbal language. The most widely<br />

applied symbolic communication system is the Picture Exchange Communication System<br />

(PECS) (Bondy & Frost, 1994), which capitalizes on the strong visual processing of many<br />

children with autism. PECS is a systematic communication program designed to assist<br />

47


48<br />

children with autism, regardless of their verbal and nonverbal skill to initiate communication<br />

with others. Also based on a modified behavioral model, PECS teaches the child to approach<br />

her or his communication partner by exchanging a picture symbol for the desired object or<br />

activity. PECS is used within ongoing classroom routines and activities, and can function as<br />

a bridge between learning to communicate and using independent speech.<br />

Challenging behaviors. One of the most stressful issues faced by parents and<br />

professionals in their efforts to include children with autism in preschool educational<br />

programs are the exhibition of challenging behaviors such as “lack of compliance with or<br />

disruption of classroom routines, tantrums, destruction of property, and aggression against<br />

self or others” (National Research Council, p. 116). There is no single strategy that has<br />

proved effective in reducing challenging behaviors in all children with autism. However,<br />

there is a consensus that such problems should be addressed in the child’s early years, and<br />

prevented before they occur, by strongly engaging the child in classroom activities (Darwson<br />

& Osterling, 1997).<br />

To reduce challenging behaviors, interventions vary from discrete trial, and<br />

naturalistic behavior approaches such as pivotal response training and incidental teaching, to<br />

developmental approaches, which recognize the needs of highly structured environments,<br />

adult attention and consistency. Consequence-base approaches (with an applied behavioral<br />

analysis theoretical framework) are the most empirically evaluated approaches, designed to<br />

decrease or eliminate challenging behaviors (National Research Council, 2001). In pivotal<br />

response training, children are given some control over their environments and are allowed


choices in certain areas, which might contribute to reducing challenging behaviors (Koegel et<br />

al., 1992). Increasing the child’s interest and engagement by providing highly preferred play<br />

materials or topic areas and choices regarding preferred activities is described as “Increase<br />

fun decreases problem behaviors” by McGee and colleagues (2001, p. 176). Structured<br />

teaching and highly structured environments also prevent problem behaviors, because<br />

structure increases the child’s understanding of the classroom routines and activities (Marcus<br />

et al., 2001). If the challenging behavior continues, a functional assessment of the behavior<br />

is made and teaching alternative appropriate behaviors is provided. The functional behavior<br />

assessment involves:<br />

• keeping a detailed record of the behavior, including situations, time of the day, events<br />

that proceed the behavior, and how others respond;<br />

• developing hypotheses regarding the functioning of the behaviors; and<br />

• changing the environment to support appropriate behaviors, teaching appropriate<br />

behaviors, teaching to request help through words, objects, gestures or sign language<br />

(Donnellan et al., 1984).<br />

The wide range of learning styles and individual differences within children with<br />

ASD requires that educational strategies and approaches be tailored to each child’s individual<br />

strengths and needs. A combination of approaches is essential to successfully include<br />

children with autism in preschool education programs. The approaches notwithstanding,<br />

according to Dawson and Osterling (1997) effective early intervention models include the<br />

following common features:<br />

• specific curriculum content;<br />

49


50<br />

• highly supportive teaching environment and generalization strategies;<br />

• predictability and routine;<br />

• use of functional approaches to problem behaviors;<br />

• carefully planned transitions from preschool to school; and<br />

• active family involvement.<br />

Additionally, Anderson and Romanczyk, (1999) report that effective programs have<br />

• Highly trained staff, specialized in autism<br />

• Adequate resources<br />

• Supervisory and review mechanisms<br />

All programs emphasize the importance of starting interventions as early as possible, are<br />

intensive in hours, and have a low staff-child ratio (i.e., 1:1 or 1:2) (National Research<br />

Council, 2001).<br />

Specialized therapies in these programs include speech language pathologists,<br />

physical therapists, social workers, school psychologists, and occupational therapists. Only<br />

one program (The Children’s Unit for Treatment and Evaluation, State University of New<br />

York, SUNY) describes having art and music therapists on staff. Programs vary as to<br />

whether specialized therapists are part of the regular staff. Research strongly suggests that<br />

generalization is the most valuable effect of direct treatments for children with autism.<br />

Therefore, most programs emphasize that the therapist’s role is one of consultant who assists<br />

in embeding therapeutic goals in ongoing classroom routines. The Committee of Educational<br />

Interventions for Children with Autism (National Research Council, 2001) states: “There is<br />

little reason to believe that individual therapies carried out infrequently (e.g., once or twice a


week) have a long-term value for young children, unless the techniques are taught to and<br />

used regularly by the child and the people whom they interact with in natural contexts”<br />

(National Research Council, p. 139).<br />

Grandin (in Grandin & Scariano, 1986) concludes: “ I have visited many autism<br />

programs for young children. Effective programs often use many of the same procedures<br />

even though the theoretical orientation is different. The most successful programs start<br />

treatment by age three or four and provide contact with normal children. They are also very<br />

intense. Passive approaches do not work. A good program should also have flexible<br />

nonaversive behavior modification, sensory treatments, speech therapy, exercise, and music<br />

therapy” (p. 179).<br />

2.2.5 <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> and Children with Autism<br />

2.2.5.1 Benefits of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> for Children with Autism<br />

The American Association of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> (AMTA) (2003c) states: “For<br />

individuals with diagnoses on the autism spectrum, music therapy provides a unique variety<br />

of music experiences in an intentional and developmentally appropriate manner to effect<br />

changes in behavior and facilitate development of skills” (p.1).<br />

<strong>Music</strong> therapy can be effective in addressing difficulties associated with autism, because<br />

music might<br />

• capture the child’s attention and cause positive response;<br />

• encourage social interaction and build relationships in a non-threatening way;<br />

51


52<br />

• facilitate verbal and nonverbal communication;<br />

• reduce negative and/or self-stimulatory responses;<br />

• provide multi-sensory stimulation and enhance perceptual and motor functioning;<br />

and<br />

• be a vehicle for identifying and expressing emotions.<br />

In addition, musical elements and music therapy sessions provide structure and<br />

predictability, thereby emphasizing the child’s strengths and individual needs. <strong>Music</strong> therapy<br />

might also have positive impacts on a child’s family by improving the child’s skills, sharing<br />

music-making among family members, and showing alternative ways to interact and<br />

communicate with one another. Families might see their child in a “different light,” which<br />

might give confidence in the child’s abilities and growth (AMTA, 2003).<br />

2.2.5.2 <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> and Autism: Clinical Practice and Research<br />

Early pioneers in music therapy described the positive impact of music therapy on<br />

children with autism in the seventies. Nordoff and Robbins (1977) illustrated the progress of<br />

a five-and-a-half-year-old boy with autism who was also emotionally and behaviorally<br />

disturbed. <strong>Music</strong>ally matching and meeting the boy’s emotional intensity of screaming and<br />

crying enhanced the child’s self-awareness, leading to “singing-crying response” that<br />

demonstrated features of musical relatedness in pitch, in rhythm and in melody patterns.<br />

With the assistance of improvised music, this boy was able to express enjoyment, engage in<br />

nonverbal conversational exchanges, use words more appropriately, and was more<br />

emotionally stable. Juliette Alvin (in Alvin & Warwick, 1991) described a case study with a


oy, diagnosed with autism, who was hyperactive and had uncontrolled strength, no speech,<br />

and seemed fairly unaware of his environment. During a short moment of quietness, Alvin<br />

struck a cymbal near to the boy’s face. Alvin describes his unexpected responds as follow:<br />

“His expression of wonder when he heard the sound turned into a radiant smile and he looked<br />

me in the eyes” (p. 4). In the following sessions the boy related to Alvin through the cymbal,<br />

learned to play the cymbal and listen to the sound.<br />

Meanwhile, the interest in and positive response to music, as well as improvements in<br />

core difficulties of children with autism through music, is extensively described in numerous<br />

case studies (e.g., Allgood, 2002; Alvin & Warwick, 1991; Brown, 1994; Clarkson, 1991;<br />

Fisher, 1991; Gustdoff & Neugebauer, 1997; Hales et al. 2002; Howat, 1995; Lecourt, 1991;<br />

Mahlberg, 1973; Nordoff-Robbins, 1968a, 1977, 1992; Schuhmacher, 1994; Warwick, 1995),<br />

and by anecdotal evidence from parents (e.g., in Gustdoff & Neugebauer, 1997; Nordoff &<br />

Robbins, 1992; Stevens & Clark, 1969; Saperston, 1973; Warwick, 1995). As documented<br />

by these authors, children with autism frequently exhibit a relatively high interest in and<br />

positive response to musical stimuli. Therefore, music has often been introduced to develop<br />

curiosity and exploratory interests in children with autism, and as a motivator to participate<br />

in activities aimed at achieving therapeutic goals (Nelson et al., 1984).<br />

Interest in music, and the relative strength of musical abilities in children with autism,<br />

is also supported by various research studies. Applebaum, Egel, Koegel and Imhoff (1979)<br />

measure musical abilities of children with autism and find that these children imitated<br />

individual tones and series of atonal tones including timbre as well as or better than typically<br />

53


54<br />

developing children. In an experiment by Thaut (1987), children with autism and two control<br />

groups (including a group of typically developing children) were given a choice between<br />

auditory musical and visual stimuli. Results indicated that children with autism showed no<br />

significant preference for the auditory musical stimulus, but engaged significantly longer in<br />

the musical stimuli than their typically developing peers. In a later study Thaut (1988) finds<br />

that children with autism score almost as well as typically developing children with respect to<br />

the rhythm, restriction, complexity, rule adherence, originality of improvised tetrachordic<br />

tone sequences on a xylophone, and scored significantly higher than children with mental<br />

retardations. Several additional studies support the interest in music and musical abilities of<br />

children with autism (e.g., Blackstock, 1978; Frith, 1972; Hairston, 1990; Sherwin, 1953).<br />

The effectiveness of music therapy interventions in addressing the characteristics of<br />

autism is documented by anecdotal reports, as well as numerous research accounts. One of<br />

the earliest studies addressing social skills in five boys diagnosed with autism comes from<br />

Stevens and Clarks (1969). By using a preexisting Autism Scale (Ruttenberg et al., 1966),<br />

pre- and posttest data were gained on the subject’s functioning in the following categories:<br />

nature and degree of relationship to adult, communication, and drive to mastery. During the<br />

18 weeks of music therapy interventions, a variety of instruments and techniques such as<br />

singing, improvising on the child’s responses and action songs were applied. Results<br />

indicate that four of five subjects significantly improved their skills in all categories. Only<br />

one subject made no significant gains. According to the authors, three of the children were<br />

able to attend regular kindergarten and first grade class afterwards.


Wimpory, Chadwick and Nash (1995) explore the effects of synchronizing live piano<br />

music to mother-child interactions (<strong>Music</strong>al Interaction <strong>Therapy</strong>, MIT) to foster the social<br />

and symbolic development of a young child with autism. The mood, timing, and meaning of<br />

the dyad’s activities were reflected musically on the piano to motivate the child to anticipate<br />

her mother’s actions. The result of this single case study and a two-year follow-up indicate<br />

an improvement in the child’s social acknowledgement, eye contact, and the child’s<br />

initiations of interactions, which obtained over time.<br />

In a case study by Brownell (2002), the effects of a musical presentation of a social<br />

story on the social learning and behavioral appropriateness of four first- and second-grade<br />

students were investigated. Social stories are individualized short stories sharing relevant<br />

information surrounding a particular situation or event to assist the child to understand the<br />

expected or appropriate response (Gray & Garand, 1993). Following three treatment<br />

conditions (i.e., baseline, reading social story, singing social story), Brownell finds that both<br />

reading and singing the social story to be significantly more effective in reducing target<br />

behaviors than baseline condition. For one participant, the musical presentation of the social<br />

story was more effective than the reading condition, whereas reading was slightly more<br />

successful in changing the other participants target behavior.<br />

Hollander and Juhrs (1974) introduced a boy named Bernie, diagnosed with autism<br />

who was able to speak, but was not motivated to use language. However, he did respond to<br />

chants and songs by dancing and moving to the rhythm. In a rondo form (as used in the Orff<br />

Schulwerk method), Bernie used words to identify “body parts.” Hollander and Juhrs<br />

55


56<br />

describe the further process as follows: “Then in an attempt to encourage Bernie to speak, all<br />

rhythm and music was omitted and the ‘A’ part of the rondo was ‘mimed’ silently. He<br />

immediately began pinching us [the authors] to attempt to make us sing the verse. When he<br />

realized he could not force us to sing, he began singing the verse and we joined in with him.<br />

Over a period of six months, his negativity towards verbal language has been greatly reduced<br />

and he is using more appropriate speech spontaneously in Orff-Schulwerk as well as in the<br />

ward family and on home visits” (p. 7).<br />

Edgerton, (1994) provides evidence of the effectiveness of improvised music on the<br />

communication behavior of eleven children with autism ranging in age from six to nine<br />

years. During 10 weeks of individual sessions based on the Nordoff- Robbins’ (1977)<br />

Creative <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> approach, the experimenter created music (i.e., piano and singing) as<br />

a means of contact, and enabled the children to respond and develop musical<br />

communicativeness. A reversal design and a specifically designed checklist (Communicative<br />

Response/Act Score Sheet, CRASS) that provided pre- and posttest data on the subject’s<br />

musical and nonmusical communication was used. During the reversal session playing and<br />

singing structured and pre-composed music versus improvised music were used. Analysis of<br />

the pre and post data following the categories of the checklist revealed that improvisational<br />

music therapy significantly and constantly increased the communication behaviors (musical<br />

vocal behaviors and nonmusical speech production) of each child within the musical setting.<br />

When reversal was applied, an abrupt decrease in data was noted in all subjects. Edgerton<br />

suggested that children with autism can make gains in communication during low-structured<br />

interventions. This notion contradicts the current literature suggesting structured approaches.


However, this statement does not find strong support in the application of the reversal design,<br />

because experimental control was only established by way of one data point. To measure<br />

consistency in response, it is essential that sufficient stability be exhibited (i.e., stable pattern,<br />

constant trend) (Aldridge, 1996; Holcombe, Wolery & Gast, 1994; Kazdin, 1982; Tawney &<br />

Gast, 1984). The resistance to change in individuals with autism was taken into account for<br />

the first and last intervention phase, but not during the reversal phase. In addition, a<br />

Behavior Change Survey was handed out to parents, teachers and speech therapists. Little or<br />

no change in the child’s communication behavior outside the music sessions was reported.<br />

A study by Bunday (1995) indicate that the use of manual signs in conjunction with<br />

music and speech with children with autism is significantly more effective in achieving<br />

accuracy of imitating signs and spoken words than the use of manual signs in conjunction<br />

with rhythm and speech. These results have been obtained by testing 10 children in a public<br />

school offering a special program for children with autism by using a within-subject design.<br />

Koska’s (1993) research investigation has examined the arm flapping, body swaying,<br />

and appropriate participation of a nine-year-old boy diagnosed with autism while attending<br />

regular elementary music classes in comparison to his special education classes. Data was<br />

collected by analyzing videotaped segments using time sampling techniques. The results<br />

suggest that all three behaviors were less frequent during regular music classes. Detailed<br />

analysis of the activities (i.e., singing, playing, moving, listening) revealed that the subject<br />

was most attentive during listening. Koska concluded: “This data appears to indicate that, for<br />

57


58<br />

this particular subject, being mainstreamed had a positive effect on his appropriate social<br />

behaviors” (p.1).<br />

Skille (1989) gave an overview of the effects and applications of VibroAcoustic<br />

<strong>Therapy</strong> (VA). In VA, low-frequency sinus tones in combination with selected music are<br />

transmitted as a vibration to the body. It is reported that after receiving VA, children with<br />

autism subsequently allowed more body contact and stimulation by staff than in other<br />

situations. However, no data are provided to support this observation.<br />

Schuhmacher (1999) evaluated the effects of a long-term music therapy treatment of a<br />

child with autism by using an analyzing system (Assessment of the Quality of the<br />

Relationship, AQR), including seven modi of contact/relation developed by the authors. In a<br />

detailed qualitative analysis of the children’s physical, vocal, and instrumental responses, the<br />

ability of this child in expressing himself and building relationships through music is<br />

demonstrated.<br />

Additionally, the literature contains several reviews that outline music therapy<br />

strategies and techniques accommodating the characteristics of autism (e.g., Thaut, 1984;<br />

Nelson et al., 1984; Toigo, 1992). There is no standardized music therapy assessment for<br />

children with autism, but Snell, (2002), Edgerton (1994), Nordoff & Robbins (1977), and<br />

Wigram (1995, 2000, 2002) have developed descriptive “codings” of musical relationship,<br />

communicativeness, behaviors, and adaptive skills.


Although methods and strategies used in educating and treating children with autism<br />

have changed over the past thirty years, the clinical documentation and research-based<br />

knowledge of the impact of music for children with autism continue to have value. Still,<br />

more updated intervention research is warranted that incorporates contemporary strategies on<br />

the effectiveness of music therapy interventions for children with autism.<br />

2.2.5.3 <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> in Inclusive Preschool Education Programs for Children with<br />

Autism<br />

It is currently unknown how many music therapists are providing services for<br />

children with autism in inclusive preschool settings, nor what model of service delivery is<br />

generally used. The music therapy literature contains only a few descriptions that outline<br />

music therapy services explicitly for children with autism in school settings. The following<br />

paragraphs describe three music therapy programs of key authors in this particular field.<br />

Snell (2002) describes serving students with autism in the Monroe County<br />

Intermediate School District, Michigan. <strong>Music</strong> therapy is included in the continuum of<br />

programs and services for Monroe County and serves many other students with special<br />

needs, including preschoolers with autism who are partly enrolled in inclusive settings. The<br />

music therapy program uses both an Integrated Service Delivery Model and a Caseload<br />

Service Delivery Model. That means music therapy services are provided as direct therapy<br />

(small and large group therapy), consultation (parents, teachers, support personnel,<br />

community), and workshops and presentations (parents, staff, community). The music<br />

therapy program emphasizes the connection to the community and the support of the child’s<br />

59


60<br />

individual growth toward independence. In several case studies, Snell describes the close<br />

collaboration with teachers and the importance of incorporating the student’s curriculum<br />

content in the music therapy intervention. According to Snell, it is imperative that music<br />

therapists working in school settings be familiar with strategies used with a particular student<br />

by other team members (e.g., behavioral approaches, communication systems, sensory<br />

program) to facilitate the student’s development and generalization of skills in different<br />

settings. She also notes that administrators, teachers and parents are often not aware of the<br />

unique qualities of music therapy in assessing and serving children with autism. Therefore, it<br />

is the music therapist’s role to educate and advocate making music therapy accessible for<br />

children with autism within public schools.<br />

Warwick (1995) reports on her work with school-aged children with autism, both in<br />

special and mainstreamed settings, within the Oxfordshire Education Authority in Britain.<br />

According to her, one of the major issues in providing music therapy services in educational<br />

settings is the economic climate and the lack of knowledge about music therapy. She states:<br />

“(…) music has seemed to become over-specialized. Rather than being central to our culture,<br />

it is too often considered only for those who are talented” (p. 209). It is the music therapist’s<br />

responsibility to share the value of music as a means of “(…) promoting the balance between<br />

the emotional, physical, intellectual and social development of the individual” (p. 209). Like<br />

Snell, Warwick also asserts that exchanging information on the child’s progress with<br />

teachers, specialists and parents is a vital part of working in public school systems.<br />

However, Warwick’s music therapy services are based on a psychotherapeutic approach and<br />

seen as a (…) “space and time to explore who [students with autism] are and how they can


elate to an adult who is not a teacher or classroom assistant - someone outside the world of<br />

school who can support and encourage a creative journey where music is the medium for<br />

self-discovery” (Warwick in Hales et al., 2002, p. 1). Students are seen once a week, both<br />

individually and in groups outside the classroom. Advisory services are provided to teachers<br />

regarding specific problems related to the child’s pathology, and are aimed at developing a<br />

suitable repertoire to be implemented by the teacher in the classroom.<br />

Furman (2002) describes the music therapy program at the Longfellow Early<br />

Education Center in Minneapolis, Minnesota. The center hosts two Early Childhood Special<br />

Education-Autism Spectrum Disorder (ECSE-ASD) classes among other ECSE and regular<br />

classes. <strong>Music</strong> therapy has been a part of the program since the seventies and plays a vital<br />

role in serving children with autism among other children with special needs starting from<br />

age two. Therapists practice inclusion by using reverse mainstreaming, mainstreaming, and<br />

special grouping, depending on the child’s readiness for successful inclusion. The program<br />

follows an integrated therapy approach (McWilliam, 1995). <strong>Music</strong> therapy sessions are<br />

integrated in the child’s classroom routine, and incorporate or generalize current therapeutic<br />

goals developed in collaboration with other team members for each child with autism.<br />

“Developing and demonstrating activities that allow the student to participate at their<br />

individual level and yet function as a part of the group is a key responsibility of the music<br />

therapy position” (p. 382). In addition, Furman (2001) gives clinical examples of how to<br />

combine music activities in accordance with the characteristics of autism and contemporary<br />

strategies used with children with autism during music and movement time within the<br />

classroom routine. For example, to provide successful experiences, she suggests<br />

61


62<br />

accommodating the need for structure and predictable routines of children with autism by<br />

starting classes at the same location, with a clearly identified place to sit, and the same<br />

opening song each time. Additionally, placing the child with autism across from the leader<br />

of the group and next to a good role model and friend might enhance the child’s<br />

understanding of the activity and by extension, his or her participation. Furman emphasize<br />

the need of visual cues (e.g., PECS) to indicate transitions and prevent problem behaviors.<br />

As an example, she illustrates a visual lesson plan, pictures for song selection, and pictures<br />

representing activities in action songs. To fully experience the music and movement<br />

activities, some children with autism might need additional support from an adult.<br />

According to Furman, children with autism experience social interactions, learn to imitate,<br />

and might accept transitions when participating in music group activities. She also gives<br />

examples of how skills learned during group activities can be generalized to free-play<br />

situations. Like Snell and Warwick, Furman states that it is essential to communicate and<br />

share information with teachers and incorporate strategies and goals used by the classroom<br />

teachers in order to be most beneficial for the development and growth of the child with<br />

autism.<br />

In summary, all authors report the unique potential of music for accommodating<br />

the needs of children with autism. As demonstrated in clinical documentations, music<br />

therapy has given students with autism positive learning experiences and has supported<br />

their personal growth. Each music therapy program has been recognized and valued by<br />

administrators and parents as an integral part of the excellence in programming for<br />

children with autism. However, the positive effects of the described music therapy


programs are not results of research investigations. More research is needed to<br />

understand the effects of music therapy for children with autism within inclusive<br />

preschool settings.<br />

2.2.5.4. Inside-Out Reports Related to <strong>Music</strong><br />

2.2.5.4.1 From Adults’ Perspectives<br />

Clinical documentations and research-based knowledge from various disciplines,<br />

along with parental reports, have provided a better understanding of autism and of the<br />

development of services for individuals with autism. Although there are unique<br />

characteristics that identify individuals with autism, as well as educational/therapeutic<br />

approaches that enhance the individual’s ability to participate meaningfully in their family<br />

and community life, no one individual with autism exhibits the same behaviors or responses<br />

in the same way. Professionals serving individuals with autism must realize that current<br />

perceptions of children with autism are limited. Reports from individuals diagnosed with<br />

autism who are articulate, and willing to share their experiences and perspectives, give<br />

valuable insight and can be helpful to professionals to increase understanding and awareness<br />

of the condition. Temple Grandin and Donna Williams, both diagnosed with autism in<br />

childhood, reference in publications the following musical abilities and the impact of music<br />

in their lives:<br />

Grandin (2003) reports that throughout elementary school, she had difficulties with<br />

speech, but ”singing however was easy” (p. 2). She also describes that melodies are the only<br />

nonverbal information she can memorize without a visual image (Grandin & Scariano, 1996).<br />

63


64<br />

She states: “I have perfect pitch and I can effortlessly hum back the tune of a song I have<br />

heard only once or twice” (Grandin, 2003, p. 2). Rhythms such as synchronizing clapping at<br />

a concert or following the rhythm of a conversation, however, seem difficult to her. Grandin<br />

believes rhythmic activities with instruments for children with autism enhance their rhythm<br />

skill she associates with having difficulties with speech (Grandin, 1988a). According to<br />

Grandin, music might also have value for children with autism because of its pleasurable and<br />

predictable component (Grandin, 1988b).<br />

Donna Williams (1996) describes having “memory for long strings of patterns” such<br />

as movement and sound patterns. When she was a child, words, pitch, movements and<br />

sounds of TV advertisements, jingles and tunes triggered a “mental replay” of the music<br />

accompanied with her fingers dancing to it in her. When she was a teenager, Williams had a<br />

deep experience with playing a piano, which she described as follows: “I had sneaked peeks<br />

at the piano and hit one note here and another note there. I was left in the room with it and I<br />

played the range of its notes (…). A composition created itself in my head. My fingers<br />

danced along the keys. The sound coming back was coherent and whole and beautiful” (p.<br />

245). Later in her life, she played various music styles on the piano, which had an emotional<br />

impact on her. “Some [music] created definite feelings of aloneness, some made me wild<br />

and manic, some made me cheerful, some felt ‘beautiful’ ”(p. 245). She never shared her<br />

music making with others, because she felt that what “came out” of her in music was too<br />

intimate and was anxious not to expose it. Through the lyrics and music of tunes of others,<br />

Williams experienced thoughts and feelings. She says: “The lyrics spoke my life and the<br />

music spoke my emotions” (p. 246). Williams recommends the use of songs for memorizing


a sequence (e.g., teeth brushing) and the use of sounds and musical compositions to express<br />

emotions. She adds: “To allow you to hear what the ‘autistic’ person has created can be an<br />

indirect sharing of emotions and true self. In this way, a musical ‘dialogue’ can be<br />

established back and forth in an equally indirect-confrontational way” (p. 298).<br />

Toigo (1992) describes synthesized Temple Grandin’s insights with practice in music<br />

therapy in accordance with the challenges and needs of individuals with autism. Another<br />

literature review on the insights of adults diagnosed with autism combined with implications<br />

and applications in music therapy practice, is found in Gottschewski (2001).<br />

Both Grandin (1988a) and Williams (1996) recommend the nurturing of talents and<br />

vocational skills of individuals with autism. For those who are musically gifted, a musical<br />

career can provide a socially valued arena for the expression of competence and the<br />

achievement of self-worth.<br />

2.2.5.4.2 Interview with a Ten-Year-Old Jazz Pianist<br />

The following chapter features an interview with a 10-year-old jazz-pianist diagnosed<br />

with high functioning autism. The intention of the interview was to learn more about the<br />

benefits and impact of music on individuals with autism from a child’s perspective.<br />

Although the young musician and his mother gave permission to publish his musical<br />

experiences and thoughts in this thesis, his name is omitted in order to respect his privacy<br />

and that of his family. The young jazz-pianist’s extraordinary musicianship and original<br />

compositions can be heard during live performances around the world and on several<br />

65


66<br />

recordings. He has met and played for many famous jazz musicians, and is the only child<br />

honored by a well-known piano company. In his music and recordings, his interplay with<br />

other musicians, empathy, creativity, joy and great humor can be heard. The author is<br />

grateful for his insights, his mother’s support and the opportunity to share his wisdom with<br />

the professional community.<br />

1. Does music get your attention when you are not playing an instrument yourself?<br />

Oh yeah, in fact you'll probably pass by me playing an imaginary piano.<br />

2. When do you like listening to music and when does music or sound bother you? Can you<br />

give an example?<br />

I like listening to music when I need some resting time and music or sound bothers me<br />

when I'm doing something like schoolwork.<br />

3. Why do you like jazz music most? Can you describe it in a picture or story? Playing jazz<br />

music is like...<br />

Because instead of being restricted to some melody or lyrics like some types of music you<br />

are free to do whatever you want. It's simply the liberty of playing freely that is missing in<br />

other types of music. Playing Jazz is like a world in which you can do anything you want,<br />

simply, and I feel like I'm in a new world.<br />

4. What is it in the music that fascinates you most? Is it the rhythm, melody, harmony, tempo<br />

or something else? Why?


The answer? None of the above. In fact, as I already told you, it's the freeness in jazz...<br />

and the solos are probably my favorite part.<br />

5. Is the structure of jazz pieces helpful? Why yes or no?<br />

Chords, yes. Melody, no. Well, the chords keep the piece from being free jazz. Without<br />

the chords, jazz would simply be Cecil Taylor Jazz (Free Jazz). Melody is simply the<br />

tune itself and the solos are pretty much what make it fun to listen to.<br />

6. Why have you chosen to play the piano? Are there other instruments you like to play,<br />

too?<br />

Well the piano gives more notes (88 of them), while other instruments have only a few<br />

octaves. I also like the sound of the piano. I like to play other instruments, but the piano<br />

seems to go with me.<br />

7. Is it more fun playing alone or with others?<br />

A jazz ensemble is a few people communicating together. A single person is like<br />

someone who is acting like everyone in the ensemble at once. The ensemble gets the real<br />

or imaginary people better communicated, which means better music. So, that's why I<br />

like the ensemble better.<br />

8. When you play with your jazz trio, how do the other musicians fit into your music?<br />

67


68<br />

The drum creates the rhythm, which holds the music together. The bass controls the<br />

form, which makes the piece be a piece (as in question 5). They, as I told you, make very<br />

good communication together.<br />

9. How does it feel playing with them?<br />

They are very good musicians, and they are good friends, and the combination of those<br />

two good things means a well-connected trio.<br />

10. How do you let each other know who is playing a solo or when to end the piece?<br />

To show the end of a solo, you let the intensity of the music drop down until it becomes<br />

almost nothing for a few measures, and then the people know it is time for a solo. You<br />

can also give cues with your body or you can talk softly. To end a piece, you always<br />

have an out chorus, and to show that the out chorus is coming up, the last soloist of the<br />

piece communicates with his body, mouth, or music to tell that the piece is going to end.<br />

This is the communication part of playing.<br />

11. What comes to your mind, when you play for thousands of people?<br />

Thousands of people simply means thousands of fans. I know that the thousands of<br />

people weren't built to make me nervous, which is why I usually DON'T get nervous.<br />

The thousands of people are just people who like my talent.<br />

12. Why do you think that young children with autism like to sing and play instruments?<br />

I think that when they get autism, they start getting the behavioral problems, and then


they start playing the instruments because they know it in their hearts that they can be<br />

good musicians, they just don't know it with their brain.<br />

13. What would you like to tell adults to think of or do when playing music with children?<br />

You can be honest! Tell us...<br />

Age is not a factor in talent. Babies are very good at some things and not at others.<br />

Children can also be stars. Shirley Temple did. And when I get together with two adult<br />

musicians, when the trio makes music, it doesn't sound like it is a kid playing. The adults<br />

simply don't know that kids can be as good as adults, and I would like the adults to know<br />

that.<br />

14. Is there something else you would like to share?<br />

Do you know about my new CD? It's called "(…)." It includes 10 songs, including four<br />

"world premier" tunes. Seven of the ten songs were written by me. You can find out<br />

more at my website.<br />

Note from his mother: “My son read your questions one at a time, and answered them in his<br />

own words. I transcribed exactly what he said. I found his insights to be amazing! I<br />

thoroughly enjoyed learning more about my son.”<br />

69


3. Methods<br />

Given the importance of early intervention and inclusion, and the increasing trend to<br />

enroll young children with ASD in regular childcare programs, there is a need to demonstrate<br />

innovative therapeutic techniques and interventions for successful inclusion. As mentioned<br />

in section 2.2.4.3, quite a few university-based model programs empirically demonstrate the<br />

effectiveness of individualized strategies for the inclusion of young children with ASD<br />

(Dawson & Osterling, 1997; Handleman & Harris, 2001; National Research Council, 2001).<br />

However, no model has yet demonstrated the effectiveness of music therapy interventions in<br />

these settings. The purpose of this cumulative study was to understand whether individually<br />

designed music therapy interventions will increase the performance of young children with<br />

autism during challenging routines in an inclusive childcare program, and whether teacher<br />

can embed these interventions in the ongoing classroom routine.<br />

The following intervention studies were conducted within the context of the “Center-<br />

Based Early Intervention Demonstration Project for Young Children with Autism” (Wolery<br />

et al., 2001), which operated within the Family and Childcare program of the Frank Porter<br />

Graham (FPG) Child Development Institute at the University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill<br />

(FPG Child Development Institute, 2003a, 2003b). The Family and Childcare program<br />

enrolled about 80 children from six weeks of age to five years old. Of the 30% of the<br />

children with disabilities, 11 were diagnosed with ASD. The Autism Project’s goals were<br />

threefold: to develop a model for serving children with autism in inclusive childcare classes;<br />

to provide families individualized support and assistance; and to train others to use the<br />

71


72<br />

model. The philosophy of the project was based on a synthesis of current practice and<br />

research-based knowledge in early childhood education, early intervention for young<br />

children with disabilities, and specific strategies developed for children with autism and their<br />

families. The design of the Autism Project was a cooperative venture between the FPG Child<br />

Development Institute and the Division TEACCH of the University of North Carolina at<br />

Chapel Hill. The University’s Division of Speech-Hearing Sciences and Division of<br />

Occupational <strong>Therapy</strong> provided additional support and consultation. Two components were<br />

important to the project: (1) the classroom component: inclusion of two and three year old<br />

children with autism, use of individualized and structured teaching, establishment of<br />

predictable routines and schedules, the use of integrated therapy; and (2) the family support<br />

component: regular communication and home visits, and adherence to family centered<br />

practices. The Early Intervention Program of the State of North Carolina funded this project.<br />

Because the children identified for this study participated in the Autism Project, the<br />

music therapy interventions attempted to follow parts of the projects’ conceptual design. As<br />

such, the interventions were undertaken in inclusive classrooms using the integrated therapy<br />

approach. Specific goals, strategies and procedures were individualized for each target child,<br />

and predictable routines, structured teaching, and visual cues were taken into consideration in<br />

the interventions’ design. In addition, parents and caregivers were partly included in the<br />

intervention and supported through frequent communication.<br />

The target children were diagnosed with ASD by external agencies using the<br />

Psychoeducational Profile-Revised (PEP-R), Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule


(ADOS), Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS),<br />

clinical observation, and parent interviews. The Psychoeducational Profile-Revised (PEP-R)<br />

assesses skills across a range of seven areas: Imitation, Perception, Fine Motor, Gross Motor,<br />

Eye-Hand Integration, Cognitive Performance, and Cognitive Verbal Performance (Schopler<br />

et al., 1990). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) is a semi-structured<br />

play-assessment, designed to elicit social, communication and behavior skills to aid in the<br />

diagnosis of autism. This test was originally designed for research purposes, and is<br />

recognized as an international instrument for the diagnosis of autism (Lord et al., 1999). The<br />

Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (Sparrow et al., 1984) is a structured interview designed<br />

to assess the child’s communication, social, and motor skills as they are used in daily life.<br />

And finally, the Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) is a 15-item behavior rating scale<br />

used to identify children with autism, estimate severity of the condition (from mild to<br />

moderate to severe), and differentiate autism from other disorders (Schopler, Reichler, &<br />

Renner 1988).<br />

Classroom peers directly participated in Experiment I and III, and caregivers in<br />

Experiment I, whereas classroom teachers participated in all three experiments. Consent was<br />

obtained from parents and teachers to share the results of these studies with the professional<br />

community. In addition, parents and teachers gave permission to use photographs, audio and<br />

video recordings of the study participants for publication, teaching, and professional<br />

presentations. Classroom peers included both male and females from different ethnic groups<br />

and were ages two to five. Children with and without disabilities attended the same<br />

classroom, where the philosophy followed developmentally appropriate guidelines<br />

73


74<br />

(Bredekamp & Copple, 1997). Individual Family Service Plan (IFSP) and Individual<br />

Education Program (IEP) objectives were addressed within the context of regularly occurring<br />

classroom activities and routines. Teachers were trained in using routine-based interventions<br />

(McWilliam, 1996), and a combination of curricular modifications, embedded learning<br />

opportunities, and specialized instruction to address individualized learning objectives<br />

(Sandall & Schwartz, 2002). All teachers had prior experience and expertise with young<br />

children with autism, and attended in-services on autism held by the investigator of the<br />

“Center Based Early Intervention Demonstration Project for Young Children with Autism.”<br />

None of them had used music therapy interventions prior to the study. The composition of<br />

each of the three experimental groups is summarized in Table 3.1.<br />

Table 3.1. Participant composition of experimental groups.<br />

Participant type Experiment I<br />

n<br />

Target children<br />

Peers with and without<br />

disabilities<br />

Teachers<br />

Parent/Caregiver<br />

2<br />

13<br />

5<br />

2<br />

Experiment II<br />

n<br />

1<br />

--<br />

1<br />

Experiment III<br />

n<br />

As described in the literature, songs are used by a wide range of professions working<br />

with preschoolers with and without disabilities to address academic, social, language and<br />

communication skills, as well as motor skills, and to express emotions (e.g., Enoch, 2002;<br />

Furman, 2001; Humpal, 1998). At the FPG childcare program, well-known children songs<br />

--<br />

4<br />

32<br />

6<br />

--


were part of the curriculum and practiced during group activities. Therefore, using<br />

individualized songs with young children with autism to increase their performance during<br />

challenging classroom routines seemed to be a successful and manageable approach for<br />

teachers to implement. In all experiments a unique song, matching the target child’s<br />

temperament and demands of the identified difficulties, was written and implemented by the<br />

classroom teachers after staff development activities were provided.<br />

The interventions addressed the following challenging moments: the morning<br />

greeting routine, multiple-step tasks within classroom routines, and peer interactions on the<br />

playground. Additional IEP goals, identified by the childcare’s interdisciplinary team, were<br />

included in the intervention’s design. Since the songs were the vehicles for accomplishing<br />

these goals, the song’s content and musical manifestation, as well as the staff development<br />

activities, played an important role in the study.<br />

The effects of the music therapy interventions were evaluated using single-case<br />

experimental designs (A-B-A-B withdrawal design, alternating treatment design, and<br />

multiple baseline design). This research methodology provides professionals in early<br />

intervention a controlled experimental approach to the investigation of a single child under<br />

different circumstances, as well as the flexibility to adapt the intervention to the child’s needs<br />

and the particular treatment approach. Experimental control is achieved within the child,<br />

meaning that each child serves as his or her own control by comparing the child’s<br />

performance across two or more conditions over time (Alberto & Troutman, 1995; Aldridge,<br />

1996; Barlow & Hayes, 1984; Hanser, 1995; Holcombe, Wolery, & Gast, 1994; Kazdin,<br />

75


76<br />

1982; Tawney & Gast, 1984; Wolery, Bailey, & Sugai, 1988). “Single-case designs<br />

highlight individual change in daily clinical practice and allow the practitioner to relate those<br />

changes to therapeutic interventions” (Aldridge, 1994, p. 335). Single-case experimental<br />

designs are commonly used across different professions in early intervention, and are a<br />

scientifically accepted and valid method (e.g., Stile, 1988). Such designs are not unknown to<br />

music therapists, and are quite prevalent in the literature (e.g., Nicholas, & Boyle 1983;<br />

Gregory, 2002). Surprisingly, only a few music therapists have done early intervention<br />

research using single-case experimental designs (e.g., Harding & Ballard, 1982; Kern &<br />

Wolery, 2001). However, as Aldridge (1998, p.335) has poignantly stated: “There are many<br />

methods; finding the appropriate method to answer the question that we are asking is the<br />

central issue.”<br />

Experiment I, II and III, including the specific introduction, research questions,<br />

method, results and discussion are described in greater detail in the three chapters that follow.


4. Experiment I: Increasing Independent Performance of Children with Autism<br />

during the Morning Greeting Routine<br />

4.1 Introduction<br />

The childcare day is filled with daily transitions between activities and routines<br />

within the classroom, as well as to and from the school setting. These transitions include<br />

arrival at the childcare program, greeting of teachers and peers, cleaning up materials,<br />

preparing for new activities, going from indoor to outdoor play, preparing for mealtimes, and<br />

getting down and up from naptime (Alger, 1984; Baker, 1992). Observational studies of the<br />

way young children spend time in center-based programs indicate that they spend substantial<br />

portions of their days making these transitions (Carta, Greenwood, & Robinson, 1987). For<br />

many children, whether with and without disabilities, one of the most crucial transitions is<br />

that from home to school. Sometimes children cry or are unhappy when separated from their<br />

parents or caregivers. Parents and caregivers might be stressed and unsure of how to cope<br />

with the situation (Alger, 1984). The child’s behavior might trigger contagious reactions<br />

from peers or peers may turn away from the crying child. (Osborn & Osborn, 1981).<br />

Making transitions is particularly difficult for children with autism. In fact, the<br />

preferences for sameness and sensitivity to changes are among the defining characteristics of<br />

the condition (Dawson & Osterling, 1997; Mesibov et al., 1997; National Research Council,<br />

2001). In addition, children with autism often lack understanding of symbolic gestures such<br />

as waving for signing hello or good-bye. This can make the morning greeting routine a<br />

challenging experience. A research study by Hobson and Lee (1998), conducted among<br />

77


78<br />

young adults with mental retardation and those with autism, investigate the spontaneous and<br />

prompted greeting and farewell behaviors toward an unfamiliar adult. Results of this<br />

comparison study indicate that persons with autism are less likely to express spontaneous<br />

verbal and nonverbal gestures of greeting and farewell, and are less likely to establish eye<br />

contact, even when offered a greeting. Compared with the control group, fewer people with<br />

autism smile or wave good-bye.<br />

A number of strategies have arisen to support children with autism in making<br />

successful transitions. These strategies include: (1) the use of structure and predictable<br />

routines (Dawson & Osterling, 1997; Marcus et al., 2001; Trillingsgaard, 1999; Wakeford,<br />

2002), (2) the use of visual cues (Bryan, & Gast, 2000; Dettmer et al., 2000; Quill, 2001;<br />

Schmit et al., 2000), and (3) the use of songs (Baker, 1992; Cole, 1993; Furman, 2001;<br />

Gottschewski, 2001; Williams, 1999). “Hello” and “good-bye” songs are often used in music<br />

therapy to establish a predictable routine and to structure the session through a clear<br />

beginning and end, to welcome and get in contact with the individual or group, to give the<br />

individual undivided attention and respect, and to establish awareness of where persons are<br />

and what comes next (Bailey, 1984; Hughes et al., 1990; Krout, 1987; Loewy, 1995;<br />

Schmidt, 1984). Many hello and good-bye songs are published in the music therapy<br />

literature (e.g., Hafner, 1998, 1999; Nordoff & Robbins, 1968b, 1995; Schnur-Ritholz &<br />

Robbins, 1999). All of these were invariably developed through music improvisation with<br />

clients, or uniquely composed for a particularly situation. While there is empirical support<br />

for using songs for transitioning, no studies have evaluated the effectiveness of greeting and


good-bye songs with young children with autism specifically within the context of a<br />

childcare program.<br />

The purpose of this initial experiment was to evaluate the effectiveness of a music<br />

therapy intervention designed to increase the independent performance of two young boys<br />

with autism, during the morning greeting time. This transition was chosen because the target<br />

children, who attended the FPG childcare program, were having difficulty entering the<br />

classroom in the morning. The difficulty they exhibited during the greeting time transition<br />

was characterized by problems separating from the caregiver; hitting, biting or screaming<br />

upon entering the classroom; difficulty engaging with materials in the classroom; and<br />

interacting with peers. The effect of these problems were that the target children’s caregivers<br />

reported feeling “bad” about leaving the child while he was upset, the parents of other<br />

children in the class reported that their children were “afraid of” the target child, and the<br />

teachers “dreaded” the arrival of the child, saying things like “it’s fine once he’s here, but<br />

just getting him here is hard.” It should be noted that the target children were having<br />

difficulties with this transition in spite of the fact that a set routine had been in place for<br />

several months.<br />

4.2 Research Questions<br />

The purpose of this study was to evaluate the effects of individually composed greeting<br />

songs on the independent performance of two young children with autism during the morning<br />

greeting routine, implemented by the classroom teachers. The evaluation was guided by<br />

three overarching research questions:<br />

79


80<br />

(1) Does the use of an individually composed song, sung by teachers, increase<br />

appropriate independent performance during the morning arrival routine of young<br />

children with autism?<br />

(2) Can classroom teachers learn to use the principles important to music therapy in a<br />

particular routine?<br />

(3) Does the song increase interaction of peers with the target?<br />

4.3 Method<br />

4.3.1 Participants<br />

The participants in this study were two young children with autism (n=2), typically<br />

developing children and children with other special needs (n=13), classroom teachers (n=5),<br />

and the target children’s respective caregivers (n=2).<br />

Target Children. Two boys with ASD, both three years old, participated in this study.<br />

Both children were diagnosed with ASD by external agencies, using the diagnostic tools<br />

described in chapter 3. The children used the Picture Exchange Communication System<br />

(PECS) (Bondy & Frost, 1994), as well as other visual cues and prompts to support their<br />

communication. Subject 2 participated in the study five months after subject 1, at which time<br />

a new school year had begun. Since most of the classes are structured newly each school<br />

year, subject 2 was attending the same class as subject 1. The children were selected for<br />

participation in this study on the recommendation of their classroom teachers,<br />

parent/caregiver and therapists based on their diagnosis of ASD, their difficulty with the<br />

morning greeting routine, and interest in and positive response to music.


Subject 1. Phillip was a 3-year, 5-month-old African-American,<br />

enrolled in the childcare program for 10 months prior to this study.<br />

Phillip was a lively and humorous child. However, he was functioning<br />

on the Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) in the mild to moderate<br />

range on the autism spectrum. Phillip’s communication skills were very restricted, he<br />

demonstrated a deficit in social interactions, and he had a limited interest in play.<br />

Occasionally, Phillip had episodes of staring into space and engaging in stereotypic<br />

behaviors such as spinning objects or flapping his arms. Phillip communicated by using the<br />

Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS) (Bondy & Frost, 1994). He was able to<br />

independently select a picture and give it to the appropriate communicative partner. He also<br />

had started to use some verbal sounds and a few words related to repeated social play<br />

routines, food, or songs. His interaction with toys was limited and consisted mostly of<br />

moving the objects around for a short period of time. He began to expand his ability to<br />

perform a play sequence (i.e., cutting out, drawing a picture, placing a sticker on a paper)<br />

with teachers’ support. Phillip showed sudden changes in mood and feelings, and he was<br />

easily frustrated and frequently protested vocally. He tended to withdraw himself from social<br />

interactions, but had a positive relationship with his family and familiar adults. Social peer<br />

interaction only occurred when supported by adults in structured play activities. Phillip was<br />

a large boy and sometimes used aggression (e.g., pushing, hitting, screaming) to start<br />

interactions with peers or was physically too rough. He also took away or destroyed<br />

belongings of other children. Peers seemed to be afraid of him and denied playing or<br />

exchanging toys with him. Phillip received speech/language and occupational therapy<br />

services while in the classroom.<br />

81


82<br />

Phillip tended to have difficulty with transitions and changes in routines. The<br />

transition from his mother to the classroom teachers was troublesome for Phillip every<br />

morning. The difficulty Phillip had during the morning transition was characterized by his<br />

refusal to enter the classroom; and slamming the door, escaping, lying on the floor, hitting,<br />

biting, or screaming upon entering the classroom.<br />

Phillip was quite interested in, and responded well to, music. He preferred<br />

noisemaker toys, and loved to listen to selected musical pieces. Rhythmically based music<br />

made him move and dance.<br />

Classroom Peers. In addition to the target child, six other children between two and<br />

three years of age were enrolled in the classroom. Peers included both males and females<br />

from different ethnic groups. Five of the children were typically developing and one of the<br />

classroom peers had Down Syndrome. The classroom peers participated in the study<br />

voluntarily. More specifically, children who came forward to greet the target child were<br />

included in the morning greeting routine. Thus, the particular children who participated each<br />

day varied, based on the children’s own actions and on class attendance.<br />

Classroom Teachers. A classroom teacher and an assistant teacher participated in the<br />

study. The classroom teacher had a bachelor’s degree and was certified in early childhood<br />

education. She had taught young children for four years. The assistant teacher had a high<br />

school diploma, was licensed to work with infants and toddlers, and was working toward her<br />

associate’s degree in child development. She had been teaching for two and a half years.


Both teachers participated based on their schedule (i.e., teachers’ start times are staggered,<br />

such that the entire childcare day has coverage). However, the lead teacher participated more<br />

frequently than the teaching assistant. Neither teacher had had prior experience with music<br />

therapy interventions.<br />

Target Child’s Parent. The target child’s mother requested strategies to facilitate the<br />

transition of her child from home to school. She participated in the study on a daily basis by<br />

bringing her child to the classroom and by participating in the greeting time procedures.<br />

From time to time, parents of peers participated by singing and greeting, when<br />

arriving while the intervention was going on. As the childcare program was affiliated with<br />

the university, students, observers and visitors were occasionally in the classroom or<br />

observing behind the one-way mirror in addition to the children and childcare program staff.<br />

Video Excerpt 4.1. Phillip’s morning greeting time behaviors during baseline<br />

condition.<br />

Subject 2. Ben was a 3-year, 2-month-old European-American,<br />

enrolled in the childcare program for 10-month prior this study. Ben<br />

was a beautiful and adorable boy. However, he was functioning on<br />

the Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) in the mild to moderate<br />

range on the autism spectrum. Ben had severe delays in<br />

communication, social interactions, and play, and engaged frequently in repetitive interests<br />

83


84<br />

and behaviors such as spinning around. Ben used the Picture Exchange Communication<br />

System (PECS) (Bondy & Frost, 1994), and was beginning to develop some functional<br />

speech. He had difficulties settling into play activities. His play involved mostly exploring<br />

and manipulating toys, and simple interactive games initiated by familiar adults. Ben was<br />

able to express emotions and show affections. He indicated joy and happiness through<br />

smiling, vocalization, jumping, or spinning around, discontent through crying, pushing an<br />

item away, or turning away from a person. Ben had limited interest in peers and was more<br />

social with familiar adults. Social peer interaction only occurred when supported by adults in<br />

structured play activities. Peers seemed not to take notice of him. Ben received<br />

speech/language and occupational therapy services while in the classroom, and engaged in a<br />

designed home program.<br />

Transitions and changes in routines were somewhat difficult for Ben. The transition<br />

from his caretaker (most frequently his nanny) to the classroom teachers caused great<br />

difficulties every morning. The difficulty Ben had during the morning greeting time<br />

transition was characterized by holding on to his caretaker, whining to intensive crying or<br />

screaming for a long period of time (between five and 25 minutes), ignoring the efforts of his<br />

classroom teachers’ greeting and play offers, stiffening his body, pulling teacher’s hair, or<br />

wetting his pants.<br />

Ben was very interested in actions that accompany familiar songs. He preferred<br />

listening to selected music (e.g., “Twinkle, Twinkle Little Star”), playing computer games, or


watching videos including music. Additionally he engaged in playing musical instruments,<br />

and soft and mellow music seemed to comfort and calm him.<br />

Classroom Peers. In addition to the target child, seven other children between three<br />

and four years of age were enrolled in the classroom. Peers included both males and females<br />

from different ethnic groups. Five of the children were typically developing, one of the<br />

classroom peers had Down Syndrome, and Phillip had been diagnosed with ASD. As with<br />

subject 1, classroom peers participated in the study voluntarily.<br />

Classroom Teachers. Ben’s lead teacher, together with a female and male assistant<br />

teacher, participated in this study. The lead teacher was the same teacher as for Phillip<br />

(subject 1). Neither of the two assistant teachers had been previously involved in any study<br />

based on music therapy. One assistant teacher had a high school diploma and had been<br />

teaching at the childcare program for two years. The other teaching assistant had a<br />

bachelor’s degree in a field unrelated to child development. It was his first year working<br />

with preschool-aged children. All classroom teachers participated equally in the<br />

intervention.<br />

Target Child’s Caregiver. Ben’s nanny participated in the study on a daily basis.<br />

She was responsible for transporting him to his classroom and picking him up after lunch.<br />

While Ben was participating in classroom activities, she frequently waited for him in the<br />

teachers’ lounge.<br />

85


86<br />

4.3.2 Setting<br />

Video Excerpt 4.2. Ben’s morning greeting time behaviors during baseline<br />

condition.<br />

All experimental sessions occurred in the classroom of the target child’s childcare<br />

program where the philosophy followed the guidelines for Developmentally Appropriate<br />

Practice (DAP) (Bredekamp & Copple, 2002). The classrooms were structured in clearly<br />

defined play areas such as a <strong>Music</strong>/Movement area, an Art area, a Language area, a Dramatic<br />

Play/Housekeeping area, a Manipulative area, a Block area, a Nature/Science area, and a<br />

Sensory Table area. Additionally, Ben’s class had a Computer area. Each area contained<br />

clearly labeled toys and materials corresponding to the given areas (Harms, Clifford, &<br />

Cryer, 1998; Marcus et al., 2001). Daily classroom schedules, predicable routines, and clear<br />

and concrete visual information were used to allow the children with autism to function most<br />

independently during the childcare day (Marcus et al., 2001; Schopler, et al., 1971). Located<br />

in the hallway outside the class was a cubby for each child where she or he placed her or his<br />

personal items. A picture symbol used by the subjects for the morning greeting routine was<br />

placed at the target children’s cubby.<br />

In the mornings, children arrived individually over a one-and-a-half-hour time period.<br />

Each child, and her or his parent, placed her/his personal items, in the child’s cubby, and<br />

entered the classroom together. The child would be greeted by and greet, the teacher and<br />

peers, then engage in play. The classroom curriculum allowed free play during the morning<br />

arrival time. Children engaged in different play areas by themselves or with each other. The


parents signed the child in and had a brief conversation with the teacher before saying “good-<br />

bye” to the child and leaving the classroom. Sometimes parents would stay for a short period<br />

of time and play or read a book to their child and/or classmates.<br />

4.3.3 Materials<br />

The materials used for Experiment 1 were as follows: (1) a picture symbol, (2) a<br />

unique song composed for each target child, and (3) a practice compact disc (CD)<br />

accompanied by a song transcription. Each is described in turn below.<br />

(1) Picture Symbol. Prior to the study, the teachers<br />

had used a 10 x 10 cm square laminated picture<br />

communication symbol (Mayer-Johnson, 1992)<br />

showing a waving stick figure and the word “Hello”<br />

87<br />

printed on the top using 18 pitch letters and the Arial<br />

font. The symbol was used to assist the target<br />

children to initiate greeting classroom teachers and<br />

peers, regardless of their language and<br />

communication skills.<br />

Figure 4.1. “Hello” picture symbol employed to greet classroom teachers and peers during<br />

the morning greeting routine.


88<br />

(2) Unique song composed for each target subject. The songs were individualized<br />

and matched each subject’s temperament and the demands of the morning greeting routine<br />

musically. The intention of both songs was to ease the transition from home to school, to<br />

increase the target children’s independent performance during the morning greeting routine,<br />

and to support their interaction with peers. The greeting routine, developed by an<br />

interdisciplinary team (i.e., classroom teachers, a special educator, and a music therapist),<br />

was similar to the greeting procedure used by the target children’s classmates. The form and<br />

lyrics of the “Song for Phillip” and “Song for Ben” focused on each step of the routine and<br />

were incorporated composed as follows:<br />

1. The teacher/peers greet the target child.<br />

2. The target child greets a person (teacher or peer) in the classroom.<br />

3. The target child greets a second person (teacher or peer) in the classroom.<br />

4. The target child says “good-bye” to the caregiver and the caregiver leaves the<br />

classroom.<br />

5. The target child engages with a toy or material found in the classroom.<br />

The five described steps are presented in a musical AAABA form and the lyrics as<br />

outlined in Figure 4.2 and 4.3. The A parts reflect steps 1., 2., 3., and 5 as the procedure<br />

happening in the classroom. Part B reflects step 4., the “good-bye” part, where the melody<br />

and mood of the song change in order to emphasis the detachment from the caregiver. Step 5<br />

(the last verse) encourages the target children to engage and settle into a play activity. Social<br />

interaction is supported by greeting at least two peers as the song proceeded, and by handing


over the picture symbol showing the stick figure waving “hello.” Additional Individual<br />

Education Plan (IEP) goals, such as verbalization for Ben and choice making for both Ben<br />

and Phillip were incorporated in both songs. Choice-making was included by choosing two<br />

different peers to greet, a toy to play with, and a weather condition at certain points of the<br />

songs. Verbalization was practiced by saying or singing “hello” to a person, naming the<br />

greeted person or saying “bye-bye” to the caregiver. The parents and caregivers were<br />

included in the songs by participating in singing, greeting peers and teachers, and waving<br />

good-bye. Table 4.1 furnishes an overview of the IEP goals incorporated in “Song for<br />

Phillip,” and “Song for Ben.”<br />

Table 4.1. Overview of Phillip’s and Ben’s IEP goals incorporated in “Song for Phillip” and<br />

“Song for Ben.”<br />

Make choices<br />

Increase verbal skills<br />

Increase peer interaction<br />

IEP Goals Song<br />

Choose different peers, toys/materials to play<br />

with, and a weather condition<br />

Say or sing “hello,” “good-bye,” or name peers<br />

Greeting a peer by handing over a picture<br />

symbol, saying/singing her or his name<br />

89


Figure 4.2. Transcript of ”Song for Phillip,” written to address Phillip’s demands of the<br />

morning greeting routine, social interaction and specific IEP goals.<br />

91


92<br />

Figure 4.3. Transcript of ”Song for Ben,” written to address Ben’s demands of the morning<br />

greeting routine, social interaction and specific IEP goals.


(2) Practice compact disc (CD) accompanied by a song transcription. Each unique<br />

song was recorded on separate practice CD and given to the teachers and parent/caregiver.<br />

To ensure accurate learning, each song on the practice CD was presented as follow: piano<br />

accompaniment and vocals with lyrics, to give the whole impression of the song, melody<br />

played by the piano, to learn the melody properly, sing along with piano accompaniment, to<br />

practice singing the song, and melody sung with lyrics, to model how to sing during the<br />

morning greeting routine. Additionally, each song was transcribed as music and lyrics,<br />

including melody, chords, and lyrics and handed out to the teachers and parent/caregiver.<br />

4.3.4 Procedure<br />

Experimental sessions occurred Monday through Friday during the morning arrival<br />

time. Phillip usually arrived between 8:00 and 9:00 am, and Ben between 8:30 and 9:30 am.<br />

Observation was based on the subject’s presence and availability of the teachers.<br />

Staff and Parent/Caregiver Development Activities. Initially, a verbal orientation to<br />

the intervention procedure was discussed with the teachers and parents/caregiver, as was a<br />

sense of how the intervention would fit into existing classroom routines. Training and<br />

consultation occurred after the intervention goals and procedures were identified and after<br />

songs were composed. The songs, and how to use the practice CD (as described in the<br />

material section), were introduced to the teachers and parents/caregiver individually, and to<br />

children during circle time prior to the intervention. Specific instructions were given to<br />

teachers and caregivers regarding how to greet and assist the target children to greet and<br />

interact with their peers. This included explaining and modeling:<br />

93


94<br />

• how to be prepared for the target child, by meeting him at the entrance of the<br />

classroom and be present;<br />

• how to personally greet the target child, by making eye and/or body contact, and<br />

going to the physical and emotional level of the child;<br />

• how to help the target child to find somebody to greet by gathering peers around<br />

him and prompt him to hand over the picture symbol or model to say “Hello”<br />

verbally when necessary;<br />

• how to wave or shout bye-bye as incorporated in the song and, for the caregiver,<br />

how to leave the classroom and wait outside if questions arise or information<br />

needs to be shared;<br />

• how to engage the target child in play by having toys/materials of interest of him<br />

available and within the teacher’s reach;<br />

• how to guide the child to a play area by offering a toy/material with which to<br />

play, if necessary;<br />

• how to follow the target child’s pace and give enough time to respond to each<br />

step, by pausing or slowing down in the music;<br />

• how to motivate and prepare peers to participate in greeting by singing the<br />

greeting song and modeling how to take the picture symbol, as well as giving<br />

verbal praise as positive reinforcement; and<br />

• how to communicate a positive attitude toward the target children by anticipating<br />

the greeting routine with enthusiasm and emphasis the strength of the target<br />

children.


The teachers were asked to include more than two peers in the greeting routine, if<br />

more than peers two showed interest in participating in the morning greeting routine. Staff<br />

training ended when the teachers and parents/caregiver signaled to be comfortable with the<br />

song and the procedure. During the intervention, feedback and suggestions about the<br />

implementation of the intervention were provided to the teachers and parent/caregivers.<br />

Baseline Procedure. During baseline condition (Condition A), the session started as<br />

soon as the target child and his parent/caregiver opened the classroom door. The child had<br />

already put his personal belongings in his cubby outside the classroom and picked up the<br />

picture symbol showing the stick figure waving ”hello.” First, teachers greeted the subject<br />

(step 1). Second, the subject greeted a teacher or peer by handing over the picture symbol,<br />

and/or saying the greeted person’s name (step 2). Third, the subject greeted a second teacher<br />

or peer by handing over the picture symbol, and/or saying the greeted person’s name (step 3).<br />

Fourth, the subject said or waved “good-bye” to the caregiver and the caregiver left the<br />

classroom (step 4). Fifth, the subject was encouraged to find a toy or material with which to<br />

play or engage (step 5). A system of least prompts (Doyle et al., 1990; Wolery, 1992) was<br />

used to both give the subject time to respond independently to each step of the greeting<br />

routine, but also to ensure that the subject completed each step of the greeting routine.<br />

Intervention. In the intervention phase (Condition B), the conditions were similar to<br />

the baseline conditions. The same five steps and the system of least prompts were used. The<br />

only change between the baseline conditions and the intervention conditions was the<br />

introduction of each subject’s unique song. The song, whose musical format and lyrics<br />

95


96<br />

matched each of the five steps of the greeting routine, was sung during each step of the<br />

routine. That is, for Step 1, the teacher sang the first verse of the song. Thus, as the routine<br />

progressed, so too did the song in tandem.<br />

Modified Intervention for Ben. Due to the fact that Ben’s (subject 2) number of<br />

independent behaviors did not change significantly with the introduction of the song<br />

(Condition B), an error analysis was conducted and a change in the intervention procedure<br />

implemented. The change in this modified intervention (Condition C) consisted of omitting<br />

the good-bye section (step 4) of the routine. This was done because Ben had ongoing<br />

difficulties separating himself from his caregiver. As soon as Ben entered the classroom<br />

independently, his caregiver left and was not visible for him during the entire time Ben spent<br />

in his daycare program. Teachers immediately started to sing and engage him in greeting his<br />

peers and finding a preferred toy/material with which to play. Toys/Materials of interest to<br />

him (e.g., a drum) were moved to the activity tables close to the entrance of the classroom.<br />

Notwithstanding all prompting procedures, remained the same.<br />

4.3.5 Experimental Design<br />

An A-B-A-B withdrawal design was used for Phillip (subject 1) to evaluate the<br />

effects of the individually composed greeting song sung by the classroom teachers, the<br />

caregivers and peers during the morning greeting routine. In an A-B-A-B withdrawal design,<br />

experimental control is established by comparing baseline performance (Condition A) with<br />

intervention performance (Condition B). If a difference in the data patterns exists each time<br />

the conditions change, and if those differences are replicated across conditions, then


experimental control is achieved. Specifically, if there is a stable pattern of data in baseline,<br />

followed by a change in the data pattern when intervention is introduced (that is, a change in<br />

level, a sustained trend in one direction or the other, or both), a counter-therapeutic change<br />

when the baseline condition is reintroduced, and finally another therapeutic change when the<br />

intervention is reintroduced, then the investigator can conclude the changes are related to<br />

intervention. This is considered to be a powerful demonstration of the effectiveness on an<br />

intervention because it controls for threats to internal validity (Aldridge 1994; Aldridge,<br />

1996; Hanser, 1995; Holcombe, Wolery & Gast, 1994; Kazdin, 1982; Tawney & Gast, 1984;<br />

Wolery, Bailey, & Sugai, 1988).<br />

The experimental design in Experiment I involved the following two conditions:<br />

• The Baseline (Conditions A) followed the five steps of the morning greeting routine.<br />

• The Intervention (Condition B) followed the five steps of the morning greeting<br />

routine by using Phillip’s unique greeting song.<br />

A variation of the same experimental design (A-B-C-A-C withdrawal design) was<br />

used for Ben (subject 2). As mentioned earlier, the song intervention (Condition B) was<br />

modified by eliminating the “good-bye” section (step 4) of the routine. Therefore the<br />

Modified Intervention (Condition C), followed four steps (steps 1, 2, 3, and 5) of the morning<br />

greeting routine using Ben’s unique greeting song. Data collection occurred over two month<br />

for Phillip and over three month for Ben.<br />

97


98<br />

4.3.6 Measurements<br />

The following measurement procedure and responds definitions applied for Phillip<br />

(subject 1): Seven categories of behaviors were measured through direct observation using<br />

an event recording system. That is, data were collected as the target child and adult<br />

performed each step of the greeting routine. Data were recorded during Conditions A and B<br />

(baseline- intervention) when the subject and his caregiver arrived at school before the<br />

transition to breakfast, when the teachers were present and available, and when classroom<br />

peers were present. Data were collected by direct observation during the morning arrival<br />

time. The observation time started as soon as the target child and his caregiver stepped over<br />

the threshold of the doorway that leads from the classroom to the hallway. Data collection<br />

time ended as soon as the target child picked up a toy/material in the classroom. Data<br />

collection stopped even if the target child had not said “hello” or “good-bye.” Data<br />

collection sessions lasted between two and ten minutes depending on the day. Some of the<br />

sessions were videotaped with a Panasonic AG-195 Camcorder and analyzed immediately<br />

afterwards. The event-recording system was employed as follows: (a) The observer made a<br />

judgment about the target child’s and adult’s behaviors in each step of the morning greeting<br />

routine; and (b) The observer recorded the results for each step on a specially designed data<br />

sheet (see Appendix A).<br />

The definitions of the categories were as follows:<br />

(a) Adult behavior<br />

• An Adult prompt was defined as a teacher or an adult other then the classroom teacher<br />

(i.e., the subject’s mother, etc.) assisting the target child. This assistance was either


verbal (i.e., “Say, ‘Hello.’”) or physical (i.e., the classroom teacher or adult put her<br />

hand on the subject in order to facilitate the exchange of the picture symbol).<br />

• No adult prompt was recorded if the teacher or adult did not need to (or did not)<br />

provide any support that helped the subject to perform the target behavior.<br />

(b) Child behavior<br />

• Independent response was defined as the target child performing the correct behavior,<br />

without any adult support.<br />

• Prompted response was defined as the target child performing the correct action, but<br />

needing a verbal on physical prompt by an adult to do so.<br />

• Error was defined as the target child either not following the sequence of the routine<br />

or engaging in an appropriate behavior not prescribed by the routine.<br />

• Inappropriate response was defined as the target child engaging in a behavior that<br />

was inappropriate (i.e., hitting, screaming, biting, etc.).<br />

• No response was defined as the target child not responding at all, even when<br />

prompted.<br />

These categories were coded for each of the five steps of the greeting routine.<br />

The measures used for Ben (subject 2) were similar to those used for Phillip (subject<br />

1). The only difference was that one additional measure was added to capture the number of<br />

classroom peers with and without disabilities greeting Ben independently during the morning<br />

greeting routine. The number of peers greeting independently was defined as peers receiving<br />

99


100<br />

the “Hello” symbol from Ben without verbal or physical prompting from an adult. This<br />

measure was added to answer research question 3: “Does the song increase interaction of<br />

peers with the target child?”<br />

Additionally, field notes were recorded for both subjects on the specially designed<br />

data sheet provided in Appendix A.<br />

4.3.7 Reliability<br />

Interobserver agreement data were collected on the subjects, and adult’s behavior<br />

during all conditions. A special educator and the investigator collected interobserver<br />

agreement data for Phillip during baseline session (Condition A), and intervention session<br />

(Condition B). Before the baseline was started the two observers coded the target child’s and<br />

the adult’s performance during the greeting time by direct observation until they reached a<br />

mean agreement level of greater than 80% for three consecutive sessions. A second observer<br />

(a research assistant) was trained to collect interobserver reliability for Ben (subject 2) during<br />

baseline sessions (Condition A), the intervention session (Condition B), and the modified<br />

intervention session (Condition C). Agreement between the scores from the first and the<br />

second reliability observer was calculated prior the start of the data collection phase, using<br />

videotaped segments from previously evaluated sessions, to establish an acceptable level of<br />

agreement of greater than 85%.<br />

Reliability checks were carried out on an average of 22% of total observations within<br />

each phase and for each child. Two coders coded the real-time performance or video taped


segments for each target behavior. Interobserver agreement levels for each code category<br />

were calculated by summing the number of agreements (defined as the same coding interval)<br />

and dividing the number by sum of agreements plus disagreements and multiplying by 100.<br />

4.4 Results<br />

4.4.1 Interobserver Agreement<br />

Interobserver agreement data on the target children’s performance were collected in<br />

21% of the baseline sessions (Condition A), 22.5% of the intervention sessions (Condition B)<br />

for each child, and 22.2% of the modified intervention session (Condition C) for Ben.<br />

During baseline condition (Condition A), reliability data illustrated observers agreed on the<br />

participants’ performance in 97.7% (range: 90.9% - 100%) of the trials. The percentage of<br />

agreement was 100% for Phillip, and 96.9% for Ben. During the intervention phase<br />

(Condition B), reliability data demonstrated observers agreed on the participants’<br />

performance in 90.7% (range: 75% - 100%) of the trials. More specifically, the percentage<br />

of agreement on the participant’s responses was 92% for Phillip and 87.5% for Ben. During<br />

the modified intervention phase (Condition C), reliability data showed that observers agreed<br />

on Ben’s performance 100% of the time. The overall interobserver agreement was 94%<br />

(range: 75% - 100%). Table 4.2 gives a summary of the interobserver agreement.<br />

101


102<br />

Table 4.2. Interobserver agreement in Conditions A, B, and C.<br />

Participants Condition A in % Condition B in % Condition C in %<br />

Phillip 100 92 N/A<br />

Ben 96.9 87.5 100<br />

Overall 97.7 90.7 N/A<br />

4.4.2 Outcome Data<br />

The data associated with Experiment I present two outcomes: the number of<br />

independent performance during the morning greeting routine for both Phillip and Ben, and<br />

the number of peers greeting Ben during the morning greeting routine.<br />

(1) Number of independent performance during the morning greeting routine<br />

Both subjects with autism had a low level of independent performance (that is,<br />

completing the correct steps of the five-step greeting routine without any adult support)<br />

during baseline condition. With the introduction of the song intervention (Condition B), their<br />

independent performance increased. This result was statistically significant for Phillip. After<br />

the intervention was modified, Ben showed a significant increase in his independent<br />

performance, too. After withdrawing the song intervention, both observers recorded a drop<br />

in children’s independent performance. After re-introducing the intervention conversely,<br />

Phillip’s and Ben’s independent performance was back to a very high level of independent<br />

steps. Both children ended the intervention at the highest level. The number of peers<br />

greeting Ben changed from no greeting during baseline condition to two peers greeting,<br />

which was part of the original composition of the song.


Phillip. Phillip was observed for a total of 28 sessions. Figure 4.4 shows the number<br />

of steps of the greeting routine that Phillip completed independently by each condition of the<br />

study. In the first baseline condition, Phillip’s performance was steady. In all of the sessions<br />

except session number four, Philip completed two steps of the five-step routine<br />

independently. In session four, he did not do any of the routine independently. The steps<br />

Phillip completed independently were entering the classroom and finding a toy with which to<br />

play. With the introduction of the song intervention, Phillip’s performance initially dropped<br />

to one step independently (entering the classroom), but after two days of song intervention,<br />

Phillip’s performance was back at baseline level. By the forth day of intervention, Phillip’s<br />

performance was better than at baseline level and by the sixth session, Phillip’s performance<br />

was consistently higher than the baseline level. Throughout the intervention phase there was<br />

a steadily acceleration, with a slope that indicates that Phillip’s performance steadily moved<br />

toward independence. After ten sessions in intervention, Phillip’s performance appeared<br />

consistent, as evidenced by three consecutive sessions with four independent steps at the<br />

same level so that the intervention was withdrawn. Phillip’s performance immediately<br />

decreased from intervention level. By the second day of withdrawal, Phillip’s performance<br />

had returned to the level of his initial baseline performance with two independent steps<br />

(again entering the classroom and finding a toy to play with). Once Phillip’s performance<br />

was stable at baseline levels, the song intervention was again introduced. Immediately,<br />

Phillip’s performance increased. After four sessions, Phillip’s performance was equal to his<br />

performance at the end of the initial intervention phase. His performance remained steady at<br />

this level until the ninth session of intervention where Phillip performed all of the steps of the<br />

routine independently.<br />

103


104<br />

Number of Independent Responses<br />

5<br />

4<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1<br />

0<br />

Baseline Intervention Baseline Intervention<br />

0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 22 24 26<br />

Sessions<br />

Figure 4.4. Number of independent responses performed by Phillip during the morning<br />

greeting routine in baseline and intervention sessions.<br />

Ben. Ben was observed for a total of 31 sessions. Figure 4.5 shows the number of<br />

steps of the greeting routine that Ben completed independently during each condition of the<br />

study. In the initial baseline condition, Ben’s performance was steady. In the majority of the<br />

sessions, Ben had one independent correct response. The step Ben completed was entering<br />

the classroom independently. On session four of the first baseline condition, Ben had three<br />

independent steps. With the introduction of the intervention, Ben’s performance was quite<br />

variable. Ben responded in the majority of sessions with one independent response, which<br />

was the same as during baseline conditions. In four out of 12 sessions, Ben completed more<br />

of the greeting routine steps independently. Overall, the data did not indicate that the<br />

implementation of the song significantly affected Ben’s greeting time behavior. After<br />

conducting an error analysis, it was determined that the “good-bye” part of the routine was


difficult for Ben. Therefore the routine was changed, and the formal “good-bye” was<br />

omitted. The modified song intervention (Condition C) resulted in an abrupt and sharp<br />

increase in the level of data. Ben had consistently been performing three out of the four steps<br />

of the routine independently. After five sessions of consistent performance, the intervention<br />

was withdrawn. There was an immediate decrease in Ben’s independent behavior ending on<br />

baseline level, which was evident in three out of the four withdrawal sessions. Only one<br />

session during the withdrawal was at the level seen during the modified intervention<br />

condition. Overall, there was a descending trend in this condition, and the behaviors were<br />

lower then they were during the modified intervention condition. Immediately after the re-<br />

introduction of the intervention, the data resulted in an abrupt and sharp increase. Ben was<br />

completing four (out of four) steps of the greeting routine independently. This high level of<br />

performance was consistently exhibited during the entire condition.<br />

105


106<br />

\<br />

Number of Independent Responses<br />

5<br />

4<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1<br />

Baseline Intervention Modified Baseline Modified<br />

Intervention Intervention<br />

0<br />

0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 22 24 26 28 30 32<br />

Sessions<br />

Figure 4.5. Number of independent responses performed by Ben during the morning<br />

greeting routine in baseline, intervention, and modified intervention sessions.<br />

(2) Number of peer greeting Ben during the morning greeting routine<br />

Figure 4.6 shows the peers’ independent participation in the classroom greeting<br />

routine. The data presented show both typically developing peers and peers with disabilities.<br />

However, 94% of the peers who independently participated in the song intervention were<br />

typically developing. During the six days of the initial baseline condition, no peers<br />

participated in the greeting routine (Condition A). With the implementation of the song<br />

intervention (Condition B), the greeting behavior of the peers changed. As soon as the song<br />

intervention was introduced, at least two classroom peers participated in the intervention.<br />

For the remainder of the study, in all but one of the experimental sessions, peer participation<br />

was above baseline level. The majority of the sessions had two peers participating in the


greeting routine. On three occasions, more than two peers participated in the routine. This<br />

was accomplished by the spontaneous decision of the teacher to repeat the greeting verse of<br />

the song in order to accommodate all of the classmates who wanted to participate in the<br />

greeting routine. Neither the modification of the intervention nor the withdrawal of the song<br />

intervention returned the peers’ behavior to baseline conditions, with the exception of one<br />

day. Nevertheless, it should be noted that in the withdrawal phase of the study the trend for<br />

classmate participation decelerates.<br />

Number of Peers Greeting Ben<br />

5<br />

4<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1<br />

0<br />

Baseline Intervention Modified Baseline Modified<br />

Intervention Intervention<br />

0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 22 24 26 28 30 32<br />

Sessions<br />

Figure 4.6. Number of peers greeting Ben during the morning greeting routine in baseline,<br />

intervention, and modified intervention sessions.<br />

107


108<br />

4.5 Discussion<br />

The purpose of Experiment I was to investigate the effects of individualized greeting<br />

songs on the independent performance of two young children with autism during the morning<br />

arrival time at an inclusive childcare program. Additional purposes were to determine<br />

whether teachers could implement the intervention in the ongoing classroom routine after<br />

staff development activities were provided, and whether the intervention increases the peer<br />

greeting behavior and positive interaction towards the target children.<br />

From this study the following findings are apparent. Prior to the song interventions,<br />

the data indicate that Phillip and Ben had great difficulties with the transition from home to<br />

school and following the classroom’s morning greeting routine. Both individualized greeting<br />

songs were effective at increasing the target children’s independent performance during the<br />

morning greeting time, although the intervention for Ben needed some modification. The<br />

interventions also had positive effects on peers’ greeting behavior toward the target children,<br />

as both the informal observation for Phillip and the data based evaluation for Ben show. As<br />

for the positive outcomes of the interventions, it can be concluded that classroom teachers<br />

successfully implemented the interventions based on music therapy principles into the<br />

ongoing classroom routine. The following field notes and reports by the target children’s<br />

classroom teachers, caregivers, and parents of peers also indicate these positive changes.<br />

Phillip. In the initial baseline phase, Phillip performed only two steps out of the<br />

morning greeting routine and no peer greeting occurred. During the first two days of<br />

intervention, his independent performance initially dropped to a lower level of data as in the


aseline phase. This observation is consistent with the defining characteristics of autism.<br />

That is, children with autism might insist on specific routines and can become very<br />

distressed, when change occurs (American Psychiatric Association, 2000; Dawson &<br />

Osterling, 1997; National Research Council, 2001). Phillip’s familiar routine was changed<br />

by having his teacher sing the greeting song to him and following the intervention procedure.<br />

In addition, the novelty of the song, and the lack of experience with implementing<br />

interventions based on music therapy, caused some excitement in the teacher’s behavior that<br />

might have also transferred to Phillip and his peers. However, as the song intervention<br />

continued, he made great progress in learning the steps and sequence of the routine, including<br />

greeting peers and adults. Phillip was able to follow all steps out of the greeting routine<br />

independently, except the “good-bye” part. It seemed to be difficult for him to understand<br />

the symbolic gesture of waving good-bye. This finding is similar to that in earlier research<br />

showing that individuals with autism are less likely to express spontaneous verbal or<br />

nonverbal gesture of farewell (Hobson & Lee, 1998). Clinically, this step needed to be<br />

practiced and given meaning for him. To understand and accomplish the farewell gestures,<br />

his mother and teachers needed to set clear prompts and definitions of saying good-bye. The<br />

withdrawal of the intervention resulted in an immediate decrease in his independent<br />

performance. When the greeting song was re-introduced, his independent performance went<br />

back to the prior intervention phase. On the last day of intervention, Phillip performed all<br />

steps out of the morning greeting routine correctly and independently.<br />

109


110<br />

A number of observations related to Phillip’s IEP goals are no less interesting than<br />

the foregoing findings. Phillip finally verbalized “Ba” for good-bye or flapped his right hand<br />

quickly next to his leg as a good-bye gesture. Most of the time this quick response was not in<br />

the order of the set greeting routine and was therefore coded as an error. Phillip also<br />

verbalized the greeting part by saying “Heho” for hello, and exchanged the picture symbol<br />

first with an adult prompt, then independently. Most of the time Phillip independently chose<br />

a toy or material with which to play. He preferred going to the art area to draw with crayons,<br />

play with playdough, or cut out paper pieces and glue them on a paper. Phillip seemed to be<br />

less frustrated when transitioning from home to school. Many times he smiled during the<br />

morning greeting time and independently approached his peers for greeting. Although he<br />

was still physically too rough at times, peers accepted him. The implementation of the<br />

intervention changed the classmates’ behavior and interaction toward Phillip. Peers<br />

volunteered in singing and greeting him, or participated by giving their input to the song<br />

(e.g., statements about the weather condition) while engaging in other activities. Some peers<br />

had stronger relationships with Phillip than others. One three-year-old African-American girl<br />

was particularly interested in interacting with him through the greeting song and was able to<br />

get his attention. Phillip often chose her to greet and gave her the picture symbol or said<br />

“Heho” to her. On many days more than two peers asked for a turn to say “Hello” to Phillip.<br />

It should also be noted that the many consumers of this study (the classroom teacher, the<br />

target child’s parent, and the parents of the other children in the classroom) believed that the<br />

intervention was clinically effective and valuable. One of Phillip’s peers’ mothers said that,<br />

prior to the study, her child had been intimidated by Phillip’s inappropriate behavior at<br />

greeting time. With the implementation of the song, her child would run to school hoping to


arrive before Phillip so that he could participate in Phillip’s greeting routine. Phillip’s<br />

mother reported that she was very pleased by the success of the intervention and requested<br />

further songs for other challenging situations.<br />

A second individualized song to facilitate waiting was written for Phillip. A<br />

transcription of the waiting song is shown in Figure 4.7. The goal of this song was to assist<br />

Phillip learn the concept of waiting. The two measures of rests in measures six, seven, eight,<br />

and nine are meant to encourage the child to wait for two periods of seven beats. The<br />

waiting is rewarded by paying attention to the child in the last line of the song saying, “Now<br />

it’s time for child’s name and me.” Counting more than seven beats can extend the waiting<br />

time. The song was introduced and practiced during circle time in Phillip’s class by using<br />

sign language for “wait,” and different instruments or body percussion to indicate the waiting<br />

beats. A compact disc (CD) recording and transcription of the song was handed out to the<br />

teachers and to Phillip’s mother. Phillip’s teachers and mother were encouraged to use the<br />

waiting song during ongoing classroom routines and during waiting situations outside school<br />

(e.g. while doing groceries). However, no data were collected on this song intervention.<br />

Video Excerpt 4.3. Phillip, his classroom peers, parents and teachers during a song<br />

intervention session (Condition B) of the morning greeting routine.<br />

111


112<br />

Figure 4.7. Transcript of the song ”Wait,” written for Phillip addressing the ability to wait.


Ben. In the majority of the initial baseline and song intervention phases of the<br />

morning greeting routine, Ben entered the classroom independently. Most of the sessions<br />

were accompanied by crying or screaming, and by the behaviors described in chapter 4.3.1.<br />

During the same baseline sessions, Ben only greeted one person with a teacher’s prompt,<br />

while in the initial song intervention sessions, Ben responded to every single prompted<br />

greeting and started to greet peers independently while crying. This performance, however,<br />

was coded as either “prompted response” or “inappropriate response.” As with Phillip, the<br />

farewell part of the song, combined with the unclear departure of the caregiver, was difficult<br />

to understand and handle for Ben. It was not until the formal good-bye was omitted and the<br />

modified song intervention was introduced to him that Ben stopped crying for most of the<br />

sessions and followed three steps of the greeting routine without showing any inappropriate<br />

behaviors. The withdrawal of the modified song intervention resulted in an immediate<br />

decrease in his independent performance, ending at baseline level. When the greeting song<br />

was re-introduced, Ben completed all steps of the modified morning greeting routine<br />

independently and for four consecutive sessions. In addition, the data collected on Ben’s<br />

classroom peers confirmed the informal observations made during the intervention with<br />

Phillip. That is, the implementation of the song intervention changed classmates’ greeting<br />

behavior toward Ben and increased peer interaction. In the initial baseline phase, no peer<br />

greeting occurred. As soon as the song was introduced, Ben’s classroom peers showed an<br />

interest in greeting and interacting with him. However, Ben’s difference in pace and<br />

response, as well as his lack of conventional nonverbal communication (i.e., eye-gaze, facial<br />

expression, and body posture), required some mediation by the children’s classroom teachers.<br />

The peer greeting behavior never went back to the initial baseline level with the exception of<br />

113


114<br />

the twentieth session, during which classroom peers were participating in a spontaneous<br />

dance and movement activity and were not available for greeting. It is especially important<br />

to note that this change was effective by the intervention alone and was not contingent on<br />

Ben’s performance. That is, the greeting song motivated the peers to interact with Ben.<br />

Ben’s performance did not change until the twelfth day of the intervention, yet the peers’<br />

behavior changed on the first day of intervention. Further, during the withdrawal phase,<br />

Ben’s independent behavior decreased whereas the peers’ behavior remained consistent.<br />

Much like the case of Phillip, with Ben there are additional observations related to his<br />

IEP goals that are noteworthy. Ben’s verbal skills increased through his verbalizing the<br />

peer’s names during the morning greeting routine and singing parts of the song during the<br />

intervention and throughout the day. The song intervention evoked a positive view and<br />

piqued the interest of peers toward Ben, which was evident in peer comments and actions.<br />

During the modified intervention phase, Ben’s peers noticed his change and made remarks<br />

such as “He doesn’t cry anymore,” or “He did a great job.” Peer interaction frequently<br />

continued when Ben played a floor drum or a hand drum that was offered to him as a toy to<br />

play with for the last step of the routine. Ben started the day in the childcare program<br />

without crying or screaming. On many days he entered the classroom with a smile on his<br />

face, joyfully jumped up and down, and vocalized while looking for a peer to greet.<br />

Furthermore, the stress level of teachers and parent/caregiver seemed to be reduced due to<br />

the smooth transition of the target children during arrival time.


From the comments and reports by the teachers, parent and caregiver alike, the<br />

intervention was determined a success as well. Ben’s caregiver, who was responsible for the<br />

transition from home to school said: “I think this was perfect for Ben. He had a hard time<br />

leaving me in the mornings, but with the help of the ‘Good Morning Song’ the transition<br />

become much easier for Ben.” After evaluating the intervention and research procedures<br />

with the lead teacher, she came to the following conclusion: “Transitions into the classroom<br />

were stressful for the children, parents and teachers. The ‘Hello Songs’ allowed us to<br />

implement a simple intervention each day. The songs are great and helped all of us<br />

tremendously.”<br />

Video Excerpt 4.4 Ben, his classroom peers, and teachers during a modified<br />

intervention session (Condition C) of the morning greeting routine.<br />

In conclusion, individualized greeting songs matching the subjects temperament and<br />

the demands of the morning greeting routine are effective in facilitating the transition from<br />

home to school, and in enhancing the morning greeting behavior of young children with<br />

autism in ongoing classroom routines. These findings support the proposed strategy to use<br />

songs for smooth transitioning for individuals with autism (Baker, 1992; Cole-Currens, 1993;<br />

Furman, 2001; Gottschewski, 2001; Williams, 1999), and they provide evidence that greeting<br />

songs can be effective in increasing the target children’s independent performance during the<br />

morning greeting time.<br />

115


116<br />

In addition, the greeting song interventions provided a valuable step toward inclusion<br />

for the target children and their families. Teachers, classroom peers and parents of peers had<br />

a more positive attitude toward the target children, as evidenced by their comments and<br />

reports. This replicates the results of earlier findings in which music therapy interventions<br />

increased the social acceptance and improved attitudes of school-aged and preschool children<br />

toward their peers with disabilities in inclusive school settings (Jellison et al., 1984; Humpal,<br />

1991).<br />

The teachers successfully embedded the intervention in the ongoing classroom<br />

routine, even though they did not previously know the greeting songs, nor did they have<br />

formal musical training or experience with music therapy interventions. Additionally, they<br />

had very short training time and ongoing classroom demands to manage. This replicates and<br />

extends earlier studies showing that classroom teachers successfully implemented<br />

interventions related to other disciplines in ongoing activities (Garfinkel & Schwartz, 2002;<br />

Kaiser et al., 1993; Kemmis & Dunn, 1996; Sewell et al., 1998; Venn et al., 1993; Wilcox &<br />

Shannon, 1996; Wolery et al., 2002). In previous studies, however, the interventions were<br />

not based on music therapy principles and guidelines.<br />

Despite their success, teachers were challenged with parts of the musical<br />

characteristics of the songs. For example, in both cases the teachers did not implement the<br />

change in music indicating the good-bye part of the songs (part B of the song). Interestingly,<br />

and perhaps coincidentally, it was exactly this part, that distressed both target children. This<br />

raises the question if the implementation of the change in music signaling the “good-bye”


part would have changed the target children’s performance during this step. No data are<br />

available that suggest that the change in music would have resulted in a change of the target<br />

children’s behavior, and other explanations for the children’s difficulty with this step (e.g.,<br />

the lack of understanding of conventional gestures) might be more likely. Furthermore,<br />

teachers did not explicitly perform musical elements such as expression or change in tempo<br />

and dynamics. The songs were mostly sung in a linear voice with little punctuation. Elefant<br />

(2001) describes musical elements such as tempo, vocal play (sound effects), inner tempo<br />

(accelerando, riterdandos and fermatas), dynamics, rhythmical and melodic patterns and<br />

upbeats as having influence on the song preference and response of girls with Rett<br />

Syndrome. We do not know if these or other musical elements would have an influence on<br />

the response of young children with autism, too. This study did not provide experimental<br />

analysis of the specific mechanisms supporting the treatment effects; the procedure was<br />

based on the assumption that the songs would improve the target children’s morning greeting<br />

routine.<br />

For future clinical applications, it might be more practical to use songs that require<br />

fewer musical skills and/or provide more intensive staff training where specific musical<br />

elements and techniques are mediated. At least for this study, assurance, motivation and<br />

monitoring of the teachers’ implementation of the interventions were needed. Therefore,<br />

prior to, during and after the intervention procedure, ongoing collaborative consultation was<br />

provided. And yet, we do not know whether or not teachers’ implementation of the<br />

intervention would continue or decline over time if they were not supported. Put simply,<br />

there exists no counterfactual against which to compare this component with the intervention.<br />

117


118<br />

This study admittedly has several limitations. For one, only a small number of<br />

participants were involved. This limitation represents an unfortunate –albeit inevitable –<br />

drawback of using the single-case experimental design. In order to understand the<br />

generalizability of the intervention, replication with multiple participants is warranted. Still,<br />

replication and generalization of these findings might be difficult, when the intervention is<br />

only implemented if the skills are useful to the child in the instructional environment and the<br />

vast difference between individuals with autism is taken in account. It cannot be assumed,<br />

based on the results of this one experiment, that these results would be replicable with other<br />

individuals, other settings, or both.<br />

Further more, Ben’s data indicate that the use of the modified intervention<br />

successfully increased his independent behavior during the greeting time routine. But due to<br />

limitations of the experimental design, a strong statement cannot be made about the<br />

effectiveness of the modified intervention alone. That is, because Ben had experience with<br />

the first intervention before the modified intervention, it is not known whether the modified<br />

intervention (without the exposure to the initial intervention) would have yielded a change, or<br />

such an immediate change, in Ben’s level of independent performance. This limitation refers<br />

to the threat to validity commonly known as learning effects.<br />

Another limitation of this study is that we do not know if other songs or interventions<br />

would have been equally or more effective. However, it is important to underscore that it<br />

was neither the purpose of this study to examine the effects of different greeting songs, nor to<br />

evaluate the superiority of greeting songs versus other strategies used for transitioning.


Rather, it was the intent to demonstrate the progress that might be accomplished by young<br />

children with autism through the use of individualized song interventions more generally.<br />

Further study should delve more deeply into specific intervention contours that result in<br />

divergent outcome levels.<br />

Several issues should be addressed in future studies. Future studies should also<br />

investigate the effects of musical elements, such as expression or change in melody, tempo<br />

and dynamics on the behavior of young children with autism. It would also be valuable to<br />

investigate social acceptance of, and attitude changes toward, children with autism in<br />

inclusive childcare programs resulting from music therapy interventions. In order to<br />

understand the generalizability of the intervention, replication with multiple participants is<br />

warranted. Further research is desirable that will provide evidence that music therapy<br />

interventions can be implemented as embedded strategies in childcare settings.<br />

119


120


5. Experiment II: Increasing Independent Performance of a Child with Autism during<br />

Multiple-Step Tasks<br />

5.1 Introduction<br />

The childcare day is not only filled with transitions, but also with many routines,<br />

which are repeated on a daily time schedule. Some of these routines include everyday skills<br />

such as hand-washing, toileting, and cleaning-up. These routines require children to follow a<br />

sequence of steps. “A child with autism typically has significant difficulty organizing and<br />

sequencing information and with attending to relevant information consistently” (Boswell &<br />

Gray, 2003, p. 1). In order to function independently in daily life, children must learn to<br />

understand, remember and perform the sequence of these tasks. Toward this end, strategies<br />

similar to those for transitioning are used to support children with autism in managing<br />

multiple-step tasks successfully. These strategies include: (1) the use of structure and<br />

predictable routines (Bailey & Wolery, 1992; Boswell & Gray, 2003; Trillingsgaard, 1999),<br />

(2) the use of visual cues (Boswell & Gray, 2003; Bryan & Gast, 2000; Fanjul & Ball, 1995;<br />

Quill, 2000; Wheeler, 1998), and (3) songs (Gottscheweski, 2001; Williams, 1996).<br />

Additional guidelines and methods, especially those used for toilet training, are described in<br />

further detail below.<br />

Research and clinical applications show that songs can transmit and be used to assist<br />

children with memorization of a sequence of information (Aasgaard, 1999; Enoch, 2001;<br />

Gervin, 1991; Gfeller, 1983; Jellison & Miller, 1982; McGuire, 2001; Wolfe & Horn, 1993),<br />

and to teach them to manage self-help skills (Michel, 1976; Nordoff & Robbins, 1992).<br />

121


122<br />

Therefore, using songs to cue or prompt a sequence might be a successful way for young<br />

children with autism to accomplish everyday skills. Donna Williams, herself diagnosed as<br />

having autism as a child, used songs to accomplish daily life tasks involving a sequence of<br />

steps. She suggests to use music in the following way: “If someone listens, for example, to<br />

the song Insy-Winsy-Spider, you can use this tune and write some of your words to the<br />

chords of this song and sing simple lessons. The lessons can be anything you wanted to<br />

teach from the different steps involved in going to the toilet to getting out a snack to eat or<br />

having a bath. Things like washing hands, order of dressing, emotional expressions and how<br />

they feel, whatever involves a simple lesson or sequence, can be put to music. Verses can be<br />

added later as the songs are picked up and gradually lessons can be expanded upon”<br />

(Williams, 1996, p. 298).<br />

Hand-washing. Teaching proper hand-washing techniques is part of the National<br />

Health Education Standards and therefore part of the preschool health and hygiene<br />

curriculum (Fanjul & Ball, 1995; Joint Commission on Health Education Standards, 1995;<br />

Squibb & Yardley, 1999). The rationales for hand-washing are to preserve the child’s<br />

personal health, and to promote social acceptance and inclusion by encouraging good<br />

hygiene. Within the daily classroom routine, hand-washing occurs at five key times: before<br />

and after meals, after outdoor play, after sneezing or coughing, after playing with pets, and<br />

after toileting (Education <strong>World</strong>, 2003; Geiger et al., 2000; Rumfelt-Wright, 2001). Children<br />

with autism frequently have difficulty in managing the multiple steps involved in hand-<br />

washing. The characteristics identifying children as having autism might interfere with the<br />

hand-washing strategies used with typically developing children. Some of the issues


discussed in the toilet training literature for children with autism, but not yet discussed for<br />

hand-washing, might also apply:<br />

• Limited communication skills might interfere with understanding and associating<br />

words, signs or pictures/symbols with the idea of hand-washing.<br />

• Difficulty in organizing and sequencing information might interfere with the<br />

navigation of multiple steps required in hand-washing.<br />

• Sensitivity and unusual reactions to various sensory stimuli, such as smells, texture,<br />

color, temperature or sound, might interfere with the hand-washing process (i.e., fear<br />

of or over-interest in running water, a scented soap, water temperature, etc.).<br />

These issues might contribute to feelings of frustration or anxiety, and be expressed in<br />

inappropriate behavior (Boswell & Gray, 2003; Heffner, 2002; Siegel, 1998, Wheeler, 1998).<br />

Some attention has been given to the steps involved in proper hand-washing<br />

techniques for preschoolers (Rumfelt-Wright, 2001; Geiger et al., 2000; Fanjul & Ball,<br />

1995). Guidelines have been proposed on learning strategies such as singing songs, playing<br />

games, stories, and hands-on practice (Enoch, 2001; Geiger et al., 2000; Spear, 1998). Quite<br />

a few commercial instructional products (videos, recorded music, and illustrated books) are<br />

available on hand-washing (e.g., The American Red Cross, 1996; Ross, 2000; Soap and<br />

Detergent Association, 1990). A large body of research focuses on infection control and<br />

hand-washing habits (Education <strong>World</strong>, 2003). Few research studies investigated hand-<br />

washing strategies for children with special needs (Quenville, 1980). A study by Kramer<br />

(1978) investigated the effects of a unique song in maintaining hand-washing skills in<br />

preschool children with and without special needs (learning disabilities, Down Syndrome,<br />

123


124<br />

and cerebral palsy). After 10 training sessions in which the song was used, one group<br />

continued to wash their hands without any instruction, the second with music only, the third<br />

with verbal instruction, and the fourth listened to the entire song. Results indicate the entire<br />

song and the verbal instruction proved to be equally effective in maintaining the hand-<br />

washing procedure, whereas no instruction and music alone did not maintain the skills.<br />

Kramer suggested that using songs for repeating daily routines might be more pleasant for<br />

children and staff than giving verbal reminders. However, no studies are available that<br />

compare the effectiveness of strategies used for learning a proper hand-washing procedure<br />

with young children with autism within the context of an inclusive childcare program.<br />

Toilet Training. Besides making the first step and speaking the first word,<br />

independent toileting is a major developmental achievement. The rationales for being toilet<br />

trained are at least three fold: (1) it allows the child more freedom and independence to fully<br />

participate in community activities; (2) incontinence and poor toileting hygiene can<br />

significantly interfere with social acceptance; and (3) it opens options for childcare, school<br />

programs or future residential programs (Bailey & Wolery, 1992; Cicero & Pfadt, 2002;<br />

Gallender, 1980; Heffner, 2002; Wheeler, 1998). Children with autism frequently have<br />

difficulty in managing self-care in toileting and are often confused by the sequencing actions<br />

involved (Dalrymple & Boarman, 1992; Wheeler, 1998). The characteristics identifying<br />

children as having autism might interfere with toilet-training techniques practiced with<br />

typically developing children or children with other disabilities.


• The social interaction deficits might interfere with closeness or imitation involved in<br />

toilet training. Understanding and enjoying reciprocal social relationships and social<br />

reinforcers (e.g., “big boy”) might mean very little or nothing to a child with autism.<br />

• Limited communication skills might interfere with understanding and associating<br />

words, signs or pictures/symbols with the idea of toileting. Children with autism also<br />

might have difficulties expressing their urge or need to urinate or defecate.<br />

• Difficulty in organizing and sequencing information might interfere with the<br />

following of the multiple steps required in toileting.<br />

• Preference for predictable routines and rituals, sameness and restricted interests might<br />

interfere with establishing a new routine (toileting). Although, when toileting is part<br />

of the routine, the insistence on routines might be beneficial for the process.<br />

• Sensitivity and unusual reactions to various sensory stimuli such as smells, texture,<br />

color, temperature or sound might interfere with the toileting process (i.e., fear of or<br />

over-interest in, flushing the toilet is frequently observed in children with autism; a<br />

fluffy rug, a cold tile, or colors in a bathroom might cause over-stimulation or fear).<br />

• Medical conditions might interfere with toileting and need to be identified and ruled<br />

out before toilet training.<br />

General guidelines have been proposed for teaching toileting skills to children with<br />

special needs. The first step in teaching toileting skills is to assess the child’s readiness,<br />

defined as follows: Bladder control for a certain period of time, awareness of both the urge<br />

and the need to urinate or defecate (i.e., desire to remove wet diaper) an ability to sit on the<br />

toilet for approximately five minutes; an ability to imitate actions and follow simple<br />

125


126<br />

directions, skills to communicate needs, basic dressing skills, and respond to positive<br />

reinforcement; and the desire of parents to implement the toileting procedure (Baker &<br />

Brightman, 1997; Bailey & Wolery, 1992; Gallender, 1980; Heffner, 2002; Siegel, 1998).<br />

Furthermore, it is suggested to begin with daytime training rather than nighttime training,<br />

bladder training before bowel training, initiate training in school and at home at the same<br />

time, teach children to communicate their need to urinate/defecate, and teach each steps<br />

involved in toilet training (Bailey & Wolery, 1992, Siegel, 1998).<br />

toileting skills:<br />

There are at least three established methods for teaching children with disabilities<br />

(1) the Scheduled Method, meaning children are taken to the toilet at regular intervals or near<br />

their natural elimination cycles (Bailey & Wolery, 1992; Siegel, 1998).<br />

(2) the Distributed Practice/Improved Schedule Procedure, where baseline data on the<br />

child’s elimination pattern is collected, analyzed and implemented at two identified times<br />

using positive reinforcers. Over time, toileting times are expanded and the need to<br />

urinate /defecate is communicated by the child (Fredericks et al., 1975 as cited in Bailey<br />

& Wolery, 1992).<br />

(3) the Massed Practice/Rapid Method, where self-initiated, independent toileting is<br />

established within a short period of time by increasing the frequency of urinations<br />

(through giving liquid), seating the child on the toilet for a period of time, positive<br />

reinforcement immediately after urination and for staying dry when off the toilet, using<br />

graduated guidance for prompting the child through the single steps of toileting, and<br />

finally a self-initiated toileting by the child while decreasing prompts for the whole


procedure. For accidents, verbal reprimands, positive practice, opportunity to feel the<br />

wet pants, and restitutional overcorrection are used. A urine alarm device is frequently<br />

used with this method (Azrin & Foxx, 1971). Several authors have developed and<br />

studied variations and adapted methods of the rapid method (e.g., Smith, 1979; Ando,<br />

1977).<br />

That said, teaching toilet training skills to children with autism might require different<br />

strategies, considering their broader difficulties. Toilet training needs to be a unique<br />

approach, integrating the individual’s differences. Because the toilet training is a complex<br />

task for children with autism, the starting point might consist of establishing a routine,<br />

including all steps involved in toileting, before expecting the child to use the toilet<br />

independently. In this way, a bathroom routine will be in place when the child with autism is<br />

ready for self-initiated toileting. Boswell & Gray (2003) suggest structured teaching<br />

principles to toilet training children with autism. This technique includes: (a) assessment of<br />

the child’s skills and readiness for scheduled training or for independent toileting; (b)<br />

determining realistic, individual goals and a starting point; (c) task analysis and determining<br />

each steps of the toilet routine; (d) establishing a positive and meaningful toileting routine<br />

that incorporates setting up the physical environment (bathroom) and the communication to<br />

the child (i.e., verbal and visual supported routines using transition objects and pictures for<br />

each step of the routine), including where to go, what to do, and where to go afterwards; (e)<br />

troubleshooting specific problems and making adjustments; and (f) a reward system for<br />

successful accomplishment of tasks. Learning to manage the toileting skills independent<br />

might require several months for children with autism.<br />

127


128<br />

Quite a few commercial instructional products (i.e., video tapes, recorded music, and<br />

illustrated books) are available on general toilet training (Cole & Miller, 1989; Frankel,<br />

1999; Rogers & Judkins, 1997). A small number of research studies address issues related to<br />

toilet training explicitly for children with autism. Two recent studies come from Bainbridge<br />

and Myles (1999) and Cicero and Pfadt (2001). Bainbridge & Myles investigate the use of<br />

priming (Video tape: “It’s potty time,” Howard, 1991), to introduce toilet training to children<br />

with autism. The results indicate an increase in initiation of toileting use and a decrease in<br />

wet diapers when priming was used. Cicero and Pfadt (2001) study the effectiveness of an<br />

adapted version of the rapid method. Their procedure employs a combination of positive<br />

reinforcement, graduated guidance, scheduled training and forward prompting, and<br />

elimination of the restitutional overcorrection. Results indicate that this procedure is a rapid<br />

method of toilet training with generalization to different environments. No study has yet<br />

emerged that uses song as a strategy for following the multiple steps involved in toileting<br />

with young children with autism.<br />

Cleaning-up. Cleaning-up toys and materials is a task requested many times within<br />

the childcare day: after free play periods (e.g., toys), after meals (e.g., plates and cups), after<br />

circle time (i.e., clean up sitting carpets), and after other classroom activities. The rationale<br />

for cleaning-up is at least fourfold: (1) to keep the classroom organized and functional; (2) to<br />

signal the transition to the next activity; (3) to communicate concepts of personal<br />

responsibility; and (4) to promote social skills such as helping skills. Especially for children<br />

with autism, a clearly structured environment is necessary so that the child can function most<br />

independently (Dawson & Osterling, 1997; Marcus et al., 2001; Schopler, et al., 1971).


Children with autism frequently have difficulty in understanding and managing the multiple<br />

steps involved in cleaning-up. The inability to understand the demands of this classroom<br />

routine might cause frustration and confusion, expressed in problem behaviors.<br />

Alger (1984) proposed guidelines on how to signal and structure cleaning-up time,<br />

and quite a few commercial products (recorded music and illustrated books) are available on<br />

cleaning-up (e.g., Anderson, 2002; Bridwell, 1997; Wallance, 2001; Barney & Friends, 1992;<br />

Preschool Education, 2003). Most of the clean-up songs currently employ the<br />

“piggybacking” technique, meaning words are put to a familiar tune. A pre-composed song<br />

frequently used and also suggested by Furman (2001) for cleaning-up with children with<br />

autism is “Clean up!” by Barney & Friends (Barney’s Favorites Vol. 1, 1992). No study has<br />

been found investigating cleaning-up strategies and behaviors of children with and without<br />

disabilities in preschool settings. More specifically, no study has been conducted that<br />

compares the effectiveness of strategies used for the cleaning-up task with young children<br />

with autism in the context of an inclusive childcare program.<br />

The purpose of this study was to evaluate the effectiveness of songs versus lyrics in<br />

increasing the independent performance of a boy with autism, during hand-washing,<br />

toileting, and cleaning-up. These multiple-step tasks were chosen because the target child<br />

needed to expand his skills in these areas. Preliminary observation indicated that the target<br />

child was very responsive to music. When sung to he transitioned and followed parts of<br />

multiple-step tasks, whereas in the absence of the song, he demonstrated inappropriate<br />

behaviors.<br />

129


130<br />

5.2 Research Questions<br />

One purpose of this study was to evaluate the effectiveness of songs as structural<br />

prompts that increase independent performance for a young child with autism during<br />

multiple-step tasks within classroom routines, and specifically, to evaluate whether the<br />

musical presentation of the sequence is more effective than its verbal counterpart. Another<br />

purpose was to determine whether the classroom teacher can learn basic principles of music<br />

therapy and implement a song intervention. The specific research questions were as follows:<br />

(1) Does the use of songs increase independent performance for a young child with<br />

autism during multiple-step tasks?<br />

(2) Is the use of songs more effective in improving skills to manage multiple-step<br />

tasks than the verbal presentation of the sequence?<br />

(3) Can a classroom teacher learn basic principles of music therapy, and as a result,<br />

5.3 Method<br />

implement song interventions in classroom routines?<br />

5.3.1 Participants<br />

The participant in this study was one young child with autism (n=1), and his<br />

classroom teacher (n=1).<br />

Target Child. Andy was a 3-year, 2-month-old European-<br />

American, enrolled in the childcare program for eight months<br />

prior this study. An external agency had diagnosed him with<br />

ASD using the diagnostic tools describe in chapter 3. The child


was selected for participation in this study on the suggestion of his classroom teacher and<br />

parents based on his diagnosis of ASD, tremendous interest in, and positive response to,<br />

singing, and his difficulties with multiple-step tasks such as hand-washing, cleaning-up<br />

and toileting. Andy was a lively and humorous child with strengths in early academic<br />

concepts. However, he was functioning on the Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS)<br />

in the mild to moderate range on the autism spectrum. Andy’s communication was<br />

limited and he demonstrated a lack of interest in others. Sometimes he exhibited self-<br />

stimulating behaviors such as hand flapping and he needed assistance with managing<br />

several daily activities. Andy used the Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS)<br />

(Bondy & Frost, 1994) to communicate during mealtimes, and simple objects were used<br />

as visual cues (e.g., a bottle of soap to cue hand-washing) for transitions in the daily<br />

classroom routines. Andy was able to say quite a few words to label objects when they<br />

were in sight or in direct imitation of an adult. When he could not get his needs<br />

expressed, he became frustrated easily and exhibited behaviors such as whining, fussing,<br />

jumping up and down, or pulling on adults. Andy also had some difficulty attending to<br />

language unless it involved a familiar song or physical routine. When enjoying a game or<br />

activity, Andy accepted other children playing with him or nearby. He watched his peers<br />

and sometimes imitated their play. Often, though he liked to play on his own with his<br />

preferred toys (puzzle, magna doodle, books, shape sorters, peg boards).<br />

Andy’s favorite things were to identify letters, numbers, shapes, and listen to music.<br />

He knew the alphabet, was able to recognize all the uppercase letters, and he had already<br />

identified numbers before age two years. Andy received speech/language and occupational<br />

therapy services while in the classroom, and weekly external speech language services.<br />

131


132<br />

Singing was employed by his teachers and therapists as a motivator to use words and<br />

actions when he wanted to repeat an activity he enjoyed. For example, he had to say<br />

“wheels” to ask for a repetition of the song ”The wheels on the bus go round and round”<br />

(Traditional). To improve his language skills, songs like “Swing low, sweet chariot”<br />

(American traditional) were used to work on sounds, imitation, and direct language<br />

production during pauses in singing. Activity songs such as “Ring around the rosy”<br />

(Traditional) and “Row, row, row your boat” (Traditional) were used to create opportunities<br />

to interact with peers. Andy learned songs sung in school quickly, and repeated them at<br />

home with gestures. He seemed to love to sing, listen and dance to music.<br />

His classroom teacher described him in the following way: ”Andy really responds<br />

well to music. He makes eye contact with me as soon as I start a melody. Singing songs<br />

with him during major transitions in the classroom helps him to understand what to do next.”<br />

She also noticed that Andy would stiffen his legs and body, flap his arms, whine, try to<br />

escape and avoid transition if she just used words in the same situation. Andy’s mother<br />

reported that the family uses music with Andy for nap and nighttime, for long trips in the car<br />

and when he seems to be irritable. She observed that music calms him, and redirects him<br />

from unwanted behavior.<br />

Andy tended to have difficulties with transitioning and managing multiple-step tasks<br />

during classroom routines. With music, Andy transitioned smoothly to the next activity, but<br />

could not manage the required steps of routines such as hand-washing, toileting, and<br />

cleaning-up independently. These three multiple-step tasks were chosen for an intervention,


ecause of the teacher’s and parent’s opinion and interest in improving the target child’s<br />

hand-washing skills, his readiness to begin toilet training, and increasing his independent<br />

performance during the classroom’s cleaning-up routine.<br />

At the time of the study, washing hands throughout the day was an already familiar<br />

routine for Andy. He transitioned to the sink easily when singing to him, but was not able to<br />

follow the sequence of hand-washing independently. He was able to rub his hands under the<br />

running water when prompted by his teacher. All other steps were performed by the teacher<br />

or by using the hand-over-hand technique. The classroom teacher used the familiar song<br />

“Row your boat” (American traditional) and changed the words to “Wash, wash, wash you<br />

hands, gently in the sink, scrubbed them, scrubbed them, scrubbed them, scrubbed them, ‘til<br />

they’re nice and clean” to motivate Andy to rub his hands under the running water.<br />

Andy had started toilet training four weeks before the study. His teacher started to<br />

prompt him to transition to the bathroom on a scheduled time. No task analysis of the steps<br />

of toileting had been determined yet. For transitioning to the bathroom, the teacher<br />

experimented with a familiar tune and added novel words saying, “Let’s go to the potty,” and<br />

used a diaper as transition object. Andy wore diapers and had urination accidents on a daily<br />

basis. He met some of the prerequisite skills for toilet training, including the ability to sit for<br />

a few minutes, to imitate, and to follow simple directions. What is more, he responded<br />

favorably to music. His dressing skills were very low. His mother was requesting a “potty<br />

song,” to accomplish the new task. A toilet training video titled “It’s potty time” (Howard,<br />

133


134<br />

1991) was given to her prior the study. No sophisticated song following the single steps of<br />

toileting was found.<br />

At the time of the study, cleaning-up at different times throughout the childcare day<br />

was also a readily familiar routine for Andy. He started to clean up his toys / materials, when<br />

the already familiar, pre-composed cleaning-up song was sung to him. If the teacher did not<br />

sing, he did not engage in cleaning-up, or he demonstrated inappropriate behaviors when<br />

assisted to complete the task.<br />

Classroom Teacher. Andy’s lead teacher participated in this study. She had<br />

bachelor’s degree, was certified in Early Childhood Education, and had taught young<br />

children for 3 years. She had also participated in a continuity of care study for three years,<br />

meaning all children, except Andy, had been in her classroom since infancy. The teacher had<br />

no prior experience with music therapy interventions. She participated in the study based on<br />

her schedule and classroom activities (i.e., teachers’ start times are staggered so that the<br />

entire childcare day has coverage).<br />

Classroom Peers. In addition to the target child, there were six other children who<br />

were enrolled in the classroom. The classroom peers were between two and three years of<br />

age. Peers included both males and females from different ethnic groups. Four of the<br />

children were typically developing and one of the classroom peers had Pfeiffers Syndrome.<br />

The classroom peers were not part of the study, but participated in the described multiple-<br />

step tasks simultaneously as part of the classroom routine.


5.3.2 Setting<br />

All experimental sessions took place in the target child’s classroom, where the<br />

curriculum philosophy followed the same Developmentally Appropriate Practice (DAP)<br />

(Bredekamp & Copple, 1997) as in Experiment I. Andy’s classroom was divided into in<br />

different play areas and contained corresponding toys and materials (Harms, Clifford, & Cryer,<br />

1998; Marcus et al., 2001) similar to Experiment I. There was a food counter including a sink<br />

with a soap dispenser and a paper towel holder. A stepping stool was placed before the sink for<br />

the children to reach the sink and tap. A huge garbage can was located next to the sink. A<br />

bathroom designed for children was accessible from the classroom. It contained a small step<br />

(made out of two duct-taped phone books) to reach the toilet sit more easily, a sink, a mirror, a<br />

garbage can, and a chair for teachers.<br />

Hand-washing occurred several times during the childcare day: before and after<br />

meals, after messy activities (i.e., painting at the easel or outdoor play), after sneezing or<br />

coughing, and after toileting. Before breakfast at 9:15 am, all children were prompted to<br />

wash their hands. Each child went to the sink and waited for his or her turn to wash his or<br />

her hands. The child climbed up the stepping stool, turned the water on by manipulating the<br />

handle, got soap from the soap suspender, rubbed her/his hands against each other, rinsed the<br />

soap off under the running water, and turned the water off, dried her/his hands by using a<br />

paper towel, which was thrown in the garbage can afterwards. A sequence of pictures<br />

showing the multiple steps of hand-washing was attached to the wall behind the water tap<br />

and visible to all children. After finishing the hand-washing procedure the child climbed<br />

135


136<br />

down the stepping stool, went to the breakfast table and sat down in her/his place, marked by<br />

a picture of him or herself.<br />

All of the children in Andy’s classroom went to toilet on scheduled times throughout<br />

the day and whenever necessary. Every child went to the bathroom after breakfast.<br />

Depending on the child’s self-care skills, support was offered by the teachers to successfully<br />

manage the multiple-steps surrounding toileting. The procedure used for toilet training was<br />

to undress, sit on the toilet, urinate, wipe, dress, flush, and wash hands. For children with<br />

special needs part of the toilet training was to get used to the routine more generally and to<br />

every single step involved in the task, even when a diaper was still needed throughout the<br />

day.<br />

The classroom curriculum allowed free play after breakfast and before circle time.<br />

Children engaged in different play areas by themselves or with others. To cue the children to<br />

finish their play activity and transition to circle time, a song for cleaning- up toys and<br />

materials was used. Each child was expected to put the toys and material they used back in<br />

the designated play area independently. The song was sung as long as it took to clean up.<br />

The daily classroom schedule and predictable routines were set in place to allow the<br />

target child to function more independently during classroom routines (Marcus et al., 2001;<br />

Schopler et al., 1971). As in Experiment I, students, observers and visitors were occasionally<br />

in the classroom or observing behind the one-way mirror.


5.3.3 Materials<br />

Andy was assessed on his ability to accomplish the sequence of steps required for<br />

each task. Each song used for each task was adapted to his skills, needs and learning goals.<br />

The already pre-existing routines and songs used by the teachers within the classroom were<br />

respected and integrated into the intervention. Therefore, a combination of transition and<br />

multiple-step tasks, and familiar and novel songs were used.<br />

The exact material used for this study was as follows: (1) three different transition<br />

objects to initiate each multiple-step task; (2) a familiar, pre-composed song with<br />

individualized lyrics for hand-washing; (3) an individually composed song for toilet training;<br />

and (4) a familiar, pre-composed song for clean up. Each is described in greater detail<br />

below.<br />

(1) Transition objects. For hand-washing, a bottle of soap was used to cue Andy to<br />

transition to the sink. Additionally, pictures showing seven steps of hand-washing, published<br />

by the North Carolina Department of Human Resources, Division of Child Development<br />

(Fanjul & Ball, 1995) were attached to the wall behind the sink. For toilet training, a diaper<br />

was used to cue Andy to transition to go to the potty. For cleaning-up, the toy in use by<br />

Andy functioned as a cue.<br />

(2) Familiar, pre-composed song with individualized lyrics for hand-washing (Task<br />

#1). The pre-composed song “Row your boat” (Traditional) was used for hand-washing,<br />

because the duration of one verse matched the suggested time of 10 to 15 seconds necessary<br />

137


138<br />

to kill germs. The classroom teacher applied this song for rubbing Andy’s hands by using<br />

the “Piggybacking” technique; meaning words of a song are altered to contain the desired<br />

information (Brownell, 2002). For the intervention, the following seven steps were described<br />

within the lyrics of the song: (1) Turn the water on, (2) Wet your hands, (3) Get the soap, (4)<br />

Wash your hands, (5) Rinse your hands, (6) Turn water off, and (7) Dry your hands. Audio<br />

Excerpt 5.1 gives an example of the song and altered lyrics, sung by Andy’s lead teacher.<br />

Audio Excerpt 5.1. Recording of the first part of the hand-washing song, sung by<br />

Andy’s classroom teacher.<br />

(3) Individually composed song for toilet training (Task #2). For toilet training, a<br />

unique song was written, matching the target child’s temperament and conveying the demands of<br />

the multiple steps for Andy’s toilet training. The intention of the song was to ease the transition<br />

to the bathroom, and to increase independent performance during toileting. The toileting routine<br />

was developed in collaboration with the classroom teacher, and reflected an adapted version of<br />

the procedure used with the target child’s classmates. The lyrics of the song focused on each<br />

step of the routine and were composed as follow:<br />

1. Get up (get up from play; diaper uses as cue to transition to the bathroom)<br />

2. Walk to the toilet<br />

3. Climb (climb up a step to the toilet)<br />

4. Pants down (lower the pants)<br />

5. Sit down (sit down on toilet)<br />

6. Pee (urinate)


7. Get up (get up)<br />

8. T-Shirt up (participate in re-dressing, while teacher wipes him off and puts the<br />

diaper on)<br />

9. Pants up (re-dress)<br />

10. Flush (flush the toilet)<br />

The ten described steps were presented in the unique song “Let’s go potty,” as<br />

outlined in Figure 5.1. The verses of the song reflect each single step of the routine in the<br />

order of the toileting procedure. The refrain emphasized the major steps of toileting (go to<br />

toilet, urinate and flush) and was repeated when more time was needed to accomplish the<br />

step. Since music was one of Andy’s favorite things, the song itself functioned as a<br />

motivator and a reinforcer. The musical style used for the refrain (swing) and the<br />

accompanying activity (hand clapping) provoked positive effects on the target child’s<br />

behavior in previous situations. The statement in the lyrics “Andy knows how,” reflects the<br />

teacher’s belief in the child’s ability to accomplish the task. The Latin style used for the<br />

verses stands in contrast to the refrain, and gives the piece a certain drive that reflects a<br />

somewhat unusual way to sing about toilet procedures. The toilet training song “Let’s go<br />

potty,” was recorded on a CD with the classroom teacher on vocals. Additionally, a song<br />

transcription was handed out to both the teacher and to parents.<br />

139<br />

Audio Excerpt 5.2. Recording of the song “Let’s go potty,” featuring the classroom<br />

teacher on vocals.


140<br />

Figure 5.1. Transcript of the song ”Let’s go potty,” written for Andy, addressing the<br />

demands of his toileting routine while in the classroom.


(3) Familiar, pre-composed song for clean up (Task #3). The pre-<br />

composed song “Clean-up!” by Barney & Friends (Barney’s Favorites<br />

Vol. 1, 1992) was used for cleaning-up toys and material after play<br />

activities. Barney, the purple dinosaur in the picture is a children’s<br />

character, broadcasted by U.S. public television. The “Clean up!”<br />

song is compiled from the Barney & Friends series. This song was already an implemented<br />

classroom routine for cleaning-up prior the study. The lyrics of the song read:<br />

“Clean up, clean up! Everybody everywhere!<br />

Clean up, clean up! Everybody do your share!”<br />

141<br />

The lyrics of the song do not describe the steps of cleaning-up or define how many things<br />

need to be put away. In the already established classroom routine, children were expected to<br />

get up from play or an activity, pick up their toys or material, put them away and go where<br />

directed by teachers.<br />

5.3.4 Procedure<br />

Audio Excerpt 5.3. Recording of the first part of the cleaning-up song, sung by the<br />

children of the FPG childcare program.<br />

Experimental sessions occurred Monday to Friday, one time daily for each multiple-<br />

step task. Hand-washing occurred before breakfast at 9:15 am, toilet training after breakfast


142<br />

at 9:30 am, and clean up before circle time at 9:55 am. As elsewhere in this inquiry, the<br />

observation was based on the subject’s presence and the availability of the classroom teacher.<br />

Staff Development Activities. The goals and procedures of the intervention were<br />

identified and discussed with the classroom teacher. The unique song for toilet training was<br />

developed and recorded in collaboration with the classroom teacher. The CD and transcribed<br />

music was given to the teacher and parents. Specific instructions regarding how to involve<br />

and prompt the child during the intervention was discussed with and demonstrated to the<br />

teacher prior to the study. It involved the following topics:<br />

• how to cue the child with different objects to transition to the multiple-step task<br />

by walking toward the child approach his physical level and show him the object;<br />

• how to sing the songs or speak the words during the multiple-step tasks by using<br />

an engaging voice and facial expression, and looking at the child;<br />

• how to prompt the target child, and how much support to give by singing or<br />

saying each step, pointing to the next step, then using the hand-over-hand<br />

technique; and<br />

• how to follow the target child’s tempo and give him enough time to respond to<br />

each step by pausing or slowing down in the music.<br />

During the intervention, ongoing collaborative consultation was provided.<br />

Song Procedure. In the song procedure (Condition A), each particular song for the<br />

three selected multiple-step tasks was sung to the subject by the classroom teacher.


Task #1: Hand-washing. The already established and familiar routine for hand-<br />

washing was kept and extended. As prior to the study, the subject was prompted with a<br />

bottle of soap, accompanied by his teacher singing to him “ wash, wash, wash your hands,<br />

wash your hands right now, lalala lalala lalala la, wash your hands right now,” as<br />

demonstrated in Audio Excerpt 5.1. The subject transitioned to hand-washing by walking to<br />

the sink. The experimental session started as soon as the subject climbed up the stepping<br />

stool to reach the sink and was ready to manipulate the handle to turn the water on. The<br />

teacher started to sing about the first step of the hand-washing procedure which was: “Turn,<br />

turn, turn it on, turn the water on, lalala lalala lalala la, turn the water on.” As the song<br />

proceeded, the teacher sang about the next steps of hand-washing, which was wet one’s<br />

hands (step 2), get the soap (step 3), wash one’s hands (step 4), rinse the soap off (step 5),<br />

turn the water off (step 6) and dry one’s hands (step7). For step 4, the words remained the<br />

same as in the routine established prior to the study.<br />

Task #2: Toilet training. The teachers and parents used the scheduled method and<br />

structured teaching principles to toilet-train Andy. The song “Let’s go potty” was used as a<br />

structural prompt to signal each step of the toileting routine. Since task #2 was a fairly new<br />

to Andy, and since an established routine had been never introduced to him before, the<br />

transition to the bathroom was included in the intervention. The experimental session started<br />

as soon as the subject was prompted with a diaper accompanied by his teacher singing to him<br />

“(Teacher’s name) brings the diaper, what does this mean …” as demonstrated in Audio<br />

Excerpt 5.2. The subject got up (step 1) and transitioned to the bathroom (step 2). Thus, as<br />

the song proceeded, so too did the further steps of the toileting routine. The next steps of<br />

143


144<br />

toileting routine were climb up the step (step 3), un-dress (step 4), sit down on toilet (step 5),<br />

urinate (step 6), get up from toilet bowl (step 7), assist teacher by holding shirt up while she<br />

wipes him off and puts his diaper on (step 8), dress up (step 9), and flush toilet (step 10).<br />

Task #3: Cleaning-up. The already-established and familiar routine for clean up was<br />

kept. The subject was prompted with the toy or material in use for free play (e.g., bear cubs)<br />

to start to clean up, accompanied by his teachers and class room peers singing the “Clean up”<br />

song (Barney & Friends, 1992). The experimental session started as soon as the teachers<br />

started the song. While singing the song, the subject was expected to manage the following<br />

six steps: Get up and get ready to clean up (step 1), pick up toy/material (step 2), put it away<br />

(step 3), pick up a toy/material (step 4), put it away (step 5), and go where directed by<br />

teacher (step 6).<br />

Lyric Procedure. In the lyric procedure (Condition B), the conditions were similar to<br />

the baseline conditions. The same steps for each task were used. The only change between<br />

the song procedure and the lyric procedure was the withdrawal of the music. Each single<br />

step was verbally spoken twice for each step of the routines.<br />

5.3.5 Experimental Design<br />

An alternating treatment design (ATD), replicated across three tasks, was used to<br />

evaluate the effectiveness of two different interventions (songs versus spoken word) in a<br />

single subject. In an alternating treatment design, two or more interventions rapidly alternate<br />

in a systematic order. The number of observation days for each intervention should occur


equally. The different effects of the interventions can be observed by comparing the<br />

subject’s performance under each of the conditions. If a consistent difference in the trend<br />

and/or level of data occurs, superiority of one treatment over another is demonstrated. The<br />

advantage of this single-subject research design is the rapid comparison of the efficacy<br />

between two interventions (Alberto & Troutman, 1995; Barlow & Hayes, 1979; Holcombe,<br />

Wolery & Gast, 1994; Tawney & Gast, 1984 ). Because the child was leaving the childcare<br />

program two months from the initiation of the study, this design allowed collecting data and<br />

evaluating effects quickly.<br />

In Experiment II, a Song intervention (Condition A) was compared to a Lyric<br />

intervention (Condition B). The Conditions A (songs) and Condition B (lyrics) altered day<br />

by day and were replicated over three tasks. The alternation of the treatment order replicated<br />

across tasks was implemented to minimize any learning or order effects associated with<br />

receiving the song intervention (Condition A) or lyric intervention (Condition B) first. The<br />

treatment order for each task was as follow: Task #1 (hand-washing) A-B, Task #2 (toilet<br />

training) B-A, and Task #3 (clean up) A-B. The Song Procedure (Conditions A), followed<br />

the demands of the three selected multiple-step tasks by using songs. The Lyric Intervention<br />

(Condition B), followed the demands of the three selected multiple-step tasks by using words<br />

only. Data collection occurred over two months.<br />

5.3.6 Measurements<br />

In Experiment II, several measurement procedures and response definitions applied.<br />

Essentially six categories of behaviors for each multiple step task were measured through<br />

145


146<br />

direct observation using event recording. That is, data were collected as the target child<br />

performed each step of each task (hand-washing, toilet training, cleaning-up). Data were<br />

recorded during Condition A and B (song intervention, lyric intervention) when the subject<br />

arrived to school before the transition to breakfast has started; and the classroom teacher was<br />

available.<br />

Data were collected by direct observation once a day on hand-washing before<br />

breakfast, toilet training after breakfast, and clean up before the morning circle time. The<br />

observation time started for Task #1 (hand-washing) as soon as the target child was ready to<br />

manipulate the handle to turn the water on and his teacher started to sing or said “turn the<br />

water on.” For Task #2 (toilet training) the observation time started as soon as the subject<br />

was prompted with a diaper to transition to the bathroom and the teacher sang the song or<br />

said ”Get up.” For Task #3 (cleaning-up) the observation time started as soon as the teacher<br />

sang the clean up song or said “clean up.” Data collection ended as soon as the teacher went<br />

through all the steps of each task. Some of the sessions were videotaped with a Panasonic<br />

AG-195 Camcorder for reliability purposes, then analyzed afterwards. The event recording<br />

was employed based on the observers judgment about the target child’s behaviors in each<br />

step of each task, and on the results the observer recorded for each step and each task on a<br />

specially designed data sheet.<br />

The definition of the categories for each task were as follows:<br />

• Did it was recorded when the subject performed the action by himself. His<br />

teacher did not need to (or did not) provide any support that helped the subject to<br />

perform the target behavior. It was an independent performance of the child.


Appendix B.<br />

• Did not do it was recorded if the subject did not perform the correct step or<br />

needed to be prompted more than once.<br />

• Did it with a prompt was recorded if the subject responded correctly, but only<br />

after receiving a physical prompt (i.e., the teacher put her hand on the subject’s<br />

elbow in order to facilitate a step) to do so. Visual prompts (e.g., soap bottle,<br />

diaper and toy/material) were not recorded as a prompt. After the first time<br />

through the song, or after two times saying the step, a prompt could be made. If<br />

the subject needed to be prompted more than one time, it was evaluated, as “Did<br />

not do it.”<br />

• Negative Verbalization<br />

o Yes was recorded if the subject displayed any verbal behavior that was not<br />

appropriate. This included screaming or whining.<br />

o No was recorded, if the subject displayed appropriate verbal behavior.<br />

• Escapes were recorded if the subject displayed any physical behavior that was not<br />

acceptable. This included running away, flopping to the floor, or pulling away.<br />

• The number of times he did any of the negative or positive verbalizations was<br />

counted.<br />

• Skipped the part was recorded, if the subject or his teacher skipped a step (e.g. the<br />

subject was already “up” or “go where directed by teacher” was not requested).<br />

These categories were coded for each of the steps of each multiple-step task.<br />

Additionally, field notes were recorded on the bottom of the data sheet, as shown in<br />

147


148<br />

5.3.7 Reliability<br />

Interobserver agreement data were collected on the subject’s performance by an<br />

educational specialist consultant, and by the investigator, for each multiple-step task and<br />

during both conditions. Agreement between the scores from the two reliability observers was<br />

calculated prior to the reliability data collection phase by using videotaped segments from<br />

previously evaluated sessions.<br />

Reliability checks were carried out on an average of 44.7% of total observations<br />

within each phase and for each multiple step task. Two coders coded videotaped segments<br />

for each target behavior. Interobserver agreement levels for each code category were<br />

calculated by summing the number of agreements (defined as the same coding interval) and<br />

dividing the number by sum of agreements plus disagreements and multiplying by 100.<br />

5.4 Results<br />

5.4.1 Interobserver Agreement<br />

Interobserver agreement data on the subject’s performance were collected in 44.7%<br />

for all multiple-step tasks. Reliability data showed that observers agreed on the participant’s<br />

hand-washing performance in 98.5% (range: 92.8% - 100%) of the trials, on the toilet<br />

training performance 93.8% (range: 85% - 100%) of the trials, and on the cleaning-up<br />

performance 94.8% (range: 79.2% - 100%) of the trials. The overall interobserver agreement<br />

was 96% (range: 79.2% - 100%) of the trials. Table 5.1 gives a summary of the interobserver<br />

agreement.


Table 5.1. Interobserver agreement over Task #1, #2, and #3.<br />

Multiple-step tasks InterobserverAgreement in %<br />

Task #1 98.5<br />

Task #2 93.8<br />

Task #3 94.8<br />

Overall 96<br />

5.4.2 Outcome Data<br />

Results of this study indicate that the target child’s number of independent steps<br />

increased for each condition and for each multiple-step task. The positive effects of the<br />

intervention were higher for hand-washing and clean up than for toileting. The use of<br />

song was more effective in improving the target child’s skill for hand-washing and clean<br />

up than for the toileting task. There was a significant difference between the song<br />

intervention and the lyric intervention for Task #3 (clean up).<br />

149<br />

Hand-washing. Andy’s hand-washing performance was observed a total of 17 days: nine<br />

days for the song intervention and eight days for the lyric intervention alternating day by day.<br />

The top panel of Figure 5.2 shows the number of independent steps of the hand-washing routine<br />

completed correctly and independently by Andy in both conditions of the study. In the song<br />

intervention condition, Andy’s performance was quite constant and on a high level. On the<br />

majority of days, Andy completed five steps of the seven-step routine independently. Except on<br />

day one and day eleven, he only performed three steps, and on day nine he performed six steps of<br />

the routine. The steps Andy completed independently were not predictable (i.e., some days he


150<br />

turned the water on, on others he completed other steps of the routine). In the lyric intervention<br />

condition, Andy’s performance increased steadily but started on a lower level than the song<br />

intervention condition. Initially, he completed two steps independently, but after ten days of<br />

intervention, Andy’s performance was at the same level as the song intervention condition and<br />

appeared consistent as evidenced by three consecutive sessions with five independent steps. On<br />

the last day of the lyric intervention condition, his performance dropped to four independent<br />

steps.


Number of Independent Steps<br />

Alternating Treatments<br />

7<br />

6<br />

5<br />

4<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1<br />

0<br />

Song Lyrics<br />

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17<br />

Days<br />

10<br />

0<br />

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11<br />

6<br />

Days<br />

Figure 5.2. Number of independent steps correctly performed by Andy for hand-washing, toilet<br />

training, and cleaning-up.<br />

9<br />

8<br />

7<br />

6<br />

5<br />

4<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1<br />

5<br />

4<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1<br />

0<br />

0 1 2 3 4 5<br />

Days<br />

6 7 8 9 10<br />

Hand-washing<br />

Toilet Training<br />

Cleaning-up<br />

151


152<br />

Toilet Training. Andy’s independent toileting performance was observed a total of 11<br />

days: six days for the lyric intervention and five days for the song intervention alternating day by<br />

day. The middle panel of Figure 5.2 shows the number of independent steps of the toilet training<br />

procedure that Andy completed correctly and independently in both conditions of the study. In<br />

both conditions, little change was observed. Andy’s independent performance ranged from three<br />

to five independent steps out of the 10-step routine. The steps Andy completed independently in<br />

both conditions were mostly walking to the bathroom, sitting down, and getting up from the<br />

toilet bowl. In the lyric intervention condition, Andy’s performance was slightly higher and<br />

more stable than the song intervention condition. The song intervention condition was quite<br />

variable and decreased over time.<br />

Cleaning-up. The bottom panel of Figure 5.2 shows the number of independent steps<br />

of the clean up routine that Andy completed correctly and independently in both conditions<br />

of the study. Andy’s cleaning-up performance was observed a total of 10 days: five days for<br />

the song intervention and five days for the lyric intervention alternating day by day. In the<br />

song intervention condition, Andy’s performance was very consistently on a high level. In<br />

all of the sessions Andy completed four steps out of the six-step routine independently. The<br />

steps Andy completed independently were “pick up a toy/material” and “put it away,”<br />

completing these two steps twice. In the lyric intervention condition, Andy initially had one<br />

independent response, which was “put it away.” His independent performance was variable,<br />

but increased to song level (four steps). However, immediately afterwards it dropped to two<br />

independent steps, which were “pick up something” and “put it away.”


5.5 Discussion<br />

The major purpose of the presented study was to evaluate the effectiveness of songs<br />

as structural prompts to increase the independent performance for a young child with autism<br />

during multiple-step tasks within classroom routines, and to evaluate whether the musical<br />

presentation or the verbal presentation of the sequencing was more effective. Another<br />

purpose was to determine whether or not the classroom teacher could learn basic principles<br />

of music therapy and implement a song intervention.<br />

From this study a series findings is warranted. Prior to the implementation of the<br />

routines developed for the targeted multiple-step tasks, Andy had difficulties managing the<br />

sequences required for hand-washing, toileting and cleaning-up independently. As<br />

statistically claimed, the implementation of either form of the structured routines was<br />

successful in increasing the target child’s independent performance. Nevertheless, the song<br />

intervention was more effective than the lyric intervention for the hand-washing and<br />

cleaning-up procedures, whereas for toileting the lyric intervention was slightly more<br />

effective than the song intervention. As for the child’s positive learning progress, it can be<br />

affirmed that the teacher effectively embedded both forms of the presented sequencing in the<br />

ongoing classroom routines. Accordingly, it can be concluded that the teacher was able to<br />

learn basic principles important to music therapy and successfully implement the song<br />

interventions.<br />

The results of the multiple-step task hand-washing show that Andy’s independent<br />

performance increased during both conditions. Learning occurred as soon as either form of<br />

153


154<br />

the routine was implemented. In the beginning, of the study Andy only accomplished one<br />

step out of the routine independently (rubbing his hands). As the intervention progressed<br />

Andy learned each step of the hand-washing routine, yet he did not practice them every day<br />

or in a row. Overall Andy performed 62.2% of the hand-washing procedure. Particularly in<br />

the beginning of the study the song intervention produced greater independent performance<br />

(M= 66%) than the lyric intervention (M=57.1%). Over time, Andy became occupied by<br />

playing with the running water, which distracted him from finishing the sequencing. In both<br />

conditions the classroom teacher’s prompting was necessary to engage him in the next step of<br />

the routine (turn water off). This observation is consistent with the reports from Boswell and<br />

Gray (2003), which describe the excessive interest in running water during toilet flushing in<br />

children with autism. For future clinical application, this step might need more attention and<br />

support by teachers.<br />

However, the results of this study confirm the effectiveness of one proposed learning<br />

strategy which suggests singing songs for memorizing and structuring the hand-washing<br />

routine as practiced in inclusive childcare settings (Enoch, 2001; Geiger et al., 2000; Spear,<br />

1998; William, 1996). They also replicate earlier studies in which music was used to assist<br />

individuals with memorization of sequences of information (Gfeller, 1983; Jellison & Miller,<br />

1982; Wolfe & Horn, 1993) and extend previous studies showing that a pre-composed song<br />

with individualized lyrics can be effectively used with children with autism to accomplish the<br />

hand-washing task in ongoing classroom routines.


In spite of the short intervention phase (11 days), Andy made some progress in<br />

learning parts of the toileting procedure as evidenced by the data. Andy learned and<br />

performed all steps of the toileting routine, except step eight (assisting the teacher to re-dress<br />

by holding his shirt up). During both conditions he performed a maximum of five steps of<br />

the ten-step toileting routine at once. Overall, Andy learned 35.5% of the toileting<br />

procedure. The lyric intervention produced a slightly greater independent performance<br />

(M=38.2%) than the song intervention (M=32%). The complexity and novelty of both the<br />

toileting procedure and the unique song might have been overwhelming for Andy. This<br />

replicates earlier observations claiming that children with autism are often confused by the<br />

sequencing actions involved in toileting (Dalrymple & Boarman, 1992; Wheeler, 1998), and<br />

the novelty of songs (Brownell, 2002). The results of this study support the premise that<br />

toilet training using the scheduled method and structured teaching approach might require<br />

several months, particularly for children with autism (Boswell & Gray, 2003; Heffner, 2002;<br />

Siegel, 1998, Wheeler, 1998).<br />

In addition, the ATD used to evaluate whether the song intervention or lyric<br />

intervention was more effective might have interfered with the child’s learning process. It<br />

might have been confusing for Andy that the song was sung on one day and not on others.<br />

Boswell and Gray (2003) strongly suggest using a predictable routine when toilet-training<br />

children with autism. The predictable routine was only partly kept by following the same<br />

sequence, but the presentation of the sequence (song-lyrics) alternated daily. An A-B-A-B<br />

withdrawal design might have given clearer information on whether songs as a structural<br />

prompt are more effective than the verbal presentation of this fairly novel routine and song.<br />

155


156<br />

However, the ATD was used because it is an effective and rapid means of evaluating the<br />

superiority of one treatment over another (Alberto & Troutman, 1995; Barlow & Hayes,<br />

1979; Holcombe, Wolery & Gast, 1994; Tawney & Gast, 1984). This research design was<br />

also chosen for the convenience of completing this project – that is, limited time available,<br />

due to Andy’s impending transitioning to another school district.<br />

As suggested in the literature (Bailey & Wolery, 1992; Boswell & Gray, 2003), this<br />

intervention established an individualized toileting routine for Andy. It can be seen as a<br />

starting point for his toilet training. This way, a bathroom routine will be in place when<br />

Andy is ready for self-initiated toileting. For future implications it might be useful to<br />

approximate more time, follow a steady routine, and to use a familiar song or practice the<br />

unique song prior to the intervention with the target child to produce spontaneous and<br />

frequent song recall.<br />

Singing the cleaning-up song (Barney’s Favorites Vol. 1, 1992) clearly resulted in<br />

higher independent performance (M=66.6%) than the verbal presentation of the sequencing<br />

(M=36.7%). In all of the sessions, Andy completed four steps of the six-step routine<br />

independently. Overall, Andy performed 51.7% of the clean up procedure. It should be<br />

noticed that step 1 (“Get up!”) and 6 (“Get up and go to where the teacher wants him to go”)<br />

almost never occurred. The reason for that was two fold: (1) most of the time Andy was next<br />

to the area where his toys belonged and did not have to get up in order to clean them up, and<br />

(2) his teacher never asked him to go anywhere after cleaning-up because the class was<br />

waiting for the arrival of two other classmates before starting circle time activities.


Therefore, Andy accomplished all steps his teacher expected him to do for cleaning-up when<br />

singing to him.<br />

The results of this study confirm the effectiveness of the clinical use of songs for<br />

cleaning-up (Alger, 1984; Furman, 2001). As the results of the first task, these findings are<br />

similar to those in earlier research suggesting the use of songs for the memorization of<br />

sequences of information (Gfeller, 1983; Jellison & Miller, 1982; Wolfe & Horn, 1993).<br />

However, previous investigations have not examined the use of cleaning-up songs with<br />

children with autism, nor were the studies embedded in an inclusive preschool program.<br />

Video Excerpt 5.1. Andy performing the multiple steps included in cleaning-up<br />

during lyric intervention and song intervention.<br />

In summary, the multiple-step task cleaning-up, represented by a familiar pre-<br />

composed song (Barney’s Favorites Vol. 1, 1992) and an already established routine was<br />

most efficient in accomplishing the task. The multiple-step task hand-washing, which was<br />

combined with a familiar, pre-composed song and individualized lyrics (“Row your boat”<br />

with words altered to hand-washing), was more efficient than the verbal presentation of the<br />

routine. The multiple step task toileting, which was combined with a novel song (“Let’s go<br />

potty!” by author) and a novel routine was less efficient than the verbal presentation of the<br />

routine. These findings are to some extent similar to those reported by Wolfe and Horn<br />

(1993) who investigate the impact of combining spoken and sung stimulus input using<br />

familiar and unfamiliar songs on typically developing preschool students. Results indicate<br />

157


158<br />

that familiar and sung material promotes greater learning than unfamiliar songs or the spoken<br />

word. Unlike Wolfe and Horn’s (1993) study and Experiment I, the unfamiliar song was less<br />

efficient than the verbal presentation of the routine. However, it can be concluded that the<br />

use of more familiar material promoted greater learning and overall the song interventions<br />

were more effective in accomplishing the multiple-step tasks than the lyric interventions. For<br />

future implications it might be more useful to select familiar songs and/or melodies, or<br />

practice a new song prior to clinical application with the target child. In this way, the effects<br />

of the novelty of a song can be eliminated and spontaneous and frequent recall in desired<br />

situations can be given.<br />

As in experiment I, here too there arose several salient findings above and beyond<br />

those flowing from the main questions posed. During the intervention Andy started<br />

unexpectedly speaking in two-word sentences. As a result, every so often he used the verbal<br />

praise (e.g., “good job”) when accomplishing a multiple-step task. Andy repeatedly sang<br />

parts of the song or clapped his hands. As reported by the classroom teacher prior to the<br />

study, transitioning from one activity to an other went more smoothly, meaning Andy cried<br />

less and transitioned better when sung to. Classroom peers often sang along with the songs<br />

and participated in the routines as well. The participating classroom teacher summarized the<br />

success of the intervention in the following way: “I have enjoyed doing this study, because it<br />

has brought more structure to our routines. I believe it has made Andy understand what he is<br />

doing at each step and what will come next. He loved to sing along, too.”


From this study, the following conclusions can be drawn: First, songs can be<br />

effective for sequential learning for children with autism within inclusive classroom routines.<br />

Secondly, as in Experiment I, the teacher successfully implemented the song intervention, in<br />

spite of the short amount of training time, the novelty of implementing music therapy<br />

interventions, and the demanding classroom setting. However, closer analysis indicates that,<br />

in challenging moments (e.g., a large number of observers in the classroom), the teacher<br />

would give prompts earlier than at other times. In any case, the teacher understood the<br />

principles of music therapy and accurately implemented the procedures practiced during staff<br />

development activities. Third, the song intervention was easily implemented by the teacher<br />

within the daily classroom routine and was not more time-consuming than giving verbal<br />

prompts. Fourth, as previously suggested by Kramer (1978) and Brownell (2002), using<br />

songs for repeating classroom routines might be more pleasant and motivating for children<br />

and staff than giving verbal reminders throughout the childcare day.<br />

As with the previous study, this investigation has limitations. There are limitations<br />

imposed by the experimental designs used. As mentioned earlier, the lack of consistency<br />

within each condition when applying the ATD might have interfered with Andy’s leaning<br />

progress. Additionally, the intervention has only been implemented for one participant,<br />

because of Andy’s unique difficulties with the classroom routine. Generalization of these<br />

findings to multiple participants, as well as other situations, is warranted. Moreover, as<br />

elsewhere, it cannot be assumed from this study that other teachers are willing or able to<br />

implement song interventions.<br />

159


160<br />

From this study, a number of questions remain. For example, would an individually<br />

composed song that matches the child’s personality and the demands of the task be more<br />

effective than a familiar traditional song if practiced and well known to the target child prior<br />

to the intervention? What influence do musical elements such as tempo, rhythm, or<br />

dynamics have on sequential learning for children with autism? Does the vocal expression of<br />

the songs have an influence on the child’s behavior? Would musical accompaniment<br />

including harmonies be more motivating and structuring? Further research should<br />

investigate the effects of those musical variables to better select melodies and improve<br />

learning for children with autism. In addition, more research is warranted to provide<br />

evidence that song interventions can be embedded by teachers in inclusive childcare<br />

programs.


6. Experiment III: Increasing Peer Interaction of Children with Autism on a<br />

Childcare Playground through <strong>Music</strong><br />

6.1. Introduction<br />

One of the characteristics of children with autism is their severe delay in<br />

understanding social relationships and communication, which often results in a lack of peer<br />

interactions. Therefore, a major therapeutic objective is to increase the child’s interaction –<br />

whether dichotomous or continuous, and whether quantitative or qualitative in nature – in the<br />

context of their environment and among their peers (National Research Council, 2001). It is<br />

recommended practice for preschool programs to give children a daily opportunity to spend<br />

large blocks of time in outdoor play (Bredekamp & Copple, 2002). Playground time is<br />

important for learning and social development for preschool children, and offers a variety of<br />

play opportunities that distinguish it from indoor play (Cullen, 1993). There is a “free<br />

choice” element to outdoor play that typically developing children generally perceive and<br />

value. However, for children with special needs, especially those with ASD, huge and<br />

unorganized spaces such as playgrounds are often not meaningful. Thus, predictable play<br />

routines and therapeutic goals necessary to facilitate their participation in play and social<br />

skill development are frequently missing. A reason for this might be the common belief that<br />

playgrounds are a free play setting in which peer interactions occur naturally (Nabors et al.,<br />

2001).<br />

That this is not the case for children with special needs has been demonstrated in a<br />

study by Nabors and Badawi (1997). A total of 45 typically developing children and 19<br />

161


162<br />

children with special need ages three to five years from two childcare centers were observed<br />

interacting on the playground. <strong>Using</strong> a scan-sampling method (“snap-shot” observation of a<br />

child for 10 seconds at a time), the experimenter recorded three play types: playing alone,<br />

playing with a teacher or playing cooperatively with peers. Results indicated that children<br />

with special needs engaged less frequently in cooperative play and more often in playing<br />

alone or with a teacher than their typically developing peers.<br />

Fujiki, Brinton, Isaacson and Summer (2001) also provide evidence that elementary<br />

students with language impairment spend significantly less time interacting with peers on<br />

playgrounds than did their typically developing peers. They also demonstrate significantly<br />

more withdrawn behaviors than their typical counterparts. No other notable differences in<br />

other coded categories (adult interaction, aggression, victimization, and so forth) were<br />

observed.<br />

However, in a free play setting where no play routines are established, it is difficult, if<br />

not outright impossible, for children with autism to join in the fast play pace and play styles<br />

of their typically developing peers (Nabors et al., 2001). As a result, children with autism<br />

often wander aimlessly around, or engage in stereotypic patterns such as spinning a leaf.<br />

Clear structure of the physical environment, predictable play routines, and clear visual<br />

information are among the educational/therapeutic strategies used for children with autism to<br />

enable them to function independently in their classrooms (Marcus et al., 2001). But these<br />

strategies have not yet been implemented on playgrounds. Playgrounds need to be viewed as<br />

therapeutic settings as are classrooms (Nabors et al., 2001), especially in early childhood


programs using an integrated therapeutic approach. To utilize playground time to its fullest<br />

potential in enhancing inclusion, predictable play routines and play activities supporting peer<br />

interaction of children with autism need to be identified and established.<br />

Specific individualized interventions incorporating the children’s strengths and<br />

preferred activities need to be developed for both children with autism and their typically<br />

developing peers. Peer-mediated strategies as described in chapter 2.2.4 have proved<br />

successful in increasing peer interactions (Strain et al., 1979; Goldstein et al., 1992; McGee<br />

et al., 1992). Teacher-mediated interventions, in which the typically developing children are<br />

trained to engage a child with autism interpret their behavior and communication style, is a<br />

first step toward more independent play among the children on playgrounds. Other supports,<br />

such as prompts, cues, and reinforcement to interact successfully with one another, might be<br />

necessary (Nabors et al., 2001). Typical peers who demonstrate high social skills and<br />

interest in certain activities can be excellent role models and trainers for less skilled peers.<br />

This procedure is referred to as Vygotsky’s “zone of proximal development,” meaning that<br />

there is a range of tasks that a child cannot yet handle alone but accomplish with the help of<br />

more skilled partners (Berk & Winsler, 2002).<br />

Although many inclusive childcare programs enroll young children with autism<br />

(Dawson & Osterling, 1997; Handleman & Harris, 2001; National Research Council, 2001),<br />

no studies in the early intervention, special education or music therapy literature related to<br />

promoting desirable outcomes in increasing positive peer interactions on inclusive childcare<br />

playgrounds for preschoolers with autism are available. General guidelines have been<br />

163


164<br />

proposed for constructing safe playgrounds (American Society for Testing and Material,<br />

1993; Taylor & Morris, 1996; U.S. Consumer Product Safety Commission, 1997), for<br />

playground activities (Henninger, 1994; Griffin & Rinn, 1998; Crosser, 1994), and for<br />

promoting interaction and inclusion (Nabors & Badawi, 1997; Nabors et al., 2001; Stine,<br />

1997; Wortham & Wortham, 1989). Additionally, attention has been given to constructing<br />

playgrounds so they are accessible to young children with disabilities (Ministry of Education,<br />

1990). Only a few studies have investigated the social behaviors of typically developed<br />

children and children with special needs on playgrounds (Fujiki et al., 2001; Nabors &<br />

Badawi, 1997 as described above), and some have examined play behavior and play styles on<br />

playgrounds (Cullen, 1993; Ladd & Price, 1986). Only a small amount of research has<br />

focused on promoting interactions between children with and without disabilities on<br />

playgrounds (Hundert & Hopkins, 1992, Kern & Wolery, 2001).<br />

The following study was undertaken for three reasons. First, the FPG childcare<br />

schedule involves large blocks of time outdoors. Second, the playground is a huge space<br />

with no predictable play routines. And third, the target children with autism had difficulty<br />

engaging in meaningful play and interacting with their peers during outdoor play. An<br />

important aspect of the physical outdoor environment is to provide play opportunities<br />

accommodating different developmental levels, abilities and interests of children (Goldstein,<br />

1991). To support social interaction on playgrounds, equipment and toys should encourage<br />

cooperative play rather than solitary play (Martin et al., 1991). Preliminary observations of<br />

the target children revealed an interest in the exciting musical equipment (The Sound Path)<br />

on the FPG childcare playground (Kern & Wolery, 2001, 2002). Therefore, an outdoor


music center (<strong>Music</strong> Hut) was designed and built, incorporating elements grounded in the<br />

relevant literature (e.g., Cuvo et al., 2001; Nabors et al., 2001; Thaut, 1984). Additionally,<br />

an individualized music therapy intervention using the outdoor music center was designed<br />

and implemented. This intervention was based on an integrated therapy model by the<br />

classroom teachers and continued by the target children’s peers.<br />

6.2 Research Questions<br />

The purpose of this study was to evaluate the effects of a musical adaptation of a<br />

childcare playground and an individually designed music therapy intervention, to increase<br />

peer interaction of four young children with ASD on a playground, implemented by the<br />

classroom teachers and subsequently by classroom peers. The specific research questions<br />

asked in this experiment were as follows:<br />

(1) Does the musical adaptation of a playground (<strong>Music</strong> Hut) increase peer interactions<br />

among young children with autism on playgrounds?<br />

(2) Does the use of an individually composed song, sung by the teachers and peers,<br />

increase positive peer interaction on the playground for young children with autism?<br />

(3) Can classroom teachers learn the principles important to music therapy to increase<br />

peer interaction on the playground for young children with autism?<br />

(4) Do peers participate and model targeted tasks?<br />

6.3 Method<br />

6.3.1 Participants<br />

165<br />

The participants in this study consisted of young children with autism (n=4), typically


166<br />

developing children and children with other special needs (n=32), and their teachers (n=6).<br />

All attended the Frank Porter Graham inclusive childcare program.<br />

Target Children. Four boys with ASD, ages 3 to 5 years old participated in this<br />

study. All children were diagnosed with ASD by external agencies, using the in chapter 3<br />

described diagnostic tools. The children used Picture Communication Symbols to a certain<br />

extent as cues and visual prompts to support their communication. Two of the children<br />

(subjects 2 and 3) had participated in Experiment I, while the other children had no prior<br />

experience in music therapy-based interventions. Subject 2 and 3 attended the same<br />

classroom. The children were identified for this study by suggestions of their classroom<br />

teachers, parents and therapists based on (a) the diagnosis of ASD, (b) IEP goals, (c) absence<br />

of specific peer-related interventions on the playground, and (d) interest in music. In order to<br />

become more independent on the playground and in outdoor play, all children needed to<br />

develop greater skills in interacting with peers, and be motivated to participate in ongoing<br />

playground activities. A summary of demographic characteristics for each subject appears in<br />

Table 6.1.<br />

Subject 1. Eric was a 3-year, 4-month-old European-American boy,<br />

enrolled in the childcare program for 12 months prior to this study. He<br />

was a likeable, cheerful child and had a good temperament. However,<br />

Eric was functioning on the Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS)<br />

behavior rating scale in the mild to moderate range on the autism spectrum. He had<br />

significant weaknesses in communication and social skills. Eric was communicating with


gestures and motor actions and used some non-speech sounds. He also used several words<br />

and some approximations of two-word phrases such as “Hey you” and “All done.” Eric had<br />

a strength in imitation, but had difficulties naming objects and vocalizing desires. He also<br />

had a short attention span for play. Eric showed emotions of anger and frustration through<br />

high-pitched vocalization. He received speech/language therapy and special education<br />

services while in the classroom. Eric had positive social relationships with adults, but lacked<br />

the skills to interact appropriately with peers.<br />

Eric’s peer interaction on the playground was limited, although he approached his<br />

peers frequently and seemed to be curious and interested in what they were doing. Most of<br />

the peer interactions initiated by him were inappropriate (e.g., pushing peers, taking toys<br />

away or screaming and crying when interacting). Peers rarely initiated playing with him. He<br />

most often would ride a tricycle along the bicycle track, dig in the sandbox or manipulate a<br />

bulldozer. He also spent time observing people and running around.<br />

Eric vocalized to himself when doing activities and responded well to routines,<br />

rhythm and music. Whenever music was involved in classroom activities (e.g., during circle<br />

time), Eric was very attentive and motivated to participate. He had a good sense of rhythm<br />

and loved to sing by vocalizing.<br />

Video Excerpt 6.1. Video collage of Eric’s typical playground behaviors during<br />

baseline condition.<br />

167


168<br />

Subject 2. Ben was a 4- year old European-American boy, enrolled in<br />

the childcare program for 25 months prior to this study. He participated<br />

in Experiment I and therefore had prior experiences with music therapy<br />

interventions. Ben came to the intervention with many strengths and<br />

great potential for development. However, he was functioning on the<br />

Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) behavior rating scale in the mild to moderate range<br />

on the autism spectrum. Ben had difficulty understanding language and communication and<br />

with social interactions. He also had deficits in constructive/pretend play activities. He<br />

engaged in repetitive patterns and behaviors (e.g., spinning around). In familiar routines,<br />

Ben was able to make requests using three-word phrases with minimal prompts from adults,<br />

and he understood the meaning of single words. However, he was not able to direct his<br />

phrases towards others, and used gestures and motor actions for communication. Ben was<br />

able to imitate words and sounds, some motor actions (e.g., clapping), and some actions<br />

using objects (e.g., beating a drum), if these tasks were modeled and directly taught. He<br />

received speech/language, occupational therapy and behavioral therapy while in the<br />

classroom, as well as additional external speech/ language therapy services and a specially<br />

designed home program. Ben indicated emotions through vocalizations and gestures that<br />

reflected feelings of happiness or dislike. Ben had a limited awareness of and interest in<br />

others. He interacted with familiar adults, when he wanted something or needed to be<br />

comforted. An adult prompted nearly all of his interactions with peers.<br />

Ben rarely interacted with peers on the playground. He most often would stroll and<br />

run around aimlessly, unless an adult assisted him with meaningful play. Frequently he


turned around in a repetitive pattern while singing and signing familiar songs. Often he<br />

kneeled in the sandbox or lay on a bench. Sometimes he went down a slide independently or<br />

observed the bunny in a hutch.<br />

In general, Ben preferred videos and computer games that included music. He<br />

engaged well in musical activities (e.g., singing, dancing and drumming) during the daycare<br />

routine. Soft and mellow music seemed to interest and calm him. He seemed to enjoy<br />

accomplishing tasks and specific actions when accompanied by familiar songs.<br />

Video Excerpt 6.2. Video collage of Ben’s typical playground behaviors during<br />

baseline condition.<br />

Subject 3. Phillip was a 4-year, 9-month-old African-American boy,<br />

enrolled in the childcare program for 25 months prior to this study.<br />

Phillip was a friendly child with a sense of humor and a good<br />

169<br />

temperament. However, he was functioning on the Childhood Autism<br />

Rating Scale (CARS) behavior rating scale in the mild to moderate range on the autism<br />

spectrum. Phillip had particular difficulty with communication strategies and social<br />

interactions and deficits in his range of interests and play skills. Occasionally he engaged in<br />

repetitive movements such as spinning objects. Phillip communicated with gestures and<br />

motor actions. He also used some non-speech sounds, several words, and some<br />

approximations of two-word phrases such as “I want.” Phillip was very attentive in<br />

attempting to imitate sounds and cues, but he was not able to express comments, protests, or


170<br />

information with functional language. Phillip showed anger and frustration by vocalizing<br />

loudly, and happiness and joy by laughing, giggling and acting silly. He received<br />

speech/language and occupational therapy as well as special education services while in the<br />

classroom, and engaged in a specially designed home program. Phillip had a positive social<br />

relationship with adults unless he was asked to do a non-preferred activity.<br />

Phillip showed interest in his peers indoors as well as outdoors, but did not always<br />

know how to approach them appropriately. At times he became physically rough with peers<br />

as play progressed and peers assigned to give him a negative role or were afraid of him. He<br />

most often engaged in behaviors such as wandering aimlessly, flapping his arms, spinning a<br />

leaf or stick, sitting on a bench, or observing peers unless adults involved him in meaningful<br />

activities (e.g., riding a tricycle).<br />

Phillip responded well to singing, dancing and musical activities. His teachers used<br />

drumming as a reward for completing difficult tasks during classroom routines.<br />

Rhythmically based music seemed to capture his attention often.<br />

Video Excerpt 6.3. Video collage of Phillip’s typical playground behaviors<br />

during baseline condition.<br />

Subject 4. Lucas was a 3-year, 9-month-old African-American<br />

boy, enrolled in the childcare program for 25 months prior to this<br />

study. Lucas was a happy and energetic child. However, he was


functioning on the Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) behavior rating scale in the<br />

moderate to severe range on the autism spectrum. Lucas had severe weaknesses in imitation,<br />

social and communication skills. He engaged in play activities for four to five minutes time<br />

with maximal adult support. Lucas also engaged frequently in repetitive behaviors such as<br />

spinning toys or objects. His play activities were often accompanied by vocalization, and he<br />

engaged most often in motor activities. Lucas communicated by using objects, an adult’s<br />

hand and some non-speech sounds, but only for a limited number of purposes. Lucas<br />

received speech/language and occupational therapy as well as special education services<br />

while in the classroom. He appeared to be very interested in people and seemed to watch<br />

their activities and behaviors, but did not initiate or interact in appropriate ways. His<br />

interaction was usually limited to indicating his need for help.<br />

During playground time, Lucas frequently twisted objects (e.g., ball, acorns, plastic<br />

bottles, buckets) and followed moving objects (e.g., ball, tricycle) around the playground.<br />

He sought motor activities such as running around and engaged in body rocking. His peer<br />

interaction on the playground was minimal, often occurring only when he was interested in<br />

an object used by peers. When frustrated or overwhelmed he responded with screaming or<br />

scratching and biting others. He seemed to be scared of certain peers and some of his peers<br />

seemed to be afraid of him and refused to play with him.<br />

Lucas responded well to music combined with movement and fast changing<br />

sequences. Long sounding instruments (e.g., gong and cymbal) and rhythm, as well as the<br />

171


172<br />

imitation of his vocalization and repetitive movements, drew his attention. Dancing,<br />

spinning around, tickling and squeezing him seemed to be pleasurable for him.<br />

Video Excerpt 6.4. Video collage of Lucas’ typical playground behaviors<br />

during baseline condition.<br />

Table 6.1. Summary of characteristics for each subject.<br />

Name Chronological<br />

Age<br />

Eric 3 years<br />

4 months<br />

Developmental<br />

Information<br />

Used objects and nonspeech<br />

sounds to<br />

communicate<br />

Good imitation skills<br />

Short attention span<br />

for play<br />

Minimal interaction with<br />

peers<br />

Ben 4 years Three-word phrases and<br />

gesture, not directed to<br />

others to communicate<br />

Imitation of words,<br />

sounds, and motor<br />

actions, when directly<br />

modeled<br />

Deficits in constructive<br />

and pretend play activities<br />

Engagement in repetitive<br />

patterns<br />

Minimal interest in peers<br />

Playground Behavior <strong>Music</strong>al Preferences<br />

and Behaviors<br />

Motor activities<br />

such as riding a tricycle,<br />

digging in the sand,<br />

running around sought<br />

Interested in peers’<br />

activities, inappropriate<br />

interaction with peers,<br />

few initiations from peers<br />

Strolled and ran<br />

aimlessly around<br />

Turned around in<br />

repetitive patterns while<br />

singing and signing<br />

familiar songs<br />

Kneeled in the sandbox,<br />

or observed the bunny in<br />

a hutch<br />

Rare interaction with<br />

peers, no initiations from<br />

peers<br />

Vocalization to songs<br />

and own activities<br />

Motivated engagement in<br />

rhythm and music<br />

activities<br />

Preference for rhythmbased<br />

music<br />

Vocalization to songs<br />

and own activity<br />

Video and computer<br />

games including music<br />

<strong>Music</strong>al activities such as<br />

singing, dancing,<br />

drumming<br />

Preference for soft and<br />

mellow music<br />

Accomplishment of tasks<br />

when accompanied with<br />

familiar songs


Table 6.1. Continued<br />

Name Chronological<br />

Age<br />

Phillip 4 years<br />

9 months<br />

Lucas 3 years<br />

9 months<br />

Developmental<br />

Information<br />

Two-word phrases,<br />

gesture, and non-speech<br />

sounds to communicate<br />

Imitation sounds and cues<br />

Deficits in range of<br />

interests and play skills<br />

Engagement in repetitive<br />

patterns<br />

Interest in peers,<br />

inappropriate interaction<br />

with peers<br />

Objects and gestures to<br />

communicate<br />

Short attention span<br />

for play, only with<br />

maximal adults help<br />

Frequently engagement in<br />

repetitive patterns<br />

Interest in peers, minimal<br />

or inappropriate<br />

interaction with peers<br />

Playground Behavior <strong>Music</strong>al Preferences<br />

and Behaviors<br />

Wandered aimlessly<br />

around<br />

Engaged in respective<br />

patterns such as flapping<br />

his arms, spinning a leaf<br />

or stick<br />

Sat absent on a bench<br />

or rode tricycle, when<br />

prompted by adults<br />

Interested in peers,<br />

inappropriate interaction,<br />

peers give him negative<br />

role or avoid him<br />

Motor activities such<br />

running around and<br />

following moving objects<br />

sought<br />

Engaged in repetitive<br />

patterns such as spinning<br />

objects or body rocking<br />

Minimal or negative peer<br />

interaction, scared of<br />

peers, negative role<br />

among peers<br />

<strong>Music</strong>al activities such as<br />

singing and dancing<br />

Playing the drum, which<br />

functioned as motivator<br />

and reward<br />

Preference for rhythm<br />

Play activities<br />

accompanied by<br />

vocalization<br />

<strong>Music</strong> combined with<br />

movement and fast<br />

changing sequences<br />

Responded well to long<br />

sounding instruments,<br />

rhythm and imitation of<br />

his vocalization and<br />

movements<br />

Classroom Peers. In addition to the target children, 32 other children with and<br />

without disabilities, ages two to five years old, participated in this study. Peers included both<br />

males and females from different ethnic groups. The classroom peers participated in the<br />

study voluntarily. More specifically, children who came forward to interact with the target<br />

children were included in the intervention. Thus, two peers in each classroom were formally<br />

selected to act as “peer buddies” for each target child, and were trained by the teachers to<br />

173


174<br />

play with their classmate with autism on the playground. The “peer buddies” were identified<br />

for this study by their classroom teachers and therapists based on their (a) interest in music<br />

(b) social skills, (c) relationship to the child with autism, and (d) willingness to participate.<br />

The particular children who participated each day varied based on the children’s own actions<br />

and on class attendance.<br />

In addition to Eric (subject 1), eleven other children were<br />

enrolled in his class. The classroom peers were between<br />

three and four years of age. Nine of the children were<br />

typically developing, one of the classroom peers had<br />

cerebral palsy, and another one had autism. His “peer buddies,” were Becky, a 4-year, 5-<br />

month-old European-American girl, and Ron, a 4-year, 9-month-old African-American boy.<br />

Ben and Phillip (Subject 2 and 3), attended the same<br />

classroom. In addition to them, 10 other children were<br />

enrolled in their class. The classroom peers were<br />

between three and four years of age. Eight of the<br />

children were typically developing, one of the classroom peers had Down Syndrome, and<br />

another one was diagnosed with high functioning autism. Ben’s (subject 2) “peer buddies”<br />

were Carmen, a 4-year, 5-month-old African-American girl, and Victor, a 4-year, 4-month-<br />

old European-American boy.


175<br />

Phillip’s (subject 3) “peer buddy” Justin, was a 4- year, 10-<br />

month-old European-American boy, diagnosed as high<br />

functioning autism. One of Justin’s occupational therapy<br />

goals was to increase independent play activities on the<br />

playgrounds. Justin showed a great interest in music, which encouraged his therapists to use<br />

the music equipment (The Sound Path) as a motivator to accomplish IEP goals, such as (a)<br />

initiating play activities, (b) persisting in play for longer time, and (c) playing with peer. At<br />

the time of the study, engaging with the instruments of the Sound Path on the playground<br />

was already a familiar routine to him. When he heard about making music with friends on<br />

the playground, he made a request to his teacher to be the “peer buddy” for Phillip. Justin<br />

had good conversation and listening skills. With an adult’s present he was able to follow<br />

directions for play activities and understood turn-taking. Phillip’s second “peer buddy”<br />

originally was Jeremy, a 4-year, 2-month-old African-American boy. As the intervention<br />

began, it became clear that Jeremy was unable to sustain interest in the intervention, and he<br />

was therefore replaced by Jacky after the second session. Jacky was a 4-year, 1-month-old<br />

African-American girl who joined the class just prior the study. She initiated being part of<br />

the study by telling her the assistant teacher that she knew the song and could play with<br />

Phillip in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut.<br />

In addition to Lucas (subject 4), 13 other children<br />

were enrolled in his class. The classroom peers<br />

were between two and four years of age. Eleven of<br />

the children were typically developing, one of the


176<br />

classroom peers had Down Syndrome and another had Pfeiffers Syndrome. Lucas’ (subject<br />

2) “peer buddies, were Kayla, a 3-year, 4-month-old African-American girl, and Megan, a 2-<br />

year, 10-month-old European-American girl. Two other “peer buddies” were involved prior<br />

to Megan. However, they were replaced very early in the intervention because of frequent<br />

absences.<br />

Classroom Teachers. The teachers participating in the study were all FPG childcare<br />

program staff. Three lead teachers and three assistant teachers, all female, implemented the<br />

intervention. Teachers participated based on their interest in the study and schedule (i.e.,<br />

teacher’s break times are staggered so that the entire childcare day has coverage). Thus the<br />

lead teachers and one assistant teacher participated more of the time than did the other<br />

teaching assistants.<br />

Eric’s (subject 1) lead teacher had a master’s degree and was certified in Early<br />

Childhood Education. She had taught young children for two years. The teaching assistant<br />

had a high school diploma and had been teaching at the childcare program for two years.<br />

Neither teacher had prior experience with music therapy interventions.<br />

Ben’s and Phillip’s (subject 2 and 3) lead teacher had a bachelor’s degree and was<br />

certified in Early Childhood Education. At the time of the study, she was taking classes<br />

toward a master’s degree in Early Childhood Education. She had taught preschool children<br />

at the childcare program for five years. In this study, she implemented the intervention for<br />

Ben. The classroom’s teaching assistant implemented the intervention for Phillip. She had a


high school diploma and had been teaching at the childcare program for three years. Both<br />

teachers participated in Experiment I and therefore had prior experiences with music therapy<br />

interventions.<br />

Lucas’ (subject 4) lead teacher, had a bachelor’s degree and was certified in Early<br />

Childhood Education and had taught young children for three years. She participated in<br />

Experiment II and therefore had prior, experience with music therapy interventions. The<br />

teaching assistant had an associate’s degree in Early Childhood Education and had been<br />

teaching at the childcare program for five years. She had no prior experience with music<br />

therapy interventions.<br />

6.3.2 Setting<br />

All experimental sessions occurred on the playground of the target children’s<br />

childcare program. The playground (35 m x 47 m) contained different areas of play such as<br />

three sandboxes, a huge climbing and sliding structure, a wooden playhouse with a bunny in<br />

a hutch next to it, a concrete track (41 m x 2 m with loops on each end) for riding tricycles,<br />

two large and two small trees with wooden benches around it, and three raised flower and<br />

garden beds. The building and a 2 m high chain-linked fence to bound the playground. One<br />

corner of the playground was divided into a smaller playground by a 1m high chained-linked<br />

fence to provide a separate area for toddlers and infants. The playground contained several<br />

tricycles, wagons, large plastic building blocks, sand toys, balls, hoops, large toy trucks, and<br />

other common playground toys. Other toys and materials occasionally used by the teachers<br />

177


178<br />

and therapists on the playground included a parachute, tunnels, mats, or material for dramatic<br />

play.<br />

In addition, the physical outdoor space has been enhanced and structured for children<br />

with special needs by adding a Sound Path, which presented six music stations at meaningful<br />

locations on the playground (Kern & Wolery 2001, 2002). The stations were connected with<br />

a “path” made of a 109 m drainage pipe looping the central portion of the playground. The<br />

instruments were accessible to all children, each was easy to play, and each complied with<br />

playground safety regulations (American Society for testing and Material, 1993; U.S.<br />

Consumer Product Safety Commission, 1997). The stations were designed to be multi-<br />

sensory by using different materials, shapes, colors and sounds. Some ideas proposed by<br />

Snoezelen (Cuvo, Might & Post, 2001; Hulsegge & Verheul, 1998) for indoor multi-sensory<br />

stimulation environments were included in the station designs. All musical instruments were<br />

hand-made from donated scrap materials.<br />

The first music station, Touch Board, was near to an entrance to the playground. It<br />

consisted of a bamboo xylophone, jingle bells, and rainmaker attached in line to the chain-<br />

link fence and post. A second set of jingle bells and bamboo xylophone was set up as a<br />

mirror image of itself. The second music station was a hardwood xylophone about 1 m in<br />

length with wooden mallets for playing it. It was located near the climbing and sliding<br />

structure. The third station was a Taxi Stand. It had a yellow painted bench and trellis with a<br />

bicycle bell and horn attached. This music station was located near the tricycle track. The<br />

fourth music station, Sound Pipes, included seven copper pipes of different lengths attached


to a tree trunk with wooden mallets to activate the sound. This tree had a bench around it and<br />

was a place where teachers and children often congregate to converse and interact. The fifth<br />

station, Triangle Tree, consisted of two large triangles hung from the branches of a second<br />

tree. The wind activated them, and a metal mallet was attached to the triangles so that they<br />

could be rung. The sixth station, Bucket Drums, were three galvanized pails attached<br />

between wooden posts in one of the sandboxes. The music stations were connected to one<br />

another through the use of a 10 cm drainage pipe. The pipe had ridges on it (i.e., as a guiro)<br />

producing sound when a stick was rubbed on it. The pipe stopped near each music station to<br />

cue the location. Figure 6.1 depicts the playground equipment including the Sound Path.<br />

Figure 6.1. Playground equipment, including the Sound Path on the FPG childcare<br />

program’s playground.<br />

179


180<br />

The daily preschool schedule included two blocks of outdoor play, adjusted to the<br />

needs of the children. Experimental sessions occurred on the playground during the regular<br />

morning playground time. In addition to the three classes participating in the study, two<br />

other classes, enrolling seven children, each ages two years, were on the playground at<br />

staggered but overlapping times. As a result, the number of children during morning outdoor<br />

play varied, based on the classroom schedule, classroom activities, attendance and weather<br />

condition. Because the childcare program was affiliated with the university, students,<br />

observers and visitors were often on the playground in addition to the children and childcare<br />

program staff.<br />

6.3.3 Material<br />

Five major components were important for this study: (1) the <strong>Music</strong> Hut, (2) songs<br />

for the <strong>Music</strong> Hut, (3) a unique song composed for each target subject, (4) an individualized<br />

book, and (5) a practice CD accompaniment by a song transcription. Each is described in<br />

more detail below.<br />

(1) The <strong>Music</strong> Hut. The <strong>Music</strong> Hut is an outdoor music center, purposefully designed<br />

to expand musical opportunities and therapeutic playground activities of young children,<br />

especially those with autism (Kern, Marlette, & Snyder, 2002). It was built in collaboration<br />

with an architect and employees of the carpentry shop at the University of North Carolina at<br />

Chapel Hill. Design and construction meet the guidelines of U.S. playground safety<br />

regulations (American Society for Testing and Material, 1993; U.S. Consumer Product<br />

Safety Commission, 1997). The <strong>Music</strong> Hut is connected with the Sound Path, and is


accessible by a wooden wheelchair ramp. It consists of a 3 m x 2.4 m long hardwood deck,<br />

located in the center of the large sandbox on the playground, and is covered with a 6.1 m x<br />

8.2 m green fabric canopy.<br />

The following instruments are attached to wooden beams, steel arches and Plexiglas<br />

walls: a 56 cm Chinese Lion Wind Gong, three Tubanos in different sizes (large, medium,<br />

small), a Kid’s Floor Drum with a 20 cm x 25 cm Head, Kid’s Bongos with a 13 cm x 15 cm<br />

Heads, a High Head, a Mini Cabasa, three Sound Tubes made out of PVC pipes of different<br />

lengths, one 71 cm Marching Drum and an 53 cm Ocean Drum. Twenty-centimeter-long<br />

wooden dowels, wrapped on one quarter of an end with compressed foam material were used<br />

as a drumstick to activate the instruments. <strong>Using</strong> a spatula to hit the pipes on the top<br />

activated the Sound Tubes. The drumsticks were stored in two wooden boxes, attached to a<br />

wooden beam next to the PVC pipes and the drums. All instruments were donated from<br />

West <strong>Music</strong> and local musicians or built by the investigator. A CD player stand, as well as a<br />

step for the cymbal, was added during the intervention. For the purposes of the study, the<br />

second musical station of the Sound Path, a xylophone, was moved to the <strong>Music</strong> Hut. Figure<br />

6.2 shows a panorama view of the <strong>Music</strong> Hut.<br />

Figure 6.2. Panorama view of the outdoor music center (<strong>Music</strong> Hut), showing the<br />

instruments and construction.<br />

181


182<br />

(2) Songs for the <strong>Music</strong> Hut. A booklet and CD with songs and activities for the<br />

<strong>Music</strong> Hut was distributed to the participating teachers and childcare program’s therapists<br />

prior to the intervention. The booklet included songs incorporating instruments in the <strong>Music</strong><br />

Hut and three instrumental pieces to promote therapeutic goals such as interaction, initiation,<br />

cooperative play, self-expression and awareness, attention, and body control (Schnur Ritholz<br />

& Robbins, 1999). Piano accompaniment and voice, as well as a sing- along of each piece,<br />

were recorded on the CD. Theses materials were provided to encourage teachers to expand<br />

their own repertoire of musical activities. The teachers were also encouraged to introduce<br />

the music during circle time in the classroom to expand the children’s musical repertoire for<br />

the <strong>Music</strong> Hut.<br />

A medley entitled “Everybody in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut” with distinct contrasts of mood,<br />

tempo and rhythm for each instrument was written for the Grand Opening of the outdoor<br />

music center. A transcription of the medley is shown in Figure 6.3. All children and staff of<br />

the childcare program were invited to participate in this event. A special introduction of the<br />

instruments using a puppet, the medley and musical games was given.


183


184


185


186


Figure 6.3. Transcript of the medley: “Everybody in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut,” written for the Grand<br />

Opening of the <strong>Music</strong> Hut.<br />

(3) A unique song composed for each target subject. The themes of each song<br />

match the subjects, their needs and abilities, musically. The intention of each song is to<br />

increase positive peer interaction and to engage in meaningful play during outdoor<br />

activities. The content of each song focuses on these general goals. Additionally, IEP<br />

goals, developed by the interdisciplinary team of the FPG childcare program, were<br />

incorporated for each child individually.<br />

Eric. “Hey you” is a song written for subject 1. A transcription of the song is shown in<br />

Figure 6.4. The song builds on Eric’s available imitation and verbal skills, and mirrors his<br />

187


188<br />

cheerful personality musically. The following IEP goals were also addressed by the song:<br />

increase vocalization, use appropriate body contact, make choices, take turns, and name<br />

peers. The theme of the song picks up Eric’s favorite approximations of the two-word phrase<br />

“Hey you!” in an appropriate voice range. Tapping a partner’s shoulder on count two and<br />

three in measure one and two supports appropriate body contact and gets the children’s<br />

attention. To practice choice-making, the target child chooses one of the instruments in the<br />

<strong>Music</strong> Hut and names or points it out it in count three of measure three. The lyrics signal<br />

when the children are expected to play together or to take turns. <strong>Music</strong>al rests in measure 10,<br />

12, 14, 16, 18, 20, 22 and 24 anticipate the expected beating pattern on the chosen<br />

instrument. In measure, 18 and 22, peers model how to play, followed by imitation by the<br />

target subject in measure 20 and 24. In measure 17, 19, 21, 23 the children name the person<br />

who plays the next beats. Overall, the song displays opportunities for positive peer<br />

interactions and purposeful outdoor play. Table 6.2 gives an overview of the IEP goals<br />

incorporated in the song “Hey you.”<br />

Table 6.2. Overview of Eric’s IEP goals incorporated in the song “Hey You.”<br />

Increase vocalization<br />

Use appropriate body contact<br />

Make choices<br />

Take turns<br />

Name peers<br />

IEP Goals Song<br />

Singing along the melody of the song<br />

Clapping on partner’s shoulder<br />

Choose an instrument in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut<br />

Child 1 plays two beats, child 2 plays two beats<br />

Inclusion of peer’s name in the lyrics of the song


Figure 6.4. Transcript of the song ”Hey You,” written for Ben addressing social interaction<br />

and specific IEP goals.<br />

189


190<br />

Ben. “You and I” is a song written for subject 2. A transcription of the song is<br />

shown in Figure 6.5. The song builds on Ben’s available communication skills, imitation<br />

skills and musical preferences, and mirrors his mellow personality musically. The following<br />

IEP goals, developed by the interdisciplinary team of the FPG Childcare program, are also<br />

addressed in the song: improve communication and sign language, increase interest in and<br />

awareness of others, increase imitation of peers, increase choice-making. The theme of the<br />

song reflected the central therapeutic goal for Ben, which was to increase interest in<br />

interacting with others. American Sign Language (Bornstein, Saulnier & Hamilton, 1992), as<br />

practiced in Ben’s class, was applied for “You,” “I,” and “music.” Ben was also encouraged<br />

to sing the lyrics of the song. In measures seven and eight, the children’s attention to get<br />

ready to play is supported through the accelerando. Peers model how to play the instrument<br />

in measure nine, immediately imitated by Ben in measure ten. This alternation is continued<br />

throughout the song. The lyrics and music clearly cue the children how to proceed.<br />

Choosing and naming instruments in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut increases choice making. Overall the<br />

song provides opportunities for positive peer interactions and purposeful outdoor play. Table<br />

6.3 gives an overview of the IEP goals incorporated in the song “You and I.”<br />

Table 6.3. Overview of Ben’s IEP goals incorporated in the song “You and I.”<br />

Improve communication and sign language<br />

Increase interest in and awareness of others<br />

Increase imitation of peers<br />

Increase choice making<br />

IEP Goals Song<br />

Singing the lyrics of the song combined with<br />

signing “You”, “I,” and “music”<br />

Joint peer activity<br />

Play what peers model<br />

Choose an instrument in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut


191


192<br />

Figure 6.5. Transcript of the song ”You and I,” written for Ben addressing social interaction<br />

and specific IEP goals.<br />

Phillip. “Phillip’s Groove” is a song written for subject 3. A transcription of the<br />

song is shown in Figure 6.6. The song builds on Phillip’s available imitation and verbal<br />

skills, and musically mirrors his good temperament. The following IEP goals are also


addressed in the song: improvement of verbal skills and sign language, make choices, name<br />

peers and self, wait for turns, use appropriate body contact. Phillip’s common<br />

approximations of two-word phrases are included in the song. American Sign Language<br />

(Bornstein, Saulnier, & Hamilton, 1992) as practiced in Phillip’s class was applied for “all<br />

done,” “good-bye,” and “thank you.” For choice making, the children choose one of the<br />

instruments in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut and name it in measure two, four, and six. The chosen<br />

instrument is introduced on beat four in measures two, four and six. In measures nine and<br />

13, the children name whose turn it is to play. The named child plays along the rhythm of<br />

the melody. The use of appropriate body contact was practiced with Phillip by hugging a<br />

peer buddy and a teacher’s improvised verse on dancing (added at the second day of<br />

intervention). The last verse of the song encourages signing “All done,” “Good-bye” and<br />

“Thank you.” Overall, the song provides opportunities for positive peer interaction and<br />

purposeful outdoor play. Table 6.4 gives an overview of the IEP goals incorporated in the<br />

song “Phillip’s Groove.”<br />

Table 6.4. Overview of Phillip’s IEP goals incorporated in the song “Phillip’s Groove.”<br />

IEP Goals Song<br />

Improvement verbal skills and sign language<br />

Make choices<br />

Name peers and self<br />

Wait for turns<br />

Use appropriate body contact<br />

Singing the lyrics of the song, signing<br />

“All done!,” “Good-bye,” and “Thank you”<br />

Choose an instrument in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut<br />

Inclusion of peers and Phillip’s name in the lyrics<br />

of the song<br />

Peer plays 4 measures, Phillips plays four measures<br />

afterwards or vice versa<br />

Give hugs for “Good-bye”<br />

193


194<br />

Figure 6.6. Transcript of the song ”Phillip’s Groove,” written for Phillip addressing social<br />

interaction and specific IEP goals.


Lucas. “Lucas’ Dance” is a song written for subject 4. A transcription of the song is<br />

shown in Figure 6.7. The song builds on Lucas’ strong motor skills and his preference for<br />

high level of sensory input, and mirrors his happy and energetic personality. The following<br />

IEP goals were also addressed in the song: share attention, look at people and objects, point<br />

to things of interest, wait for turns, use appropriate body contact, and increase play and<br />

duration of play. All verses of the song require shared attention, to look at peers and objects.<br />

The lyrics in verse 1, measure one to eight might encourage Lucas to share his attention with<br />

his peers, and the activity is based on his motor skills. In verse 2 he is asked to point to his<br />

preferred instrument. Participating in turn taking is encouraged in verse 2 and 4, measure<br />

nine to 20. In verse 2, child 1 is encouraged to play the chosen instrument and hand over the<br />

drumstick to the partner for the repetition of this section. In verse 4, child 1 is encouraged to<br />

tickle Lucas, and vice versa for the repetition. To learn about appropriate body contact,<br />

dancing with each other in verse 1, giving hugs in verse 3, and tickling in verse 4 are<br />

incorporated in the lyrics of the song. The song was intended to increase Lucas’ play activity<br />

and his duration of play to 10 minutes. The song is flexible regarding the mood and<br />

temperament of the target child. The verses can be adapted to the situation, e.g., when Lucas<br />

is too excited, using verse 3 (hugs) might calm him down, or if he looses interest in an<br />

activity, verse 1 (dancing) can be repeated. Overall, the song provides opportunities for<br />

positive peer interaction and purposeful outdoor play. Table 6.5 gives an overview of the<br />

IEP goals incorporated in the song “Lucas’ Dance.”<br />

195


196<br />

Table 6.5. Overview of Lucas’ IEP goals incorporated in the song “Lucas’ Dance.”<br />

IEP Goals Song<br />

Share attention; look at people and objects<br />

Point to things of interest<br />

Wait for turns<br />

Use appropriate body contact<br />

Increase play and duration of play<br />

All verses of the song require shared attention, to look at<br />

peers and objects<br />

Point to preferred instrument<br />

Hand over the drum stick to partner<br />

Dancing, receiving/giving hugs, and tickling<br />

Maintain play activities up to 10 minutes at a time


197


198<br />

Figure 6.7. Transcript of the song ”Lucas’ Dance,” written for Lucas addressing social<br />

interaction and specific IEP goals.<br />

(4) Individualized Picture Book. To cue the transition from the classroom to the<br />

playground visually, a 18 cm x 22 cm laminated picture book was made for each subject.<br />

The book included a picture of the <strong>Music</strong> Hut on the left and a picture of each selected<br />

peer buddy on the right. The words written on the front side of the book was the<br />

subject’s name and “Playground Activity.” The words written above the <strong>Music</strong> Hut<br />

picture were “Play in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut with your friend ...,” and each peer’s name under<br />

his/her picture.


(5) Practice CD accompaniment by a song transcription. The unique song for<br />

each subject was recorded on separate practice CD’s and given to the staff. To ensure<br />

successful learning, each song on the practice CD was presented as follows: (a) piano<br />

accompaniment, vocals with lyrics and percussion, to give the whole impression of the<br />

song; (b) melody played by the piano, to learn the melody properly; (c) sing and play<br />

along with piano accompaniment, to practice singing and playing the instruments; and (d)<br />

melody sung with lyrics, to model how to sing on the playground. Additionally each<br />

song was transcribed as music and lyrics, including melody, chords, lyrics and percussion<br />

and lyrics only.<br />

6.3.4 Procedures<br />

Experimental sessions occurred Monday to Friday, one time daily for each subject<br />

during morning outdoor play. The playground schedule of each class was staggered. Lucas’<br />

classes’ morning playground time started at 10:15 a.m. to 11:00 a.m., followed by Ben’s and<br />

Phillip’s class at 10:30 a.m. to 11:15 a.m., and Eric’s class at 11:00 a.m. to 11:45 a.m. The<br />

order of observation was based on the participants’ presence or availability. In the case of<br />

Ben and Phillip, who entered the playground together, Ben was observed before Phillip for<br />

most sessions. Data collection, staff development activities and intervention procedures are<br />

described below in the order in which they occurred chronologically.<br />

Baseline Procedure. In the baseline condition (Condition A), a session started as<br />

soon as the subjects entered the playground. No instruction was given to either peers or<br />

subjects. Staff was asked not to interact with the subjects during the 10-minute period of<br />

199


200<br />

data collection, unless it was necessary (e.g., when the subject needed help) or the subject<br />

initiated the interaction. The purpose in this was to allow evaluating the natural, unstructured<br />

and unsupported peer interactions of the subjects occurring on the playground. Playground<br />

equipment and material as described in the setting of the study was available for all children.<br />

Procedure after the Adaptations of the Playground. As noted, the playground was<br />

adapted with the <strong>Music</strong> Hut (Condition B). As soon as entering the playground, staff walked<br />

the subject over to the <strong>Music</strong> Hut, gave him a drumstick and asked him to play in the <strong>Music</strong><br />

Hut. The session started as soon as the subject entered the <strong>Music</strong> Hut. As in the baseline<br />

condition, no instruction was given to either peers or subjects. Once again it was required<br />

that staff not interact with the subjects during the 10 minutes of data collection unless<br />

necessary (e.g., when the subject needed help) or the subject initiated the interaction, for the<br />

purpose stated previously under “Baseline Procedure.”<br />

Staff Development Activities. Training and collaborative consultation occurred after<br />

all goals and procedures for the intervention were identified by the interdisciplinary team<br />

(including the teachers), and after songs were composed for each child with autism. Initially<br />

the purpose of the study, how the intervention would fit into existing classroom routines, and<br />

how data would be collected during the embedded intervention, were discussed with the<br />

teacher. All teachers were trained, in a one-to-one format, to use principles of music therapy<br />

and materials individually designed for the target children. The unique song for each subject<br />

was transcribed and recorded on separate practice CD’s and given to the staff along with a<br />

written description of the study procedure. The songs and how to use the practice CD (as


described in the material section) was introduced to each teacher individually, and to the<br />

whole class during circle time indoors shortly prior to the actual intervention in the <strong>Music</strong><br />

Hut. The teachers were also encouraged to practice the specific song with their class.<br />

Additionally, a verbal orientation to the procedure in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut, including hands-on<br />

experiences and opportunities to role-play, was presented to the teachers. Specific<br />

instructions regarding how to engage and assist the subject and peers to interact with each<br />

other in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut were given verbally and modeled included:<br />

• how to use the picture book as a visual cue for the transition from the classroom to<br />

the outdoor play, by showing and reading the book to both the child with autism and<br />

his peer buddies shortly before going outside, assisting the target child to chose a<br />

peer and asking him if her or he wants to play and sing in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut;<br />

• how to take the subject and a chosen peer to the <strong>Music</strong> Hut, by holding hands and<br />

preparing/motivating the children verbally for the musical activities that follow;<br />

• how to initiate play and include children on their level, by using the ISO-Principle<br />

(Benenzon, 1997), including children’s ideas and strengths in the procedure;<br />

• how to involve the children in singing the song and playing the instruments by<br />

inviting the children to sing along the particular intervention song while playing the<br />

named instruments in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut, and giving verbal praise as reinforcer;<br />

• how to model verbalization, body contact, cooperative play, turn taking, choosing<br />

instruments<br />

o by saying/singing the words clearly and pausing to give the target child time<br />

to respond to the music, and repeat if necessary<br />

201


202<br />

o by holding the target child’s hand when guiding him to a certain area or while<br />

dancing, touching him gently to get his attention, hugging and tickling him<br />

gently<br />

o by inviting both or more children to participate in singing, playing and<br />

movement, sharing material and giving each child space and attention for<br />

his/her contribution<br />

o by following the lyrics in turn taking and/or handing over a drum stick for<br />

turns<br />

o by identifying the instrument’s name and sound and giving the children free<br />

or limited choice to pick one<br />

• how guide to other activities in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut by announcing and modeling the<br />

activity and inviting them to participate;<br />

• how to continue to play in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut for 10 minutes at a time by engaging the<br />

children and motivating them through the activity;<br />

• how to assist peers in developing a play routine by identifying their preferences and<br />

strengths and asking or suggesting how to play with the target child;<br />

• how to train the peers to model the tasks, by modeling the tasks, developing and<br />

practicing a play routine, clarifying questions and misinterpretations, mediate<br />

challenging moment, giving verbal praise;<br />

• how to gradually reduce the verbal and physical prompting during Condition D<br />

(Peer-Mediated Intervention) by giving the lead to the peers, stepping out of the<br />

<strong>Music</strong> Hut, and reducing verbal guidance as much as possible; and


• how to communicate a positive attitude toward the target child by anticipating the<br />

intervention with enthusiasm and emphasis the strengths of the target child.<br />

The teachers were asked to use the material flexibly and to improvise in order to meet<br />

the needs of the children (e.g., to add lyrics to the song, extend musical improvisation on the<br />

instruments, or include musical ideas from children). They were also encouraged to include<br />

other peers on the playground, if they showed an interest in participating in the musical<br />

activity. <strong>Collaborative</strong> consultation, including feedback and suggestions, was provided<br />

during and following intervention sessions.<br />

Teacher-Mediated Intervention Procedure. In this intervention phase (Condition C),<br />

a predictable routine and structure was established for each subject. First, an individualized<br />

picture book (as described in the material section) was shown to the subject prior the outdoor<br />

play to ease the transition from indoor to outdoor play. As a part of the intervention, two<br />

peers were formally selected for each subject as peer buddies and trained by their classroom<br />

teacher during the intervention phase. At least one peer was chosen for each day, based on<br />

which one the subject pointed out in the picture book and willingness of the peer to<br />

participate. Secondly, the teacher walked the subject and at least one peer to the <strong>Music</strong> Hut<br />

and verbally prepared them for the activity. Thirdly, as soon as they entered the <strong>Music</strong> Hut<br />

the teacher started singing the specific song written for the subject, initiated playing the<br />

instruments or dancing, and encouraged the children to participate in the activity and interact<br />

with one another as described in Staff Development Activities.<br />

203


204<br />

Peer-Mediated Intervention Procedure. The goal of the peer-mediated intervention<br />

Phase (Condition D) was to have the peer-buddy and subject interact and play with each<br />

other independently. The procedure was similar to that in the teacher-mediated intervention<br />

phase (Condition C). The only change was that teachers withdrew their support gradually<br />

after the peer buddies and subjects learned the song and play routine. First teachers reduced<br />

the physical prompting by stepping out of the <strong>Music</strong> Hut and giving verbal prompts from<br />

outside, and second by just giving prompts when necessary.<br />

6.3.5 Experimental Design<br />

A multiple baseline design replicated across four subjects was used to evaluate the<br />

effects of adapting the playground (Condition B), the teacher-mediated intervention<br />

(Condition C), and subsequently the peer-mediated intervention (Condition D). In a multiple<br />

baseline design, the first step is to identify the target behavior, which is commonly shared by<br />

all subjects. This target behavior becomes the baseline. Experimental control is established<br />

by collecting baselines for each subject until a sufficient stability is exhibited (a stable pattern<br />

or constant trend, and a minimum of three data points). Then the intervention is applied to<br />

one subject while the baseline conditions are continued with the other individuals. If stability<br />

is achieved during treatment for the first subject, the intervention is applied to the second<br />

individual. This procedure is repeated on a staggered times for all remaining subjects and for<br />

all conditions. The treatment effects can be seen clearly in comparison to baseline condition<br />

and/or in contrast to other applied conditions. Because the intervention is replicated across<br />

several subjects at staggered times, it is very unlikely that external factors cause the response<br />

change. One advantages of this single-case experimental design is that replication and


experimental control can be achieved without withdrawal of treatment (Alberto & Troutman,<br />

1995; Aldridge, 1994, 1996; Hanser, 1995; Holcombe, Wolery, & Gast, 1994; Kazdin, 1982;<br />

Tawney & Gast, 1984).<br />

conditions:<br />

The experimental design in this study involved four sequentially implemented<br />

• Baseline (Condition A)<br />

The subjects' positive interactions with all peers on the playground prior to the<br />

availability of the <strong>Music</strong> Hut.<br />

• Adaptation of the Playground (Condition B)<br />

The subjects' positive interactions with all peers on the playground when the <strong>Music</strong><br />

Hut was available.<br />

• Teacher-Mediated Intervention (Condition C)<br />

A unique song was composed for each subject. Teachers, at least one “peer-buddy,”<br />

the subject and other voluntary peers sang the song in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut.<br />

• Peer- Mediated Intervention (Condition D)<br />

The same song was sung in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut by at least one “peer-buddy” and the<br />

subject.<br />

The first two conditions (baseline without <strong>Music</strong> Hut, and <strong>Music</strong> Hut) allowed for an<br />

A-B (baseline-intervention) evaluation of the absence and presence of the <strong>Music</strong> Hut.<br />

However, this portion of the design does not allow for functional relationships to be<br />

established, as there are no replications (intersubject) of the effects due to the nature of the<br />

205


206<br />

intervention (i.e., the addition of the <strong>Music</strong> Hut). However, starting with the third condition<br />

(teacher-mediated intervention), a multiple baseline across subjects design was used. This<br />

portion of the design allows for functional relationships to be evaluated, because of the<br />

intersubject and replications. Data collection occurred over eight months.<br />

6.3.6 Measurements<br />

In Experiment III, the following three measurement procedures and responds<br />

definitions applied:<br />

(1) Categories of interaction behaviors.<br />

Categories of interaction behaviors were coded through direct observation of a 10-minutes<br />

videotaped segment using a 15-second momentary time sampling recording procedure. Data<br />

was recorded during Condition A and B (baseline- adaptation of the playground) when the<br />

subject and his peers were present on the playground. During Condition C and D (teacher-<br />

mediated intervention-peer-mediated intervention) when the subject, his teacher, peers and at<br />

least one of his “peer-buddies” were present on the playground. Each observation lasted 10-<br />

minutes per day. In Condition A (baseline) the observation started as soon as the target child<br />

and his peers entered the playground or after class activities on the playground, in Condition<br />

B (adaptation of the playground) as soon as the subject entered the <strong>Music</strong> Hut or when he<br />

was already in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut, and in Condition C and D (teacher-mediated intervention-peer-<br />

mediated intervention) as soon as the subject and at least one peer-buddy entered the <strong>Music</strong><br />

Hut. All sessions were videotaped with a Sony Digital Video Camera Recorder DCR-<br />

TRV17 and analyzed afterwards. The momentary time sampling system was employed as


follows: (a) The observer used a CD recording cuing her every 15 seconds to the time for<br />

observation/recording; (b) At each 15-second point, the observer made a judgment about the<br />

occurrence of behaviors in each category for both peers and subjects at that point in time; and<br />

(c) The observer recorded the results for each category on a specially designed data sheet.<br />

The following behaviors were coded for both (a) interaction of peer, and (b)<br />

interaction of child with autism:<br />

(a) Interaction of peer.<br />

• Initiates Interaction was defined as a peer making the first step toward an interaction.<br />

• Positive Interaction was defined as a peer initiating a positive interaction with the<br />

subject or responding positively to the interaction initiated by the subject. If the<br />

peer’s intention of interaction was positive, but received a negative response from the<br />

subject, it was evaluated as a negative interaction and not recorded on the data sheet.<br />

Negative interaction was defined as inappropriate behavior of peers and the subject<br />

(e.g., pushing, taking something away, screaming, scratching) and was not coded.<br />

• Stays in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut was defined as the chosen peer stayed in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut for the<br />

period of data collection in Condition C and D.<br />

(b) Interaction of child with autism<br />

• Initiates Interaction was defined as the subject making the first step towards the<br />

interaction.<br />

• Positive Interaction was defined as the subject initiating a positive interaction with<br />

peer or responding positively to the interaction initiated by peer. If the subject’s<br />

intention of interaction was positive, but received a negative response from peers, it<br />

207


208<br />

was evaluated as a negative interaction and not recorded on the data sheet. Negative<br />

Interaction was defined as inappropriate behavior of peers and the subject (e.g.,<br />

pushing, taking something away, screaming, scratching).<br />

• Stays in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut was defined as the subject stayed in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut for the<br />

period of data collection in Condition B, C and D (to determine weather the<br />

interaction was tied to a musical activity).<br />

• Play and Engagement with Material and Equipment was defined as actively<br />

manipulating a material and using the equipment (e.g., digging in the sand box,<br />

sliding, riding a tricycle, walking purposely to a location, activating the instruments<br />

in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut or Sound Path).<br />

• Interaction Supported by Adults was defined as the teachers prompting the subject<br />

and his peers verbally/musically (e.g., “Gentle, stop scratching!” or “Play the drum”)<br />

or physically (e.g., teacher puts her hand on either child in order to facilitate positive<br />

interaction). For both subjects and peers, examples for positive interaction are as<br />

follows:<br />

o Singing/Playing together including vocalization and/or playing an instrument<br />

or engaging in a musical or in Condition A in a non-musical game with peers<br />

o Communicate with each other, meaning any positive verbalization or<br />

vocalization was made toward each other<br />

o Giving/taking/sharing objects in a positive way<br />

o Reaching for or touching each other with the intent for positive physical<br />

contact<br />

o Watch each other’s activity or have eye contact


(2) Task behaviors of teacher and peers.<br />

Additionally five steps of teacher’s task behaviors in Condition C, and peer’s task behaviors<br />

in Condition D were recorded on a separate data sheet. A judgment was made, whether or<br />

not the sequence of tasks were completed during the 10 minutes of data collection each day.<br />

• Teacher’s task behaviors in Condition C was defined as follows: (1) entering the<br />

<strong>Music</strong> Hut with the subject and at least one peer buddy; (2) initiating play, including<br />

both children on their level; (3) singing the individually composed song and playing<br />

the instruments in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut; (4) modeling the content of the song, e.g., choosing<br />

an instrument, naming peers, waiting for turns, appropriate body contacts; and (5)<br />

continuing to play in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut for 10 minutes at a time.<br />

• Peer’s task behaviors in Condition D was defined as follows: (1) entering the <strong>Music</strong><br />

Hut with the subject, (2) initiating play, including the subject (3) singing the<br />

individually composed song and playing the instruments in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut, (4)<br />

modeling the content of the song, e.g. choosing an instrument, naming peers, waiting<br />

209<br />

for turns, appropriate body contact, and (5) continuing to play in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut for 10<br />

minutes at a time.<br />

(3) Field Notes<br />

Field notes were recorded on the bottom of the task following data sheet as<br />

outlined in Appendix C and in a separate logbook.


210<br />

6.3.7. Reliability<br />

Interobserver agreement data were collected on the subjects, peers and teachers<br />

behavior. A music therapist and the investigator collected interobserver agreement<br />

during baseline sessions (Condition A), adaptation of the playground (Condition B),<br />

intervention session (Condition C), and peer-mediated intervention session (Condition<br />

D). Before the baseline was started, the two observers coded the peer interaction on<br />

playgrounds by using 10-minute videotaped recordings until they reached a mean<br />

agreement level of greater than 85%. A second observer was trained due to the drop out<br />

of the music therapist in the last phase of Condition C and D. Agreement between the<br />

scores from the first and second reliability observer was calculated prior the start of the<br />

data collection phase by using videotaped segments from previously evaluated sessions to<br />

establish an acceptable level of agreement of greater than 85%. Reliability checks were<br />

carried out on an average of 36.8% of total observations within each phase and for each<br />

child. Two coders coded the videotaped segment for each target behavior. Interobserver<br />

agreement levels for each code category were calculated by summing the number of<br />

agreements (defined as the same coding interval) and dividing the number by sum of<br />

agreements plus disagreements and multiplying by 100.


6.4 Results<br />

6.4.1 Interobserver Agreement<br />

211<br />

Interobserver agreement data on subject, peer and teacher performance were collected<br />

in 39.5% of the baseline session, in 38.5% of the adaptation of the playground (Condition B),<br />

in 33.8% of the intervention session (Condition C), and in 35.7% of the peer-mediated<br />

intervention session (Condition D) for each child. During baseline condition, reliability data<br />

showed observers agreed on participants’ performance 99.5% of the trials (range: 99.3% to<br />

99.8%). The percentage of agreement was 99.3% for Eric, 99.5% for Ben, 99.3% for Phillip,<br />

and 99.8% for Lucas. During the adaptation of the playground phase (Condition B),<br />

reliability data showed observers agreed on the participants’ performance 99.5% of the trials<br />

(range: 99.4% to 99.6%). The percentage of agreement on participants’ responses was<br />

99.6% for Eric, 99.4% for Ben, 99.5% for Phillip, and 99.5% for Lucas. During the teacher-<br />

mediated intervention phase (Condition C), reliability data showed observers agreed on<br />

participants’ performance in 97.4% of the trials (range: 96.6% to 98.3%). The percentage of<br />

agreement on the participants’ responses was 98.3% for Eric, 98.0% for Ben, 96.6% for<br />

Phillip, and 96.7% for Lucas. During the peer-mediated intervention phase (Condition D),<br />

reliability data showed observers agreed on the participants’ performance 96.3% of the trials<br />

(range: 93.8% to 98%). The percentage of agreement on the participants’ responses was<br />

93.8% for Ben, 97.0% for Phillip, and 98.0% for Lucas. The overall interobserver agreement<br />

was 98.2% (range 93.8% to 99.8%). Table 6.6 gives a summary of the interobserver<br />

agreement in Experiment III.


212<br />

Table 6.6. Interobserver agreement for each target child in conditions A, B, C, and D.<br />

Participants Condition A, in % Condition B, in % Condition C, in % Condition D, in %<br />

Eric 99.3 99.6 98.3 N/A<br />

Ben 99.5 99.4 98 93.8<br />

Phillip 99.3 99.5 96.6 97.0<br />

Lucas 99.8 99.5 96.7 98.0<br />

Overall 99.5 99.5 97.4 96.3<br />

6.4.2 Outcome Data<br />

The outcome data of Experiment III are presented in the following order:<br />

(1) Positive Peer Interactions<br />

(2) Teacher’s Task Behaviors and Positive Peer Interactions<br />

(3) Peer’s Task Behaviors and Unsupported Positive Peer Interactions<br />

(4) Positive Peer Interactions and Stays in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut<br />

(5) Play and Engagement<br />

(1) Positive Peer Interactions<br />

All children with autism had few interactions with their peers during baseline<br />

condition (M=3.2%). After the adaptations of the playground (<strong>Music</strong> Hut), peer interactions<br />

increased slightly, but stayed at a very low level (M=7.1%). During the teacher-mediated<br />

intervention phase (Condition C), meanwhile the peer interactions increased substantially for<br />

all children. This change was abrupt and stayed high through out the entire condition<br />

(M=66.2%). During the peer-mediated intervention (Condition D), the supported peer


interaction appeared slightly lower than in Condition C, but stayed at a very high level<br />

(M=69.5%). The independent peer interaction was at a lower level as the supported peer<br />

interaction, but was significantly higher than during baseline condition and the adaptation on<br />

the playground phase (M=21.1%). One exception was Eric (subject 1). His teachers made a<br />

commitment for a limited time to participate in the study, which did not allow enough time<br />

for Eric to go into Condition D (peer-mediated intervention). Table 6.7 gives an overview of<br />

the mean intervals and range of positive peer interaction for each target child by each<br />

condition of the study. Figure 6.8 shows the percentage of positive peer interactions for each<br />

target child by each condition of the study.<br />

Table 6.7. Range (Min% and Max%), Mean (M%), and Standard Deviation (SD) of positive<br />

peer interaction for each target child, by each condition of Experiment III.<br />

Condition Min Max<br />

Eric<br />

M<br />

SD<br />

Min<br />

Ben<br />

Max<br />

M<br />

SD<br />

Min Max<br />

Phillip Lucas<br />

M<br />

SD<br />

Min Max<br />

A 0.0 17.5 5.9 6.0 0.0 12.5 1.4 3.8 0.0 15.0 4.1 6.8 0.0 5.0 1.5 1.6<br />

B 2.5 40.0 16.7 14.1 0.0 12.5 2.7 3.7 0.0 32.5 6.3 8.4 0.0 10.0 2.5 2.8<br />

C 32.5 67.5 48.8 12.1 52.5 92.5 77.5 10.8 32.5 95.0 77.4 14.5 27.5 80.0 61.0 14.4<br />

D sup N/A N/A N/A N/A 42.5 80.0 66.9 9.3 60.0 92.5 70.3 10.6 62.5 80.0 71.6 6.5<br />

D unsup N/A N/A N/A N/A 0 30 17.3 10.9 7.5 32.5 21.9 8.8 12.5 40 24.1 8.6<br />

M<br />

SD<br />

213


214<br />

A B C D<br />

100<br />

80<br />

Eric<br />

60<br />

40<br />

20<br />

0<br />

5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55 60 65 70 75 80 85 90<br />

100<br />

80<br />

Ben<br />

60<br />

40<br />

20<br />

0<br />

5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55 60 65 70 75 80 85 90<br />

100<br />

80<br />

% of Positive Peer Interactions<br />

Phillip<br />

60<br />

40<br />

20<br />

0<br />

5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55 60 65 70 75 80 85 90<br />

100<br />

80<br />

Lucas<br />

60<br />

Unsupported<br />

Interaction<br />

Supported<br />

Interaction<br />

40<br />

20<br />

0<br />

5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55 60 65 70 75 80 85 90<br />

Figure 6.8. Percentage of positive peer interactions for each target child, by each condition of Experiment III.


Eric. Eric was observed a total of 30 sessions. Figure 6.9 shows the percentage of<br />

Eric’s positive peer interactions by each condition of the study. In the baseline condition,<br />

Eric’s positive peer interaction was quite low and variable. All data points were 18% or less,<br />

with a range from 0% to 18%. In four sessions out of 11, no positive peer interaction was<br />

observed. In the majority of observations the positive peer interaction was below 6%. The<br />

adaptation of the playground (<strong>Music</strong> Hut) resulted in an increase of Eric’s positive peer<br />

interactions, but the quality of these interactions was quite inconsistent from day to day,<br />

ranging from 3.% to 40%. The teacher-mediated intervention (Condition C) resulted in an<br />

abrupt and sharp increase in the level of data. The intervention increased significantly in<br />

positive peer interaction, although it was variable across days. On all observations, Eric’s<br />

percentage of positive peer interaction was greater than 33%, with a range from 33% to 68%.<br />

For the last four observations, the positive peer interaction was stable in a range from 40% to<br />

45%. On every day of observation, positive peer interaction was noted. The peer-mediated<br />

intervention (Condition D) was not implemented due to teacher’s time commitment based on<br />

classroom schedule issues.<br />

215


216<br />

Figure 6.9. Percentage of positive peer interactions for Eric during Conditions A, B, and<br />

C.<br />

% of Positive Peer Interactions<br />

100<br />

95<br />

90<br />

85<br />

80<br />

75<br />

70<br />

65<br />

60<br />

55<br />

50<br />

45<br />

40<br />

35<br />

30<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

A B C<br />

0 5 10 15 20 25<br />

Sessions<br />

Unsupported Interaction<br />

Supported Interaction<br />

Ben. Ben was observed a total of 56 sessions. Figure 6.10 shows the percentage of<br />

Ben’s positive peer interactions by each condition of the study. In the baseline condition,<br />

Ben’s positive peer interaction was very low and fairly stable. All data points were 13% or<br />

less, and ranged from a low of 0% to a high of 13%. Only in two out of 11 sessions, positive<br />

peer interaction was observed. The adaptation of the playground (<strong>Music</strong> Hut) resulted in a<br />

small increase in Ben’s positive peer interaction, whereupon the subsequent readings were<br />

variable. All data points were still 13% or less with a range from 0% to 13%. In seven out<br />

of 15 observations, positive peer interaction was observed. The teacher-mediated


intervention (Condition C) resulted in an abrupt and sharp increase in positive peer<br />

interactions, although day-on-day changes fluctuated considerably. On all observations,<br />

Ben’s percentage of positive peer interaction was greater than 53%, with a range from 53%<br />

to 93%. The majority of observations were above 80%. On every day of observation<br />

positive peer interaction was noted. On nine out of 18 observations, peer interaction was<br />

greater than 80%. The peer-mediated intervention (Condition D) resulted in a small decrease<br />

of supported positive peer interaction. The percentage of supported positive peer interaction<br />

continued at a very high level, however, ranging from 43% to 80%. The majority of<br />

observations were above 65%, except for one day of intervention, which resulted in 43%.<br />

The peer-mediated intervention resulted in a greater percentage of unsupported positive peer<br />

interaction, compared with Condition B, and was quite variable. All data points ranged from<br />

0% to 30%. The majority of observations topped 20% and reverted back to 0% in only two<br />

sessions.<br />

217


218<br />

Figure 6.10. Percentage of positive peer interactions for Ben during Conditions A, B, C, and<br />

D.<br />

% of Positive Peer Interactions<br />

100<br />

95<br />

90<br />

85<br />

80<br />

75<br />

70<br />

65<br />

60<br />

55<br />

50<br />

45<br />

40<br />

35<br />

30<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

A B C D<br />

0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55<br />

Sessions<br />

Unsupported Interaction<br />

Supported Interaction<br />

Phillip. Phillip was observed for a total of 63 sessions. Figure 6.11 shows the<br />

percentage of Phillip’s positive peer interactions by each condition of the study. In the<br />

baseline condition, Phillip’s positive peer interaction was quite low. All data points were<br />

15.0% or less, with a range from 0.0% to 15.0%. In five out of eight sessions, no positive<br />

peer interaction was observed. The adaptation of the playground (<strong>Music</strong> Hut) resulted in a


small increase in Phillip’s positive peer interaction, and subsequent readings were quite<br />

consistent. During the first 15 observations, peer interaction was below 7% with one<br />

exception. In the second part (9 sessions), readings were variable and ranged from 0% to<br />

23%. The teacher-mediated intervention (Condition C) resulted in an abrupt and sharp<br />

increase in the level of data. The intervention increased significantly in positive peer<br />

interaction although it was variable across days. On all observations, Phillip’s percentage of<br />

positive peer interaction was upwards of 33%, with a range from 33% to 93%. The majority<br />

of observations were above 64%, except the first day of intervention, which resulted in 33%.<br />

On every day of observation positive peer interaction was noted. On nine out of 22<br />

observations, the peer interaction was greater than 80%. The peer-mediated intervention<br />

(Condition D) resulted in a small decrease of supported positive peer interaction during the<br />

first four observations and consistent day-to-day for the second half. All data points were<br />

above 60%, with a range from 60% to 93%. The peer-mediated intervention (Condition D)<br />

resulted in an increased percentage of unsupported positive peer interaction compared with<br />

Condition B and was quite variable. All data points were above 8%, with a range from 8% to<br />

33%. The majority of observations were above 23%, but never approached baseline readings<br />

of 0% positive peer interactions.<br />

219


220<br />

% of Positive Peer Interactions<br />

Figure 6.11. Percentage of positive peer interactions for Phillip during Conditions A, B, C,<br />

and D.<br />

100<br />

95<br />

90<br />

85<br />

80<br />

75<br />

70<br />

65<br />

60<br />

55<br />

50<br />

45<br />

40<br />

35<br />

30<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

A B C D<br />

0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55 60<br />

Sessions<br />

Unsupported Interaction<br />

Supported Interaction<br />

Lucas. Lucas was observed a total of 71 sessions. Figure 6.12 shows the percentage<br />

of Lucas’ positive peer interactions by each condition of the study. In the baseline condition,<br />

Lucas’ positive peer interaction was very low – and stably so. All data points were 5% or<br />

less, with a range from 0% to 5%. In six out of 13, sessions no positive peer interaction was<br />

noted. The adaptation of the playground (<strong>Music</strong> Hut) resulted in a small increase of Lucas’


positive peer interaction and was quite consistent. All data points were 10% or less with a<br />

range from 0% to 10%. In 13 out of 29, observations no positive peer interaction was<br />

observed. The teacher-mediated intervention (Condition C) resulted in a sudden and acute<br />

increase in the level of data. The intervention increased significantly in positive peer<br />

interaction, though very variable across days. On all observations, Lucas’ percentage of<br />

positive peer interaction was greater than 28%, with a range from 28% to 80%. The majority<br />

of observations lay above 58%. Day on day, positive peer interaction was noted. In the peer-<br />

mediated intervention (Condition D), no apparent change was observed on a consistent basis.<br />

All data points were above 63%, with a range from 63% to 80%. The peer-mediated<br />

intervention (Condition D) resulted in an increased percentage of unsupported positive peer<br />

interaction compared with Condition B, but this change resulted in variable readings as well.<br />

All data points were above 13% , with a range from 13% to 40%. The majority of<br />

observations was above 20%, never deflated back to baseline levels of 0%.<br />

221


222<br />

Figure 6.12. Percentage of positive peer interactions for Lucas during Conditions A, B, C,<br />

and D.<br />

% of Positive Peer Interactions<br />

100<br />

95<br />

90<br />

85<br />

80<br />

75<br />

70<br />

65<br />

60<br />

55<br />

50<br />

45<br />

40<br />

35<br />

30<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55 60 65 70<br />

(2) Teachers’ Task Behaviors and Positive Peer Interactions<br />

Procedural fidelity data were collected on teachers’ task behaviors during the teacher-<br />

mediated intervention (Condition C) to answer the third research question: Can classroom<br />

teachers learn the principles important to music therapy to increase peer interaction on the<br />

playground for young children with autism?<br />

A B C D<br />

Sessions<br />

Unsupported Interaction<br />

Supported Interaction


All data of the target children’s teachers’ task behavior were consistently on a high level,<br />

except for Eric’s teachers’ data, which dropped abruptly to a low level of data after the first<br />

half of Condition C. Interestingly, the teachers’ task behavior correlated with the positive<br />

peer interaction. That is, when the teacher’s task behavior was on a high level, the peer<br />

interaction was also on a high level and vice versa. With only one exception, all teachers<br />

implemented the intervention on a very high level.<br />

Table 6.8. Range (Min% and Max%), Mean (M), and Standard Deviation (SD) of teacher<br />

task behavior for each target child in Condition C.<br />

Condition<br />

Eric<br />

Min Max<br />

M<br />

SD<br />

Min<br />

Ben<br />

Max<br />

M<br />

SD<br />

Min Max<br />

Phillip Lucas<br />

M<br />

SD<br />

Min Max<br />

C 1 4 2.4 1.4 4 5 4.8 0.4 4 5 4.9 0.4 3 5 4.7 0.6<br />

M<br />

SD<br />

223


224<br />

% of Positive Peer Interactions<br />

100<br />

80<br />

60<br />

40<br />

20<br />

0<br />

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9<br />

Sessions<br />

100<br />

5<br />

80<br />

60<br />

40<br />

20<br />

0<br />

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17<br />

100<br />

Sessions<br />

5<br />

80<br />

60<br />

40<br />

20<br />

0<br />

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21<br />

100<br />

Sessions<br />

5<br />

80<br />

60<br />

40<br />

20<br />

0<br />

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1011121314151617181920<br />

Sessions<br />

Figure 6.13. Teacher’s task behaviors and positive peer interactions in Condition C for<br />

Eric (top panel), Ben (second panel), Phillip (third panel), and Lucas (bottom panel).<br />

4<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1<br />

0<br />

4<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1<br />

0<br />

4<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1<br />

0<br />

5<br />

4<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1<br />

0<br />

Steps of Teachers‘ Tasks<br />

Eric<br />

Ben<br />

Phillip<br />

Lucas<br />

Supported<br />

Interactions<br />

Teachers’ Task<br />

Behaviors


Eric. The teachers’ task behavior was observed a total of 10 sessions. The top panel<br />

of Figure 6.13 shows the number of steps Eric’s teachers completed correctly, as well as<br />

Eric’s positive peer interaction during Condition C. Eric’s teachers’ task behavior was on a<br />

high level and consistent for the first half of Condition C, then abruptly dropped to a low<br />

level for the second half of Condition C. During the first five sessions, except session four,<br />

Eric’s teachers completed four out of the five steps of the teacher-mediated intervention<br />

procedure correctly (except continuing to play in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut for 10 minutes). During the<br />

second half of the teacher-mediated intervention procedure, only one step was accomplished<br />

(model tasks of song), with the exception of session eight (2 steps). In the beginning of<br />

Condition C, Eric’s positive peer interaction increased. Data show a decrease in Eric’s<br />

positive peer interaction as soon as the teacher’s task behavior dropped to a very low level.<br />

Ben. The teacher’s task behavior was observed a total of 18 sessions. The second<br />

panel of Figure 6.13 shows the number of steps Ben’s teacher completed correctly, as well as<br />

Ben’s positive peer interaction during Condition C. The teachers’ task behavior was on the<br />

highest level and very consistent. On all sessions his teacher performed all steps. Only in<br />

three sessions during the second half of Condition C valid, Ben’s teacher complete four steps<br />

of the teacher-mediated intervention procedure (except continuing to play in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut<br />

for 10 minutes) correctly. Ben’s positive interaction was also at a very high level, but was<br />

variable across days. The data indicate that the variability correlated with the teacher’s task<br />

behavior, meaning that he had less positive peer interactions on the three sessions his teacher<br />

accomplished only four steps out of the five steps procedure.<br />

225


226<br />

Phillip. The teacher’s task behavior was observed a total of 22 sessions. The third<br />

panel of Figure 6.13 shows the number of steps Phillip’s teacher completed correctly,<br />

together with Phillip’s positive peer interaction during Condition C. The teachers’ task<br />

behavior was on the highest level and very consistent. In 19 sessions his teacher performed<br />

all five steps. In only three sessions did Phillip’s teacher complete “only” four steps of the<br />

teacher-mediated intervention procedure correctly (except enter <strong>Music</strong> Hut with the dyad and<br />

continuing to play in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut for 10 minutes). Not surprisingly, Phillip’s positive<br />

interaction was also on a very high level, and variable across days. The data indicate that the<br />

variability coincided closely with the teacher’s task behavior, meaning that he had less<br />

positive peer interactions on the three sessions in which his teacher accomplished only four<br />

steps out of the five-step procedure.<br />

Lucas. The teachers’ task behavior was observed a total of 21 sessions. The bottom<br />

panel of Figure 6.13 shows the number of steps Lucas’ teachers completed correctly, as well<br />

as Lucas’ positive peer interaction during Condition C. The teachers’ task behavior was on<br />

the highest level and very consistent. On 16 sessions, his teachers performed all steps. Only<br />

in four sessions did Lucas’ teachers complete four steps (continuing to play in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut<br />

for 10 minutes), and in one session only three steps, of the teacher-mediated intervention<br />

procedure (except model tasks of song and continuing to play in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut for 10<br />

minutes) correctly. On the majority of days, Lucas’ positive interaction was greater than<br />

58% but was variable across days. The data indicate that the variability correlated with the<br />

teacher’s task behavior, meaning that even small differences in the teachers’ task behavior


made a difference in Lucas’ positive peer interactions. This relationship is evident in the<br />

data.<br />

(3) Peers’ Task Behaviors and unsupported Positive Peer Interactions<br />

Procedural fidelity data were collected on peer’s task behaviors during the peer-<br />

mediated intervention (Condition D) to answer the third question: Do peers participate and<br />

model targeted tasks?<br />

The data related to peers’ task behavior show that all peers implemented parts of the<br />

five steps of the peer-mediated intervention procedure correctly. The data of Phillip’s and<br />

Lucas’ peer buddies was consistent and on a high level. The data of Ben’s peer buddies were<br />

variable, however, ranging from two steps to five steps. No data were collected on Eric’s<br />

peers’ task behavior, because Condition D was not implemented. As with teacher fidelity,<br />

peers’ task behavior correlated with the positive peer interaction for Ben and Phillip. That is,<br />

when peers’ task behavior was on a high level, the interaction among them was also on a<br />

high level and vice versa. As addressed in the Discussion section 6.5, Lucas’ anomalous<br />

reaction to the peer based intervention might be more readily understood as function of his<br />

specific ASD symptomatology.<br />

227


228<br />

Table 6.9. Range (Min % and Max %), and Standard Deviation (SD) of peer task behavior<br />

for each target child in Condition D.<br />

Condition Min Max<br />

Eric<br />

M<br />

SD<br />

Min<br />

Ben<br />

Max<br />

M<br />

SD<br />

Min Max<br />

Phillip Lucas<br />

M<br />

SD<br />

Min Max<br />

D N/A N/A N/A N/A 2 5 3.6 0.9 4 5 4.9 0.4 2 5 4.3 1<br />

M<br />

SD


% of Positive Peer Interactions<br />

100<br />

80<br />

60<br />

40<br />

20<br />

0<br />

100<br />

80<br />

60<br />

40<br />

20<br />

0<br />

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11<br />

Sessions<br />

5<br />

0<br />

0<br />

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7<br />

Sessions<br />

100<br />

5<br />

80<br />

60<br />

40<br />

20<br />

0<br />

0<br />

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7<br />

Sessions<br />

Figure 6.14. Peer’s task behaviors and positive peer interactions in Condition D for Ben (top<br />

panel), Phillip (middle panel), and Lucas (bottom panel).<br />

5<br />

4<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1<br />

4<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1<br />

4<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1<br />

Steps of Peers’ Tasks<br />

Ben<br />

Phillip<br />

Lucas<br />

Unsupported<br />

Interactions<br />

Peer Task<br />

Behaviors<br />

229


230<br />

Ben. The peers’ task behavior was observed for a total of 12 sessions. The top panel<br />

of Figure 6.14 shows the number of steps Ben’s peer buddies completed correctly, as well as<br />

Ben’s unsupported positive peer interaction during Condition D. His peers’ task behavior<br />

was very variable and ranged from two steps to five steps. On two of the sessions Ben’s<br />

peers completed five steps, and on one day only two steps (initiate to play and include child<br />

with autism, sing the song with the child with autism) of the peer-mediated intervention<br />

procedure correctly. Ben’s unsupported positive interaction was quite variable. All data<br />

points ranged from 0% to 30%, with very low levels when peers’ task following dropped to<br />

lower levels (session 2, 3 and 10).<br />

Phillip. The peers’ task behavior was observed a total of eight sessions. The middle<br />

panel of Figure 6.14 shows the number of steps Phillip’s peer buddies completed correctly as<br />

well as Phillip’s unsupported positive peer interaction during Condition D. The peers’ task<br />

behavior was on the highest level and very consistent. On seven sessions the peers<br />

completed all steps. In only two sessions, Phillip’s peers completed four steps of the peer-<br />

mediated intervention procedure correctly (except enter <strong>Music</strong> Hut together). Phillip’s<br />

unsupported positive peer interaction was variable across days and ranged from 8% to 33%.<br />

On six out of eight days, positive peer interaction was quite stable. On only two days did this<br />

drop to 8%. And on one day of this, this drop-off coincided with a concomitant drop-off in<br />

the number of peer steps completed (i.e., from five to four).<br />

Lucas. Lucas’ peers’ task behavior was also observed in eight sessions. The bottom<br />

panel of Figure 6.14 shows the number of steps Lucas’ peer buddies completed correctly, as


well as Lucas’ unsupported positive peer interaction during Condition D. The peers’ task<br />

behavior was on quite a high level, that is, always four steps or higher. Except on the first<br />

session, Lucas’ peers completed only two steps of the peer-mediated intervention procedure<br />

(initiate to play and include child with autism, sing the song with the child with autism)<br />

correctly. Lucas’ unsupported positive peer interactions were variable across days and<br />

ranged from 13% to 40%. The data indicate no correlation between peers’ task behavior and<br />

unsupported positive interactions.<br />

(4) Positive Peer Interactions and Stays in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut<br />

The correlation of positive peer interactions and staying in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut is presented<br />

to answer the third research question. Does the musical adaptation of a playground (<strong>Music</strong><br />

Hut) increase the peer interaction of young children with autism on playgrounds, and<br />

demonstrate if musical activity – this as opposed to more traditional activities on the<br />

playground increased peer interaction for the target children. When in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut, the<br />

children more likely engaged in musical activity such as playing the instruments, singing and<br />

dancing than on other areas of the playground. With the original playground setting<br />

(Condition A) the target children had few interactions with their peers. When the outdoor<br />

music center was available (Condition B), the target children spend at least some time almost<br />

every in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut. However, the data bear witness to only a small increase in positive<br />

peer interactions over the previous condition, and then at a very low level. During the<br />

teacher-mediated intervention phase (Condition C), the target children stayed significantly<br />

more time in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut. In this scenario the peer interaction spiked to a high level and<br />

stayed high through the entire condition for all children. During the peer-mediated<br />

231


232<br />

intervention (Condition D), the target children continued to stay in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut. The<br />

positive peer interaction appeared slightly lower than in Condition C, but remained a high<br />

level nonetheless.<br />

Table 6.10. Range (Min% and Max%), Mean (M%), and Standard Deviation (SD) of stays<br />

in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut for each target child by Conditions B, C, and D of Experiment III.<br />

Condition Min Max<br />

Eric<br />

M<br />

SD<br />

Min<br />

Ben<br />

Max<br />

M<br />

SD<br />

Min Max<br />

Phillip Lucas<br />

M<br />

SD<br />

Min Max<br />

B 2.5 90.0 28.1 34.5 0.0 50.0 15.7 12.8 2.5 62.5 16.9 15.7 2.5 80.0 23.0 20.7<br />

C 27.5 77.5 52.3 18.8 85.0 100 98.1 4.0 70.0 100 96.1 7.9 50.0 100 90.8 15.9<br />

D N/A N/A N/A N/A 65.0 100 92.3 10.7 95.0 100 97.8 2.1 82.5 100 92.8 5.4<br />

M<br />

SD


% of Positive Interactions and Stays in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut<br />

100<br />

80<br />

60<br />

40<br />

20<br />

0<br />

100<br />

80<br />

60<br />

40<br />

20<br />

0<br />

80<br />

60<br />

40<br />

20<br />

0<br />

A B C<br />

0 5 10 15 20 25<br />

A B C D<br />

Sessions<br />

0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55<br />

Sessions<br />

100<br />

00 05 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55 60<br />

Sessions<br />

100<br />

80<br />

60<br />

40<br />

20<br />

0<br />

0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55 60 65 70<br />

Sessions<br />

Figure 6.15. Percentage of positive peer interactions and stays in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut for Eric (top<br />

panel), Ben (second panel), Phillip (third panel), and Lucas (bottom panel) in all conditions.<br />

Eric<br />

Ben<br />

Phillip<br />

Lucas<br />

Positive<br />

Interaction<br />

In <strong>Music</strong> Hut<br />

233


234<br />

Eric. The top panel of Figure 6.15 shows the percentage of Eric’s positive peer<br />

interactions and staying in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut. Eric was observed a total of 30 sessions. In the<br />

baseline condition, no <strong>Music</strong> Hut was available, and Eric’s positive peer interaction was<br />

quite low and variable. When the <strong>Music</strong> Hut was opened (Condition B), Eric’s stay in the<br />

<strong>Music</strong> Hut ranged from 2.5% to 90% of the daily observation time. Only on three sessions<br />

he stayed more than 50%. The adaptation of the playground resulted in a small increase of<br />

Eric’s positive peer interaction. With the teacher-mediated intervention (Condition C), Eric<br />

stayed significantly longer in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut (range: 27.5% to 77.5%). The positive peer<br />

interaction increased significantly after the intervention was introduced, although it was<br />

variable across days. The peer-mediated intervention (Condition D) was not implemented.<br />

Ben. The percentage of Ben’s positive peer interactions and stays in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut is<br />

shown in the second panel of Figure 6.15, broken down by each condition of the study.<br />

Altogether, Ben was observed for a total of 56 sessions. At baseline, no <strong>Music</strong> Hut was<br />

available and Ben’s positive peer interaction was predictably quite low and very stable.<br />

When the <strong>Music</strong> Hut was opened (Condition B), however, Ben’s stay in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut<br />

ranged from 0% to 50% in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut. Only on three sessions he stayed more than<br />

27.5%. The adaptation of the playground resulted in a small increase of Ben’s positive peer<br />

interaction, and was quite consistent. With the teacher-mediated intervention (Condition C)<br />

Ben stayed significantly longer in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut overall (range: 85% to 100%). The positive<br />

peer interaction in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut increased significantly after the intervention was<br />

introduced, even though it was variable across days. During the peer-mediated intervention<br />

(Condition D) Ben’s stay in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut decreased a bit and was less stable (range: 65% to


100%). The peer-mediated intervention resulted in a small decrease in positive peer<br />

interaction. Again, however, the percentage of positive peer interaction was still at a very<br />

high level.<br />

Phillip. The third panel of Figure 6.15 represents the percentage of Phillip’s positive<br />

peer interactions and stays in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut in the four study conditions, with a total of 63<br />

sessions altogether. In baseline measurements, no <strong>Music</strong> Hut was available, and Phillip’s<br />

positive peer interactions were expectedly quite low. After the <strong>Music</strong> Hut was opened<br />

(Condition B), Phillip’s stay in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut ranged from 2.5% to 62.5%. Only in six<br />

sessions did he stay more than 30% of the allotted time. The adaptation of the playground<br />

resulted in a small increase in Phillip’s positive peer interactions with quite consistent<br />

readings to follow. With the teacher-mediated intervention (Condition C), Phillip’s stay in<br />

the <strong>Music</strong> Hut ranged from 70% to 100%. It resulted in a sharp increase in the level of data,<br />

although it was variable across days. During the peer-mediated intervention (Condition D)<br />

Phillip continued to stay in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut for a sustained period (range: 95% to 100). The<br />

peer-mediated intervention resulted in a small decrease of positive peer interaction, but the<br />

percentage of positive peer interaction remained at a very high level.<br />

Lucas. Lucas was observed a total of 71 sessions. The bottom panel of Figure 6.15<br />

shows the percentage of Lucas’ positive peer interactions and stays in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut, broken<br />

at by each condition of the study. In the baseline condition, no <strong>Music</strong> Hut was available,<br />

and Lucas’ positive peer interaction was very low and stable. When the <strong>Music</strong> Hut opened<br />

(Condition B), Lucas’ stay in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut ranged from 2.5% to 80%. In 15 sessions he<br />

235


236<br />

stayed more than 20%. Adaptation of the playground, for its part, resulted in a small increase<br />

of Lucas’ positive peer interaction and was quite consistent. With the intervention<br />

(Condition C) Lucas stayed significantly longer in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut (range 50% to 100%). The<br />

positive peer interaction in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut increased significantly after the intervention was<br />

introduced although, it was very variable across days. During the peer-mediated intervention<br />

(Condition D), Lucas’ stay in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut decreased a little and was less stable (range:<br />

82.5% to 100%). In the peer-mediated intervention (Condition D), positive peer interaction<br />

appeared to be similar to the previous condition. The percentage of positive peer interaction<br />

was still at a very high level.<br />

(5) Play and Engagement<br />

Play and engagement with material and equipment was defined as actively<br />

manipulating material and using the equipment in a meaningful way. In Condition A and B,<br />

Ben’s, Phillip’s and Lucas’ percentage of play and engagement with material and equipment<br />

was on a low level and variable across days. In Condition C and D, their play and<br />

engagement abruptly increased to a very high level while variability continued to be hallmark<br />

of postvention reading. Eric’s play and engagement was very variable across all Conditions,<br />

and in Condition A on a higher level than the other children’s play and engagement.


Table 6.11. Range (Min% and Max%), Mean (M%), and Standard Deviation (SD) of play<br />

and engagement for each target child, by each condition of Experiment III.<br />

Condition Min Max<br />

Eric<br />

M<br />

SD<br />

Min<br />

Ben<br />

Max<br />

M<br />

SD<br />

Min Max<br />

Phillip Lucas<br />

M<br />

SD<br />

Min Max<br />

A 25.0 97.5 52.7 22.9 0.0 37.5 13.6 12.8 0.0 97.5 20.0 33.4 0.0 20.0 4.0 6.7<br />

B 25.0 80.0 52.8 14.9 0.0 60.0 20.8 15.2 0.0 60.0 15.3 12.9 0.0 57.5 18.0 13.6<br />

C 37.5 95.0 65.3 19.1 40.0 95.0 72.9 14.6 47.5 95.0 71.4 12.6 17.5 57.5 40.7 12.2<br />

D N/A N/A N/A N/A 40.0 67.5 59.2 8.2 47.5 85.0 61.6 11.6 10.0 50.0 28.8 12.3<br />

M<br />

SD<br />

237


238<br />

% of Play and Engagement and Stays in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut<br />

100<br />

80<br />

60<br />

40<br />

20<br />

0<br />

0 5 10 15 20 25<br />

A B<br />

Sessions<br />

C D<br />

100<br />

80<br />

60<br />

40<br />

20<br />

A B C<br />

0<br />

0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55<br />

Sessions<br />

100<br />

80<br />

60<br />

40<br />

20<br />

0<br />

00 05 10 15 20 25 30<br />

Sessions<br />

35 40 45 50 55 60<br />

100<br />

80<br />

60<br />

40<br />

20<br />

0<br />

0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55 60 65 70<br />

Sessions<br />

Figure 6.16. Percentage of play and engagement and stays in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut for Eric (top<br />

panel), Ben (second panel), Phillip (third panel), and Lucas (bottom panel), in all conditions.<br />

Eric<br />

Ben<br />

Phillip<br />

Play and<br />

Lucas<br />

Engagement<br />

In <strong>Music</strong> Hut


Eric. Eric was observed a total of 30 sessions. The top panel of Figure 6.16 indicates<br />

the percentage of Eric’s play and engagement by each condition of the study. In the baseline<br />

condition, Eric’s play and engagement with material and equipment was very variable with a<br />

low of 25% and a high of 97.5%. The level of data appeared similar across Conditions A and<br />

B (baseline-adaptation of the playground). Eric’s play and engagement was still variable<br />

across days, with a low of 25% to a high of 80%. The teacher-mediated intervention<br />

(Condition C) resulted in an increase of Eric’s play and engagement and a decreasing trend<br />

after the third session, ranging from a low of 37.5% to a high of 95%. The peer-mediated<br />

intervention (Condition D) was not implemented.<br />

Ben. The number of Ben’s observations spanned over 56 sessions. The second panel<br />

of Figure 6.16 shows the percentage of Ben’s play and engagement with material and<br />

equipment by each condition of the study. At baseline, Ben’s play and engagement was on a<br />

low level and was variable from day to day, with a range of 0% to 37.5%. The level of data<br />

was similar across Conditions A and B (adaptation of the playground). Ben’s play and<br />

engagement was still variable across days, with a low of 0% to a high of 60%. The teacher-<br />

mediated intervention (Condition C) effected an abrupt and large change in his play and<br />

engagement, though with a low of 40% to a high of 95%, and still variable across days. The<br />

peer-mediated intervention (Condition D) resulted in a lower level of data than the teacher-<br />

mediated interventions, and ranged from a low of 40% to a high of 62.5%. Ben’s play and<br />

engagement was still significantly higher than in Conditions A and B.<br />

239


240<br />

Phillip. The third panel of Figure 6.16 corresponds to the percentage of Phillip’s play<br />

and engagement with material and equipment, by each condition of the study. Phillip was<br />

observed a total of 63 sessions. In the baseline condition, Phillip’s play and engagement was<br />

at a low level (except in session five) and variable across days, however, with a low of 0% to<br />

a high of 97.5%. The level of data appeared similar across Conditions A and B (baseline-<br />

adaptation of the playground). Phillip’s play and engagement was still variable across days,<br />

however, with a low of 0% to a high of 60%. The teacher-mediated intervention (Condition<br />

C) resulted in a sudden, striking change in data readings on Phillip. His play and engagement<br />

was on a much higher level, with a low of 47.5% and a high of 95%, while variability<br />

continued across days. The peer-mediated intervention (Condition D) resulted in a lower<br />

level of data than Condition C, ranging from a low of 47.5% to a high of 85%, but was still<br />

significantly higher than Phillip’s play and engagement in Conditions A and B.<br />

Lucas. Lucas was observed a total of 71 sessions, as represented in the bottom panel<br />

of Figure 6.16. In the baseline condition, Lucas’ play and engagement was on a low level<br />

and was variable across days, with a low of 0% to a high of 20%. The adaptation of the<br />

playground (<strong>Music</strong> Hut) shows an increasing trend of Lucas’ play and engagement.<br />

Variability continued, with a low of 0% to a high of 57.5%. The teacher-mediated<br />

intervention (Condition C) resulted in an abrupt change in the level of data. Lucas’ play and<br />

engagement was on a higher level, with a low of 17.5% to a high of 57.5%, variable across<br />

days, and shows a decreasing trend toward the end of Condition C. The peer-mediated<br />

intervention (Condition D) resulted in a lower level of data than Condition C. Lucas’ play


and engagement ranged from a low of 10% to a high of 50%, and produced similar play and<br />

engagement levels as in Condition B.<br />

6.5 Discussion<br />

The major purpose of Experiment III was to evaluate the effects of the musical<br />

adaptation of a childcare playground, more generally and more specifically, the effects of an<br />

individually designed music therapy intervention, on on-playground peer interactions among<br />

four young children diagnosed with ASD and their peers. An additional purpose was to<br />

determine whether the classroom teachers could implement the intervention into ongoing<br />

playground activities after staff development activities were provided, as well as whether the<br />

child’s peers could be trained to continue with the intervention.<br />

From this investigation an array of findings are worth noting. Prior to the musical<br />

adaptation of the playground, the data indicate that the target children with autism had hardly<br />

any positive peer interactions on the playground. The musical adaptation of the playground<br />

enhanced the target children’s positive peer interactions slightly, but not significantly. Two<br />

out of the four target children engaged in more meaningful play by using the musical<br />

equipment available in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut. The implementation of the teacher-mediated song<br />

intervention increased the children’s positive peer interactions immediately and significantly.<br />

As evidenced by the procedure fidelity data, with only one exception the teachers’ accuracy<br />

in implementing the intervention into the ongoing playground activities was very high.<br />

Furthermore, a correlation between correct implementation and the target children’s skill<br />

improvement was observed. Peers participated in the intervention and learned to facilitate<br />

241


242<br />

the song and play activity, although a minimum of ongoing mediation and encouragement by<br />

the children’s teachers was necessary. The peer-mediated intervention resulted in greater<br />

positive peer interactions compared to baseline and the adaptation of the playground, but was<br />

less effective than the teacher-mediated intervention, even though peers’ accuracy in<br />

implementing the intervention was fairly high. Both the teacher-mediated and peer-mediated<br />

song intervention enhanced the target children’s play and engagement on the playground.<br />

However, for this study, ongoing music therapy collaborative consultation was necessary. In<br />

addition to these overarching findings, a series of observations, no less important to the<br />

effectiveness of this inquiry, were noteworthy. These findings are enumerated in the<br />

following passages, broken down by child subject.<br />

Eric. The results of the study indicate that Eric’s positive peer interactions on the<br />

playground were modest (M=5.9%), whereas his play and engagement with toys and<br />

materials was rather high (M=52.7%). With the adaptation of the playground, positive peer<br />

interaction initially increased (M=16.7), while his play and engagement remained at the same<br />

level as in the previous condition (M=52.8). In playground observations, novelty of the<br />

<strong>Music</strong> Hut captured most children’s attention and motivated them to explore and play the<br />

instruments. And because Eric was consistently curious and interested in his peers’<br />

playground activities, he followed his classmates to the <strong>Music</strong> Hut where positive<br />

interactions occurred while playing the instruments. When over time a smaller number of<br />

children played in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut, Eric returned to his previously preferred solely playground<br />

activities (e.g., riding a tricycle). The introduction of the song intervention by Eric’s<br />

classroom teacher produced a significant increase in positive peer interactions (M=48.8%).


Due to the actions that are part of the song intervention, Eric’s play and engagement with the<br />

material in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut increased slightly (M= 63.3%). Eric’s tendency to return to well-<br />

known playground activities, this as soon as fewer children participated in singing and<br />

playing in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut, led to some modifications to how the song was presented. It was<br />

hypothesized that he would stay engaged in playing with peers longer if more children were<br />

engaged and more structure and predictability were given. Accordingly, Eric’s teacher took<br />

his entire class to the <strong>Music</strong> Hut, his unique song was sung with a 6.5 minute sing along<br />

recording, and a predictable sequence of the instruments used for the progression of the song<br />

was followed. However, Eric still needed to be redirected by his teacher to facilitate<br />

interactions with peers. Additionally, after the third session of the teacher-mediated<br />

intervention phase, Eric was sick for two consecutive weeks. When he returned to school, he<br />

seemed to be weak and unstable, and had great difficulty following familiar classroom<br />

routines. This absence resulted in a temporary decrease in his positive peer interactions,<br />

which increased immediately in the next session. After the sixth session, Eric’s positive peer<br />

interaction dropped to a lower level for all remaining intervention sessions. As already<br />

mentioned, the peer-mediated intervention was not implemented due to the classroom<br />

teacher’s time commitment prior to the study.<br />

Eric’s formally selected “peer-buddies,” Becky and Ron, learned the song prior to the<br />

intervention and were able to sing it independently. Both children were highly motivated to<br />

engage in the song intervention and interact with Eric for a total of four to five minutes at a<br />

time. Subsequently, they wanted to go about their play and interact with other friends on the<br />

243


244<br />

playground, while Eric’s teacher encouraged other peers playing in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut to interact<br />

with him.<br />

Eric’s lead teacher finally implemented all sessions. As the procedural fidelity data<br />

shows she performed the tasks mediated during the staff development activities on a<br />

moderate level (M=48%). After the fifth session, her task behavior dropped to one step only<br />

and stayed at a very low level for the entire intervention phase. It is very likely that it is for<br />

this reason that Eric’s positive peer interactions decreased. Eric’s teacher expressed some<br />

frustration with “peer buddies” leaving the <strong>Music</strong> Hut and with redirecting Eric to a<br />

structured playground activity (versus a free choice of play). This teacher’s decrease in<br />

accuracy in implementing the intervention might be owed to the challenge of keeping both<br />

Eric and the other children engaged in the activity, and to her belief that playground time<br />

should be a free-play setting in which peers have a free choice of play and peer interactions<br />

occur naturally. That Eric’s peer interactions do not occur naturally has been demonstrated<br />

by the baseline data. Clearly, teachers’ attitudes and beliefs have a major impact on<br />

providing successful learning experiences for students with autism in inclusive settings. This<br />

observation is consistent with previous studies, which evaluated the influence of teachers’<br />

attitudes and beliefs on successful inclusion (e.g., Mulvihill, Shearer, & Van Horn, 2002;<br />

Odom & Diamond, 1998), and is consistent with findings from previous studies providing<br />

evidence that interventions are necessary to produce increases in social interaction in children<br />

with autism (e.g., Myles et al., 1993).


What is more, Eric improved his vocalization by singing along parts of the song. He<br />

imitated his peers in playing the instruments and took turns in playing. He frequently<br />

expressed dislikes vocally when he wanted to go about his play, but he also expressed joy<br />

and happiness. After the intervention, playing instruments while vocalizing parts of the song<br />

(e.g., “Hey you!”) were some of his preferred playground activities.<br />

Video Excerpt 6.5. Eric’s performance during the teacher-mediated intervention<br />

phase of the playground interaction study.<br />

Ben. During the baseline condition, the data indicate that Ben had almost no positive<br />

peer interactions on the playground (M=1.4%), and he was modestly involved in meaningful<br />

play with toys and playground materials (M= 13.6%). With the musical adaptation of the<br />

playground, his play and engagement with toys and materials was slightly greater than in the<br />

baseline condition, but his play activities changed (M=20.8%). Ben frequently played the<br />

instruments in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut while vocalizing familiar songs. Every so often, positive peer<br />

interaction occurred when peers joined in his play (e.g. manipulating the ocean drum),<br />

resulting in a negligible increase in peer interactions (M=2.7%). The introduction of the song<br />

intervention by Ben’s classroom teacher produced a tremendous and immediate increase in<br />

positive peer interactions (M=77.5%). Owing to the design of the song intervention, his play<br />

and engagement with materials in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut increased significantly (M= 95.0%). His<br />

teacher performed the tasks, learned during the staff development activities, on a very high<br />

level (M=96.0%), and was flexible in adjusting the intervention to the needs of the children.<br />

After the sixth session of the teacher-mediated intervention phase, Ben, his family and his<br />

245


246<br />

teachers faced some challenging moments. Ben was absent from school for four consecutive<br />

weeks because of sickness, followed by a two-week winter break. Shortly afterwards, his<br />

medication changed, and his secondary caregiver left. During this time, Ben had difficulty<br />

following familiar classroom routines, and absenteeism and variations in his mood (e.g.,<br />

being needy, cranky, whinny) were observed. During the <strong>Music</strong> Hut activity, his classroom<br />

teacher needed to give physical and verbal prompts, as she had in the beginning of the<br />

teacher-mediated intervention phase.<br />

Ben’s formally selected “peer-buddies,” Carmen and Victor, learned the unique song<br />

prior to the intervention, and were able to sing Ben’s song independently. Field notes point<br />

out that during the teacher-mediated intervention, both children were very motivated in<br />

participating in the song and play activity in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut, and expressed their joy in<br />

singing and playing the instruments with one another. However, during the long period of<br />

the teacher-mediated intervention (11 weeks), Ben’s “peer buddies” lost interest in this<br />

admittedly repetitive intervention procedure. For the sake of variation, and to motivate them<br />

to continue to participate to participate, the classroom teacher altered the presentation of the<br />

song (e.g., Ben’s whole class played in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut, pretending to be a rock band<br />

accompanying the song), and gave more verbal praise. The peer-mediated intervention<br />

resulted in a slightly lower level of peer interaction than the previous condition (M=66.9%).<br />

The unsupported peer-mediated intervention, in which no teacher’s support was given,<br />

resulted in significantly fewer positive peer interactions, but was still above the baseline<br />

condition and the adaptation of the playground condition (M=17.3%). As evidenced by the<br />

procedural fidelity data, Carmen and Victor performed the tasks on a high level (M=72%).


Both children saw the song intervention as a joint mission to train Ben how to sign, sing and<br />

play the song in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut. During the peer-mediated intervention, Carmen and Victor<br />

came up with an effective self-motivator. They proposed to record the “Song for Ben,” and<br />

to produce their own CD after practicing with Ben for many more days. However,<br />

difficulties in reading Ben’s cues and with communication caused non-negligible frustration<br />

among peers and required interpretation and mediation by their classroom teacher. For<br />

example, Carmen and Victor made comments such as “He doesn’t want to play with me,”<br />

when Ben did not immediately respond with the desired action at hand (e.g., taking a drum<br />

stick), with eye contact or with verbal language. Over time, peers became more persistent in<br />

interacting with Ben, but verification and reassurance from their teacher was still necessary.<br />

Therefore, the teacher’s support was never fully withdrawn.<br />

There was also a number of major improvements in Ben that are important to<br />

highlight. During the teacher-mediated and peer-mediated intervention, Ben improved his<br />

communication and imitation skills, his awareness and interest in peers, and his play<br />

activities (including choice-making). Ben was able to stay and play in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut with<br />

his peers for 10 minutes at a time. He sang the complete song verbally, and signed “You,”<br />

“I,” and “<strong>Music</strong>.” Ben imitated his peers, and learned to pass a drumstick to his peers<br />

whenever the song required it. No teacher’s prompt was necessary. He chose different<br />

instruments independently, approached peers, and followed verbal prompts of his teachers<br />

and peers. Ben also expressed joy and happiness by smiling and jumping up and down.<br />

247


248<br />

To support and motivate Ben’s family during challenging periods, video excerpts of<br />

Ben’s positive peer interaction at school were given to the family. Ben’s parents expressed<br />

that they were delighted to see their son making progress and interacting with other children.<br />

Ben’s parents shared the video recording with family members, Ben’s home program<br />

therapists, and future kindergarten teachers. After evaluating the intervention with Ben’s<br />

teacher, she came to the following conclusion:<br />

The intervention was fun, easy and very effective. I was pleased, astonished and<br />

touched by the clinical progress Ben made. The intervention was a good learning<br />

process for myself. During the teacher-mediated intervention, I realized that I don’t<br />

have to be in Ben’s face all the time and make him do things.<br />

Video Excerpt 6.6. Ben’s performance during the teacher and peer-mediated<br />

intervention phase of the playground interaction study.<br />

Phillip. Positive peer interactions on the playground occurred very infrequently for<br />

Phillip (M=4.1%), and his play and engagement with toys and materials was moderate (M=<br />

20%). When the <strong>Music</strong> Hut was accessible, there were modest increases in positive peer<br />

interactions, with increases becoming more marked toward the end of the phase (M=6.3%).<br />

His play and engagement with toys and materials on the playground was on a slightly lower<br />

level than baseline condition, but his playground activities changed (M=15.3%). Phillip<br />

explored the instruments (e.g., feeling the vibration of the cymbal) and playfully chased his<br />

peers. What was initially positive contact turned into negative peer interaction as play<br />

continued. For example, peers were teasing him and Phillip started to show aggressive<br />

behaviors toward them. Frequently, teachers needed to facilitate conflict resolution. This<br />

behavior was not observed when he engaged in activities in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut. The introduction


of the song intervention by Phillip’s teacher produced a tremendous and immediate increase<br />

in positive peer interactions (M=77.4%). Due to the actions incorporated in the song his play<br />

and engagement also increased (M=71.4%). Phillip’s teacher performed the tasks on a very<br />

high level (M=98.0%). She was very persistent in performing exactly the tasks mediated in<br />

the staff development activities. From the seventh to the thirteenth session of the teacher-<br />

mediated intervention phase, Phillip was absent for a short period of time. He showed<br />

interest in the music by listening or playing the instruments, but did not respond to his<br />

teacher or peers. Toward the end of the teacher-mediated intervention, his positive peer<br />

interaction increased and the <strong>Music</strong> Hut routine became part of his playground activities.<br />

Phillip’s formally selected “peer-buddy,” Justin, learned the song prior to the<br />

intervention, and Jacky, who was new to the class, picked it up on the playground. Both<br />

children were able to sing Phillip’s unique song independently. During the teacher-mediated<br />

intervention, Justin was very motivated and persistent in participating in the intervention.<br />

Jacky was interested in interacting with Phillip. However, over time, her motivation to<br />

volunteer in the repetitive procedure decreased. Repetition was not an issue for Justin<br />

(diagnosed with high functioning autism). In fact, when he was available, he requested to<br />

sing and play with Phillip in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut. The peer-mediated intervention resulted in a<br />

slightly lower level of positive peer interaction than the previous condition (M=70.3%). The<br />

unsupported peer-mediated intervention, in which no teacher’s support was given, resulted in<br />

significantly fewer positive peer interactions, but was still above the baseline condition and<br />

the adaptation of the playground condition (M=21.9%). Phillip’s “peer buddies” performed<br />

the tasks on a very high level (M=98%). Both children were very persistent in following the<br />

249


250<br />

routine. Jacky was able to get Phillip’s attention easily. She was consistent, insistent, and<br />

direct in her actions, but also in singing and playing the song. She was able to follow<br />

Phillip’s lead or redirect him to the song activity, when he was distracted. Justin had the<br />

same qualities, but was not flexible in following Phillip’s lead and got distracted when<br />

Phillip did not follow the developed <strong>Music</strong> Hut routine. In these situations, Justin needed the<br />

teacher’s guidance. Justin’s behavior is characteristic of the restricted and repetitive<br />

behavior of autism (American Psychiatric Association, 2000; Dawson & Osterling, 1997;<br />

National Research Council, 2001). As a result, the teacher never fully withdrew her support.<br />

The following array of observations is also worth noting. Phillip frequently initiated<br />

playing the instruments in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut. Phillip learned to observe, listen and imitate<br />

playing the musical instruments. He also learned to wait for his turn to play. For example,<br />

he often smiled happily and waited for Justin to hand him over the drumstick for his turn. In<br />

addition, Phillip improved his language and communication skills. For example, sometimes<br />

he initiated dancing by taking Justin’s hand or signing “dance.” Phillip also started to request<br />

the song by singing the song’s opening line “I want.” When his teacher paused in singing, he<br />

filled in longer phrases of the lyrics as well as names of peers or his name. Phillip used<br />

appropriate body contact when tickling or hugging each other as a good-bye gesture. Phillip<br />

also used sign language in combination with words for “All done!” “Good-bye!” and “Thank<br />

you!” to end playing together in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut.<br />

It should also be noted that other peers on the playground, particularly those with<br />

special needs, came forward to participate in the teacher-mediated intervention, too. Phillip’s


teacher integrated them into the procedures and mediated peer interaction with Phillip.<br />

Additionally, a younger child who was anxious, but interested in interacting with Phillip, was<br />

observing him during the <strong>Music</strong> Hut activities. He overcame his fears and joined in dancing<br />

and playing with Phillip and even hugged him. In general, Phillip seemed to have fewer<br />

difficulties on the playground and frequently joined peer groups when playing in the <strong>Music</strong><br />

Hut. After the study, teachers reported that Justin and Phillip continued to sing and play<br />

together in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut, but for a shorter period of time. However, these observations fall<br />

outside the scope of this data collection period.<br />

Phillip’s mother, who observed her son’s developmental progress during the<br />

playground intervention on video recordings, evaluated the song intervention as a success.<br />

She reported sharing the song with Phillip’s home program therapist, who used it as reward<br />

when he accomplished tasks.<br />

After the intervention with Phillip, his teacher made the following comment:<br />

I have had a wonderful time working with Phillip in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut. This was a fun an<br />

effective intervention. The best part for me was having Phillip look up at me with his<br />

big brown eyes in wonder while I sang to him.<br />

Video Excerpt 6.7. Phillip’s performance during the teacher and peer-mediated<br />

intervention phase of the playground interaction study.<br />

Lucas. Per the data presented here, Lucas had almost no positive peer interaction on<br />

the playground (M=1.5%) at baseline, and his play and engagement with toys and materials<br />

251


252<br />

was very low (M= 4.0). With the musical adaptation of the playground his play and<br />

engagement was greater than in the baseline condition, and his activities changed (M=18%).<br />

Lucas frequently ran in and out of the <strong>Music</strong> Hut, especially when peers produced sounds.<br />

He also explored instruments (e.g., manipulated the ocean drum and cabasa, explored<br />

vibration of the gong), or tapped a beat on one of the wooden beams of the <strong>Music</strong> Hut while<br />

vocalizing loudly. Most of the positive peer interactions occurred accidentally while playing<br />

in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut, which resulted in a very small increase in peer interaction (M=2.5%). The<br />

introduction of the song intervention by Lucas’ classroom teachers produced a tremendous<br />

and immediate increase in positive peer interactions, especially during the second half of the<br />

teacher-mediated intervention (M=61%). Additionally, his play and engagement with<br />

materials in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut rose significantly (M=40.7%). His teachers performed the tasks<br />

on a very high level (M=94%). The lead teacher was creative and flexible in using the<br />

provided materials and training. The assistant teacher for her part, was very consistent in<br />

performing exactly the tasks mediated during staff development activities. In the beginning<br />

of the intervention Lucas didn’t have a concept of the activities and procedure. But over<br />

time, learning took place. During the second part of the teacher-mediated intervention phase,<br />

Lucas participated in the activities without trying to escape from the activity.<br />

Kayla and Megan, Lucas’ formally selected “peer buddies” learned the song prior to<br />

the intervention and were able to sing it independently. On the second day of the teacher-<br />

mediated intervention, Kayla played the gong very loudly and persistently. Lucas was most<br />

likely over-stimulated and tried to escape. Escaping and over-stimulation were the major<br />

challenges his teachers and peers had to face during the song intervention in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut.


Teachers and peers met him on his high energy level by using the following strategies<br />

offering fast-changing sequences to keep him engaged, making fast and smooth transitions to<br />

avoid waiting times, including his escapes in the intervention by adding a verse on running<br />

after him and coming back to the <strong>Music</strong> Hut. Due to Kayla’s late arrival at school, her<br />

participation was inconsistent. As the intervention progressed, other children besides Megan<br />

– especially those with special needs – were interested and got involved in participating in<br />

Lucas’ <strong>Music</strong> Hut activities. During the peer-mediated intervention, Megan and Lucas<br />

usually continued singing and playing in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut. The positive peer interaction was on<br />

a slightly higher level than the previous condition (M=71.6%). The unsupported peer-<br />

mediated intervention, in which no teacher’s support was given, resulted in significantly<br />

fewer positive peer interactions, but was still above the baseline condition and the adaptation<br />

of the playground condition (M=24.1%). Lucas’ peers performed the tasks on a high level<br />

(M=86%). However, difficulties in reading Lucas’ cues, coupled with his poor language<br />

skills, required ongoing teacher support. Over time, peers became more persistent in<br />

interacting with Lucas.<br />

In terms of qualitative statements concerning these measures, the following major<br />

improvements should be noted. Lucas was able to share his attention with peers, look at<br />

peers, manipulate the instruments in a meaningful way, choose instruments to play with and<br />

participate in turn-taking when assisted by teachers. Lucas seemed to enjoy movements and<br />

accept body contact when interacting with peers. He reduced scratching and hitting<br />

behaviors, and learned to hug and tickle peers. Often, he expressed pleasure and joy while<br />

interacting by laughing, smiling and vocalizing. Lucas was able to stay and play in the<br />

253


254<br />

<strong>Music</strong> Hut with his peers for 10 minutes at a time. A positive outcome of the intervention<br />

study is also evident in the following lead teacher’s statement: “I love music therapy<br />

research studies, because they are fun and make a lot of sense to me and the children.”<br />

Video Excerpt 6.8. Lucas performance during the teacher and peer-mediated<br />

intervention phase of the playground interaction study.<br />

In summary, the design and musical adaptation of the playground had some<br />

positive effects on the target children’s play behaviors on the playground. All of the<br />

children were attracted by the sound produced by peers in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut and explored<br />

the instruments for short periods of time. However, the musical equipment itself did not<br />

produce the desired outcomes regarding the children’s social interactions with peers.<br />

This observation is consistent with a previous study that evaluated the effects of adding<br />

musical equipment to a childcare playground to increase the social interaction with peers<br />

for a young child with visual impairment (Kern & Wolery, 2001, 2002), studies<br />

investigating the efficacy of ecological variations for increasing social interaction of<br />

young children with autism (as reviewed in McConnell, 2002), and is also described in<br />

the literature related to playground activities for children with special needs (e.g., Nabors<br />

et al., 2001). More specifically, adaptations to the environment, combined with<br />

individualized interventions, are needed to achieve desired therapeutic goals.<br />

Nevertheless, the attraction of the <strong>Music</strong> Hut, the proximity to peers, and the opportunity<br />

to engage in joint musical activities (e.g., by sharing the materials) facilitated the<br />

involvement and motivation of the children with autism to interact with peers on the


playground. This finding is consistent with other investigations describing that preschool<br />

children with autism interact more with on another when doing activities they enjoy<br />

(Koegel, Dyer, & Bell, 1987), and more social interaction might be attributed to structure<br />

and access itself (DeKlyen & Odom, 1989). However, these findings expand upon<br />

previous studies, showing that music therapy interventions can facilitate acquisition of<br />

social skills in the context of the target children’s natural environment such as the<br />

playground. Therefore playgrounds are important settings for music therapy<br />

interventions.<br />

Informal observation during this study showed that typically developing children and<br />

children with special needs other than autism also benefited from the intervention. The<br />

<strong>Music</strong> Hut provided opportunities to socialize, to develop sensory-motor and cognitive skills,<br />

to engage in self-expression and communication skills, to create games and to benefit from<br />

the enjoyment of music. In childcare programs using an integrated therapy approach, in<br />

which therapists provide services to children with special needs within the context of the<br />

child’s natural environment, as well as her or his routines and activities (Mackey &<br />

McQueen, 1997; McWilliam, 1996, 2000), the playground should be seen as an<br />

educational/therapeutic environment for promoting skills in each developmental domain.<br />

The natural interest children have in music, the inherent opportunities in outdoor play, and<br />

the engagement that comes from being creative, should be seen as a chance to enhance<br />

learning and development for both children with and without special needs.<br />

255


256<br />

The unique songs matching each target child’s personality and incorporating specific<br />

therapeutic goals produced desirable outcomes. This finding is consistent with earlier studies<br />

in which songs were used to assist individuals with autism to improve social skills (Stevens<br />

& Clark, 1969; Brownell, 2002). It also supports the clinical application of songs for skill<br />

development in early childhood intervention settings (e.g., Enoch, 2002; Furman, 2001;<br />

Humpal, 1998). However, previous studies did not examine the use of unique songs to<br />

increase positive peer interactions on inclusive childcare playgrounds for preschoolers with<br />

autism, nor were song interventions embedded by teachers and peers in ongoing playground<br />

activities. Contrary to Experiment I, the implementation of the song intervention increased<br />

the target children’s positive peer interaction immediately. Importantly, the general reaction<br />

of children with autism to be distressed when change occurs (American Psychiatric<br />

Association, 2000; Dawson & Osterling, 1997; National Research Council, 2001) was not<br />

observed.<br />

Further, the songs also had social value and provided a step toward inclusion, both for<br />

the target children and their families as well as for peers and their families. The songs were<br />

sung with siblings and extended families and facilitated the sharing of the children’s social<br />

experiences from school. These responses are consistent with reports made by other music<br />

therapists regarding the meaning and power of unique songs in the client’s social<br />

environment (e.g., Aasgaard, 1999; Ricciarelli, 2003). As Carmen and Victor suggested,<br />

Ben’s and Phillip’s class recorded the songs “You and I” and “Phillip’s Groove.” After the<br />

study, a CD with an individualized label was handed out to the class. Carmen’s parents<br />

responded in an email with the following statement:


257<br />

We believe your work with the kids is a tremendous success. When Carmen first<br />

notified us that she was going to record a CD, we said, "Carmen, what are you<br />

talking about?” She responded, “We are going to record a CD with Petra.” You<br />

should have seen the joy that was exuberating from her. After we heard the CD, we<br />

thought “WOW, these kids did a great job,” and how wonderful it was that you and<br />

the teachers explained to the kids what the instruments were and how to play them.<br />

We were so in awe that we made copies of the CD and gave them to our parents and<br />

close relatives. Everyone enjoyed them and said how great the CD was. Every now<br />

and then, when I talk with my parents they recite some of the songs on the CD, ‘play<br />

the cymbals, play the cymbals.’ We all just laugh and reflect on how well this song<br />

was sung on the CD and how the kids seemed to enjoy it so much. I feel that the<br />

songs and the instruments were more than just singing and playing but were also a<br />

lesson on how to work together and how music is for everyone. We definitely<br />

appreciate what it has done for Carmen. Thanks.<br />

And, Justin’s mother responded in an Email with the following comment:<br />

After receiving the CD, I slipped it into the CD player in the car on the way home<br />

from school without telling Justin. When the music came on, he got very excited and<br />

yelled, "Hey that's my class!" When the track of him singing "I want to play the drum<br />

with you" came on, he yelled, "Hey that's me!" and "Hey, that's Petra!" He was<br />

fascinated with hearing himself sing on the CD and asked me to play it about a<br />

hundred times. It sparked great conversations about his experience in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut<br />

and he taught me, my husband, and his little brother how to sing the song. I kept the<br />

CD in the car and for the first few weeks, he asked to listen to it over and over again<br />

on the way to and from school. Each time he heard "his song," his face would light<br />

and he'd yell, "Hey that's me!” And "Hey that's Petra!" It became known in our house<br />

as "Justin's song" and even his brother would request that I play "Justin's song,"<br />

Having the CD was a great way for him to share his experience with us.<br />

Teachers embedded the intervention successfully in the ongoing playground routine<br />

as evidenced by the target children’s skill improvements. This finding is consistent with the<br />

results of Experiment I and II, and expand upon earlier studies related to other disciplines<br />

(Garfinkel & Schwartz, 2002; Kaiser et al., 1993; Kemmis & Dunn, 1996; Sewell et al.,<br />

1998; Venn et al., 1993; Wolery et al., 2002) by showing that teachers can successfully<br />

implement interventions based on music therapy principles and guidelines in ongoing<br />

childcare routines when staff training and ongoing consultation is provided. As the


258<br />

procedural fidelity data show, with only one exception, the teachers’ accuracy in<br />

implementing the intervention was very high. Clearly, the level of teacher fidelity to this<br />

intervention is closely associated with its effect on positive interactions between children<br />

with autism and their peers.<br />

An important implication of this study is that high-quality staff training and ongoing<br />

collaborative consultation by a certified music therapist is crucial for appropriate and<br />

successful implementation of teacher-mediated interventions using music therapy principles.<br />

In addition, teacher attitudes and beliefs do have a major impact on successfully embedding<br />

interventions based on music therapy within childcare routines. There is a need for music<br />

therapy-based in-services and staff training to ensure the sharing of professional knowledge,<br />

beliefs and attitudes, as well as the teachers’ comfort level. This statement is also supported<br />

in the music therapy literature (e.g., Chester et al, 1999; Furman, 2002; Snell, 2002;<br />

Warwick, 1995). Overall, it can be concluded that the music therapy collaborative<br />

consultative approach was effective in enabling teachers to implement the intervention<br />

successfully in ongoing playground routines.<br />

For clinical application, it is vital to establish a functional relationship between the<br />

teachers and music therapist when using an integrated therapy approach. Both the teachers<br />

and music therapist share equal responsibility in implementing the intervention successfully.<br />

In consultative methods, it is the music therapist’s responsibility to provide and share<br />

professional knowledge with the teachers to meet the needs of the child. To successfully<br />

embed music therapy interventions in inclusive preschool settings, it is essential to respect


the teacher’s individual working style, be flexible and spontaneous both in scheduling and<br />

implementing interventions to communicate clearly and frequently, and to provide<br />

enthusiasm and motivation. It is also imperative to take the teachers’ fears and concerns<br />

around singing and playing instruments seriously in order to let the music become a natural<br />

activity within classroom routines.<br />

The peer-mediated strategy was effective in increasing the target children’s social<br />

interactions and meaningful play on the playground. The songs were easily learned by the<br />

“peer buddies” and other classmates, and facilitated social involvement between the peers<br />

and the children with autism. Peers’ accuracy in implementing the intervention was fairly<br />

high. This finding is consistent with earlier research showing that peer-mediated strategies to<br />

improve the social functioning of children with autism were effectively applied (DiSalvo &<br />

Oswald, 2002; Goldstein et al., 1992; McGee et al., 1992; Strain et al., 1979). This study<br />

expounds upon previous studies by showing that peer-mediated strategies can also be applied<br />

on playgrounds and through joint musical activities. However, contrary to previous studies<br />

including children with disabilities other than autism in natural environments other than<br />

playgrounds (e.g., Odom et al., 1992), teachers’ support was never fully withdrawn in this<br />

intervention. A minimum of ongoing mediation and encouragement was needed from<br />

teachers. A major challenge of the participating peers was the interpretation of the<br />

unconventional and unpredictable behaviors and communications styles, or the lack of<br />

response of the children with autism. Another challenge was differences in pace and interest<br />

of play on the playground. Overall, the freedom and variety of play opportunities inherent in<br />

outdoor play, and the chance to engage in creative play activities with friends or siblings<br />

259


260<br />

enrolled in other classrooms, makes the playground a more challenging environment to<br />

implement structured peer-mediated activities. Consequently, strategies for how to engage<br />

the child with autism in play, and how to interpret the behavior and different communication<br />

styles in addition to positive reinforcement, need to be given by the teachers at all time.<br />

These findings support the proposed clinical guidelines for encouraging peer interactions and<br />

inclusion of children with special needs on playgrounds. More specifically, teacher-directed<br />

activities are necessary to increase social experiences and involvement of children with<br />

special needs on playgrounds (Nabors & Badawi, 1997; Nabors et al., 2001; Stine, 1997;<br />

Wortham & Wortham, 1989), especially for children with limited language and<br />

communication skills (Odom, 2002).<br />

A potential implication of this study is that shorter periods of structured peer-<br />

mediated activities (i.e., five minutes at a time) should be used on playgrounds.<br />

Additionally, a greater variety of musical activities might motivate typically developing peers<br />

to engage in peer-mediated strategies over a longer period of time. To maximize the positive<br />

effects of peer-mediated strategies on playgrounds, a variety of peer-mediated strategies, and<br />

a stronger managing and fading reinforcement systems should be applied.<br />

This study has specific limitations. For example, only a small number of participants<br />

were involved. In order to understand the generalization of the intervention, replication with<br />

other young children with autism would be desirable. Further, for several reasons (e.g.,<br />

inclement weather, children’s absence, field trips, winter break), the data were not collected<br />

on a daily basis for each child. Thus, the study extended over several months because of the


above conditions and the nature of the experimental design. Another limitation, shared with<br />

Experiments I, is that this experiment was not a comparison study. We do not know if other<br />

interventions would have been equally effective, more effective, or less effective in<br />

increasing the social interchanges among children with autism and their peers on<br />

playgrounds. From this study it cannot be assumed that other teachers are willing or able to<br />

implement interventions based on music therapy principles and guidelines. Nor do we know<br />

if other “peer buddies” would have participated equally or more effectively in increasing the<br />

target children’s positive interaction and play and engagement on the playground.<br />

Several issues should be considered in future studies. For example, more research is<br />

clearly warranted to provide further validity to musical adaptations of a childcare<br />

playground. Research should investigate the use of an outdoor music center as a<br />

motivational environment for learning and development for both children with and without<br />

special needs. More intervention studies that apply music therapy on playgrounds to foster<br />

social experiences and other therapeutic goals for children with special needs, especially<br />

those with autism are needed.<br />

To recapitulate, this study was not a comparison study. Therefore, it is not known if<br />

more familiar songs (as the results of Experiment II suggest) or other interventions would<br />

have been equally, more, or less effective. These considerations lie beyond the scope of this<br />

inquiry, however, and more studies are needed that compare the effects of a variety of songs<br />

and other interventions on social skill improvements of young children with autism on<br />

261


262<br />

playgrounds. Studies are also needed to investigate which components of the music therapy<br />

intervention result in the positive outcome.<br />

Maintenance and generalization will be important features of future investigations.<br />

From this study, we do not know whether or not peers maintain and generalize playing with<br />

their classmates with autism. A major research question is how many trained peers are<br />

necessary to sustain the intervention and which managing and fading reinforcement systems<br />

works best on playgrounds. Future research should evaluate the effects of a variety of peer-<br />

mediated strategies (e.g., DiSalvo & Oswald, 2002) on playgrounds, and examine the target<br />

children’s skill generalization in different natural environments.<br />

In the study at hand, continuing collaborative consultation was necessary. It is not<br />

know, whether the teachers’ accurate implementation of the interventions will continue or if<br />

it will decrease with lack of supervision. It is also unknown if implementation of the<br />

intervention by a trained music therapist increase the participant’s acquisition of independent<br />

functioning. More research illustrating the benefits of the collaborative consultative model<br />

of service delivery in early intervention/early childhood special education is needed with<br />

specific emphasis on music therapy.


7. General Discussion<br />

This cumulative study endeavored to evaluate the effects of embedded music therapy<br />

interventions using songs and a musical outdoor center on the functioning of young children<br />

with autism during the morning greeting routine, specific multiple-step tasks and peer<br />

interactions on the playground in an inclusive childcare program. It also examines whether<br />

or not teachers can implement the interventions after staff development activities are<br />

provided, and whether or not peer-mediated strategies to increase interactions on the<br />

playground are effective.<br />

All told, there are seven overarching findings that usher from this series of studies.<br />

First, and foremost all children increase their independent performance within the childcare<br />

routine. Through music, target children acquire new skills and/or improve in key deficit<br />

areas of autism, such as transitioning, following multiple-step tasks, and social interactions<br />

with peers. These findings are consistent with the reports from numerous anecdotal case<br />

studies in music therapy, in which interventions were applied to improve core difficulties in<br />

children with autism (Allgood, 2002; Alvin & Warwick, 1991; Clarkson, 1991; Gustdoff &<br />

Neugebauer, 1997; Howat, 1995; Nordoff-Robbins, 1977; Schuhmacher, 1994; Snell, 2002).<br />

Moreover, they replicate other studies showing that children with autism acquire social skills<br />

when music therapy is provided in segregated settings by trained music therapists (Brownell,<br />

2002; Edgerton, 1994; Schuhmacher, 1999; Stevens & Clark, 1969; Wimpory et al., 1995).<br />

263


264<br />

The studies presented here expand upon previous studies by showing that acquisition<br />

of social and adaptive skills occurs when the interventions are embedded in the context of the<br />

target children’s natural environment. <strong>Music</strong> therapy interventions also facilitate a smooth<br />

transition during the morning greeting routine and enable children with autism to follow<br />

specific multiple-step tasks. Thus, as recommended by the Division of Early Childhood<br />

(DEC) (Sandall et al., 2000) and by the Committee on Educational Interventions for Children<br />

with Autism (National Research Council, 2001), embedding interventions in ongoing<br />

classroom routines is supported. Further research should investigate whether or not music<br />

therapy interventions embedded in inclusive preschool routines can be applied for<br />

development of other skills and if they are helpful for children with special needs other than<br />

autism.<br />

A second major finding is that, in eight of nine cases, the songs produce desirable<br />

outcomes. These findings are consistent with strategies that suggest the use of songs for<br />

smooth transitioning with children with autism (Baker, 1992; Cole-Currens, 1993; Furman,<br />

2001; Gottschewski, 2001; Williams, 1996) and provide evidence that greeting songs can be<br />

effective during one of the most crucial childcare transitions, the morning arrival time. They<br />

also replicate earlier studies in which music was used to assist individuals with memorization<br />

of sequence of information (Gfeller, 1983; Jellison & Miller, 1982; Wolfe & Horn, 1993), as<br />

well as to improve social skills (Stevens & Clark, 1969; Brownell, 2002). However, previous<br />

studies had not examined the use of songs with children with autism for transitioning and<br />

memorization of multiple-step tasks, nor were the studies embedded in an inclusive childcare<br />

program. Yet once again, two of the three studies were not comparison studies, so. It is not


known if other songs or other interventions would have been equally effective or more so.<br />

Only the multiple-step tasks study provided evidence that the songs were more effective than<br />

the verbal presentation of sequencing, except for one task where the verbal presentation was<br />

more (albeit not significantly) effective. More studies are needed that compare the effects of<br />

a variety of songs and other interventions on the behavior of young children with autism.<br />

A third finding centers on therapeutic effects that resulted from the design and<br />

musical adaptation of the playground in Experiment III. Specifically, two out of the four<br />

target children engaged in more meaningful play by using the musical instruments available<br />

in the outdoor music center. However, no significant changes occurred in social interactions<br />

with peers. Thus, at least for these participants, the combination of the musical playground<br />

equipment and individualized interventions were needed to obtain the desired outcomes.<br />

This was the same conclusion reached in another study of playground modifications (Kern &<br />

Wolery, 2001, 2002) and is described in the literature (e.g. Nabors et al., 2001). That is,<br />

adaptations to the environment and teacher-mediated interventions are often necessary to<br />

achieve desired outcomes. However, the musical outdoor environment facilitated the<br />

involvement and motivation of children with autism to interact with peers on the playground.<br />

Informal observation during this study reveals that, on many occasions, other children with<br />

special needs are attracted by the sounds of the instruments and become involved in musical<br />

activities in the outdoor music center.<br />

In general, the playground needs to be seen as an educational/therapeutic<br />

environment. More studies that apply music therapy on playgrounds, both to children with<br />

265


266<br />

and without disabilities, are needed. Further research should likewise investigate the use of<br />

the outdoor music center as a motivation and natural learning environment for children with<br />

special needs to accomplish IEP goals.<br />

The fourth finding surrounds the collaborative consultative approach to music<br />

therapy, and its effectiveness in enabling teachers to successfully implement the interventions<br />

in ongoing childcare activities and routines. Teachers embed interventions successfully<br />

during the morning greeting routine, specific multiple-step tasks, and on the playground, as<br />

shown by the target children’s skill improvements. This replicates and extends earlier studies<br />

showing that teachers can successfully implement interventions related to other disciplines in<br />

ongoing activities successfully when staff training (e.g., literature, lectures, modeling, role-<br />

playing, feedback) and monitoring is provided (Garfinkel & Schwartz, 2002; Kaiser et al.,<br />

1993; Kemmis & Dunn, 1996; Sewell et al., 1998; Venn et al., 1993; Wolery et al., 2002). In<br />

previous studies, however, interventions were not based on music therapy principles and<br />

guidelines.<br />

This series of studies adds to previous findings by showing that individualized<br />

interventions based on music therapy principles can be incorporated by teachers in usual<br />

classroom and playground routines, when staff development activities and ongoing<br />

consultation is provided. With only one exception, the teachers’ accuracy in implementing<br />

the intervention was very high, as evidenced by the procedural fidelity data in Experiment<br />

III. Furthermore, there is a correlation between correct implementation and the target<br />

children’s skill improvement. High quality staff development activities and ongoing


collaborative consultation are critical components for appropriate and successful<br />

implementation of teacher-mediated interventions using music therapy principles. There is a<br />

need for music therapy-based staff development activities to ensure both the teachers’<br />

comfort level and the sharing of professional knowledge. There are several ways to provide<br />

musical training to teachers as an intensive one-to-one training, as part of the classroom<br />

curriculum including both children and teachers, as an in-service, or as a part of the<br />

university early childhood intervention curriculum. Best practice depends on the time<br />

available, on the childcare curriculum, and on classroom demands the teachers face.<br />

For these studies, ongoing collaborative consultation was necessary. We do not know<br />

whether or not the teachers’ accurate implementation of the interventions will continue or<br />

decline over several months if they are not supervised. We also do not know if the<br />

implementation of the intervention by a trained music therapist will result in equal, greater,<br />

or less children’s acquisition of independent functioning. More research supporting the<br />

efficacy of a music therapy collaborative consultative model of service delivery in early<br />

intervention/early childhood special education is needed.<br />

Toward this end, the fidelity ratings in this study introduce a preliminary measure of<br />

how effective the collaborative consultation component can be in imparting intervention<br />

partners with adequate tools for intervention sustainability. In this inquiry, only Experiment<br />

III contained a systematic process for monitoring the degree of teachers’ and peers’<br />

adherence to the intervention model; as a result, fidelity as a potential predictor/moderator of<br />

intervention success could only be analyzed in this one experiment. (In Experiments I and II,<br />

267


268<br />

adherence to the intervention model was far more categorical, laid out as it was in a series of<br />

concrete, methodical steps from which little if any departure was possible.) At this early<br />

stage of the literature, however, it is important to recognize the major finding that, in this one<br />

experiment where fidelity was taken into account, fidelity indeed seemed to correlate<br />

strongly with the level of children’s peer interactions. The implications of this finding for the<br />

legitimacy of the field of music therapy cannot be underestimated.<br />

Peer-mediated strategies, as used in Experiment III, are effective in increasing peer<br />

interactions and meaningful play on the playground, and thus constitute a fifth key finding in<br />

this inquiry. This finding is similar to that in earlier research, showing that peer-mediated<br />

strategies are a powerful means of improving social interaction of children with autism and<br />

their typically developing peers in the context of classroom activities (DiSalvo & Oswald,<br />

2002; Goldstein et al., 1992; McGee et al., 1992; Strain et al., 1979). This study expands<br />

upon previous studies by showing that peer-mediated strategies can also be applied on<br />

playgrounds. Contrary to some previous studies including children with other disabilities<br />

than autism (e.g., Odom et al., 1992), the teachers’ support was never fully withdrawn. A<br />

minimum of ongoing mediation and encouragement from teachers was needed.<br />

These findings support the proposed clinical guidelines for promoting peer interaction<br />

and inclusion of children with special needs on playgrounds. That is, teacher-directed<br />

activities are necessary to increase social experiences and involvement of children with<br />

special needs on playgrounds (Nabors & Badawi, 1997; Nabors et al., 2001; Stine, 1997;<br />

Wortham & Wortham, 1989). The “free choice” that is considered a natural part of outdoor


play, along with the variety of play opportunities compared to indoor play and the chance to<br />

meet and play with friends or siblings enrolled in other classrooms, makes the playground a<br />

more challenging environment to implement structured peer-mediated activities.<br />

A potential implication of this study is that shorter periods of peer-mediated<br />

interventions and stronger managing and fading reinforcement systems should be used to<br />

maximize the positive effects. Experiment III is limited with regard to maintenance and<br />

generalization. Although teachers reported some maintenance and generalization, no<br />

measures have been applied. Based on this study it cannot be said, whether or not peers<br />

maintain playing with the target children once the intervention is no longer in place. Future<br />

research should evaluate maintenance and skill generalization, and examine the effects of a<br />

variety of peer-mediated strategies with children with autism (e.g., DiSalvo & Oswald, 2002)<br />

on playgrounds.<br />

A sixth finding is that, the song interventions provided a valuable step towards<br />

inclusion, for both the children and their families. Inclusion is facilitated on at least three<br />

levels: (a) among classroom peers, (b) among parents of peers, and (c) among the<br />

participating children’s extended families through sharing of the songs and social<br />

experiences from school. The social value of the interventions and music itself is evident<br />

through comments and reports made by the teachers, the target children’s parents, and the<br />

parents of peers, and is consistent with reports made by other music therapists (e.g.,<br />

Aasgaard, 1999; Ricciarelli, 2003). Additionally, the classroom peers changed their attitudes<br />

toward the target children. This replicates the results of earlier studies with school-aged and<br />

269


270<br />

preschool children, in which music was used for inclusion (Jellison et al., 1984; Humpal,<br />

1991). However, this generalization is not based on systematically collected and analyzed<br />

data. It would be valuable to investigate generalization and attitude changes caused by these<br />

music therapy interventions.<br />

Finally, this study demonstrates the potential benefits of a cumulative case study<br />

design for music therapists working in early intervention. Single case designs enable us to<br />

evaluate clinical practice and consultation with other professionals, and to ask important<br />

questions about the practices and principles of music therapy in a quantitative and<br />

experimental way. Although such designs are not unknown to music therapists – and in fact<br />

are quite prevalent in the literature (Nicholas & Boyle 1983; Gregory, 2002) – only a few<br />

music therapists have engaged in early intervention research using single-case experimental<br />

designs (Harding & Ballard, 1982; Kern & Wolery, 2001). Different professions involved in<br />

early intervention commonly use single-case experimental designs. They are a scientifically<br />

accepted, valid research method (Stile, 1988) and might improve the credibility of music<br />

therapy among other professions. However, a natural limitation of single-case experimental<br />

designs is the small number of participants. In order to understand the generalizability of<br />

interventions, replications with multiple participants is warranted. Nonetheless, ideally this<br />

series of studies is a small contribution to establish music therapy as a widely recognized<br />

related service in Early Intervention/Early Childhood Special Education.<br />

To have greater involvement in early intervention/early childhood special education,<br />

music therapists need to adapt their clinical practice to the needs of individuals and their


families, the public resources, and trends in Early Intervention/Early Childhood Special<br />

Education. <strong>Music</strong> therapists must expand their role along the continuum of care, from direct<br />

therapy (i.e., individual or group therapy sessions) only, to a combination of direct and<br />

consultative therapy, to consultative therapy only as other therapeutic disciplines (e.g.,<br />

speech-language therapy, occupational therapy, physical therapy) have done. <strong>Collaborative</strong><br />

consultation, the most integrated model of service delivery, is desirable because it results in a<br />

more comprehensive and holistic intervention and allows for continuity of service, as no<br />

aspect is completely isolated (Sandler, 1997). <strong>Collaborative</strong> consultation will enhance<br />

services for children with autism and their families, and the early intervention/early<br />

childhood special education discipline might be more willing to incorporate music therapy<br />

into treatment options for children with special needs if such a model is used (Furman, 2002;<br />

McWilliam, 1996; Sandler, 1997; Snell, 2002). For music therapists in training and practice<br />

this means collaborative and consultative methods (e.g., Achilles, Yates, & Freese, 1991;<br />

Buysse et al., 1994; Bruder, 1996) as well as excellent communication skills need to be<br />

learned. <strong>Music</strong> therapists need to be prepared to collaborate with a wide range of disciplines,<br />

respect individual working styles, and be flexible and spontaneous in designing, scheduling<br />

and implementing interventions in collaboration with other professionals. Sensitivity and<br />

empathy in training others, creativity in use of available resources (i.e., materials and<br />

budgets, environments, knowledge and talents of colleagues), and the ability to motivate and<br />

encourage teachers to use music as a natural tool within the childcare routines will be<br />

essential. Continuously changing conditions of classroom demands and dynamics, the<br />

constantly changing numbers of participating children and staff, and spontaneous interactions<br />

with families, all require great flexibility and improvisation skills beyond the musical level.<br />

271


272<br />

Integrated therapy has its limitations and is not the only way to provide services in<br />

early intervention. However, it is an appropriate and effective way of providing treatment<br />

and allows the extension of music therapy service. The use of a collaborative consultative<br />

approach, in music therapy as elsewhere, and for the inclusion of young children with special<br />

needs in preschool education programs, plays an important role in the growth, understanding<br />

and ultimately the broader legitimacy of music therapy as a profession.


8. Conclusions<br />

The conclusions of this study extent to teachers, parents, music therapists, other<br />

service providers, and most importantly, young children with autism. Significant<br />

implications of this cumulative study include the following:<br />

• Individualized music therapy interventions are effective in increasing the<br />

independent performance and broadening the social experiences of young<br />

children with autism within an inclusive childcare program.<br />

• Songs are powerful means of conveying therapeutic goals, and are relatively easy<br />

to use within ongoing childcare activities and routines.<br />

• Both the classroom and playground are natural environments where children’s<br />

learning and development occurs. Therefore, these are appropriate contexts for<br />

implementing music therapy interventions.<br />

• A music therapy collaborative consultative approach is effective in producing<br />

desired outcomes. <strong>Music</strong> therapists play a valuable role in consulting with, and<br />

training, classroom teachers to embed music therapy-based interventions in<br />

inclusive childcare settings. Although there are many ways of providing<br />

therapeutic services in early intervention, the collaborative consultative approach<br />

is desirable because it incorporates current trends and policy into early<br />

intervention/early childhood special education. Moreover, it brings interventions<br />

into closer proximity to children’s needs.<br />

• <strong>Music</strong> therapists must evaluate current clinical practice in early intervention and<br />

expand their role along the continuum of service delivery.<br />

273


274<br />

• <strong>Music</strong> therapy training programs and continuing education courses need to<br />

prepare students and professionals to completely fulfill contemporary models of<br />

service delivery, so they may become efficacious members of on-site early<br />

intervention teams.<br />

• The application of single-case experimental designs can produce desirable<br />

outcomes for clinical practice, and for consultation with other professions, with<br />

the effect of enhancing future integrated services of children with autism in<br />

inclusive childcare programs.<br />

<strong>Music</strong> therapy interventions provide a valuable step toward inclusion, both for<br />

children with autism and their families. Overall, music therapy enhances services for young<br />

children with autism and should be considered as a routine treatment option. On the other<br />

hand training for music therapists in collaborate consultative methods of service delivery, as<br />

well as continued research on the effects of embedded music therapy interventions in<br />

inclusive childcare programs, are needed.


“In the end, that is the real work of the explorer: to share all that you have learned, to make<br />

it available to others, and to close the cycle that you started when you began the journey.”<br />

by Robert Ballard<br />

(as cited in Salz, 2000, p. 149)<br />

It is hoped that this study demonstrates a practical way of how to better serve young children<br />

with autism in community-based, inclusive childcare programs.<br />

275


276


REFERENCES<br />

Aasgaard, T. (1999). <strong>Music</strong> therapy as milieu in the hospice and pediatric oncology<br />

ward. In Aldridge, D. (Ed). <strong>Music</strong> therapy and palliative care. London, England; Bristol,<br />

PA: Jessica Kinsley Publisher.<br />

Accardo, P. J., Magnusen, C., & Capute, A. J. (2000). Autism: Clinical and research<br />

issues. Baltimore, MD: York Press.<br />

Achilles, J., Yates, R. R., & Freese, J. M. (1991). Perspectives from the field:<br />

<strong>Collaborative</strong> consultation in the speech and language program of the Dallas Independent<br />

School District. Language, Speech, and Hearing in Schools, 22, 154-155.<br />

Alberto, P. A., & Troutman, A. C. (1995). Applied behavior analysis for teachers<br />

(5 th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill Publishing Company.<br />

Aldridge, D. (1994). Single-case research designs for the creative art therapist.<br />

The Arts in Psychotherapy, 21 (5), 333-342.<br />

Aldridge, D. (1996). <strong>Music</strong> therapy research and practice in medicine: From out of<br />

the silence. London, England; Bristol, PA: Jessica Kingsley Publisher.<br />

Aldridge, D. (1998). A social scientist reflects on 15 years of research in<br />

complementary medicine. Complementary Therapies in Medicine, 6, 145-151.<br />

Aldridge, D., Gustorff, D., & Neugebauer, L. (1995). A pilot study of music therapy<br />

in the treatment of children with developmental delay. Complementary Therapies in<br />

Medicine, 3, 197-205.<br />

Alger, H. A. (1984). Transitions: Alternatives to manipulative management<br />

technique. Young Children, 39 (6), 16-25.<br />

Allgood, N. (2002). <strong>Music</strong> therapy and sensory integration for children with<br />

autism spectrum disorders. Paper presented at the 10 th <strong>World</strong> Congress of <strong>Music</strong><br />

<strong>Therapy</strong>, <strong>World</strong> Federation of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>. Oxford, England, July, 2002.<br />

277


278<br />

Alvin, J., & Warwick, A. (1991). <strong>Music</strong> therapy for autistic children (2 nd ed.).<br />

Oxford, England: University Press.<br />

American Association of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> (AMTA) (2003a). New <strong>Music</strong> Education<br />

Initiative. <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Matters, 6 (1), 1.<br />

American Association of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> (AMTA) (2003b). AMTA Standards of<br />

Clinical Practice. AMTA Member Sourcebook 2003, 20-25.<br />

American Association of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> (AMTA) (2003c). <strong>Music</strong> therapy and<br />

individuals with diagnosis on the autism spectrum. Retrieved June 3, 2003 from the Internet:<br />

http://www.musictherapy.org/factsheets/autism.html/.<br />

American Psychiatry Association (APA) (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual<br />

of mental disorders (4 th ed.), Text Revision. Washington, DC: Author.<br />

American Society for Testing and Material (1993). Standard consumer safety<br />

performance specification for playgrounds equipment for public use, Author, Philadelphia,<br />

PA.<br />

Anderson, P. P. (2002). Let’s clean up! NY, NY: Houghton Mifflin.<br />

Anderson, S. R., & Romanczyk, R. G. (1999). Early intervention for young children<br />

with autism: Continuum-based behavioral models. Journal of the Association for the<br />

Severely Handicapped, 24, 162-173.<br />

Ando, H. (1977). Training autistic children to urinate in the toilet through operant<br />

conditioning techniques. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 7, 151-163.<br />

Applebaum, E., Egel, A. L., Koegel, R. L., & Imhoff, B. (1979). Measuring<br />

musical abilities of autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9<br />

(3), 279-285.<br />

Autism Society of America (ASA) (2003). Autism info. Author. Retrieved May,<br />

13, 2003 from the Internet: http://www.autism-society.org.


Autism Treatment Center of America (2003). The Son-Rise Program: Frequently<br />

asked questions. Author. Retrieved May, 20, 2003 from the Internet: http://www.sonrise.org/faq.html/.<br />

Ayres, A. J., & Tickle, L. S. (1980). Hyper-responsivity to touch and vestibular<br />

stimuli as a predictor of positive response to sensory integration procedures by autistic<br />

children. American Journal of Occupational <strong>Therapy</strong>, 34, 375-381.<br />

Azrin, N.H., & Foxx, R. M. (1971). A rapid method of toilet training the<br />

institutionalized retarded. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 4, 249-253.<br />

Bailey, L. M. (1984). The use of songs in music therapy with cancer patients and<br />

their families. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 4 (1), 5-17.<br />

Bailey, D. B., & Wolery, M. (1992). Teaching infants and preschoolers with<br />

disabilities. Columbus, Ohio: Simon & Schuster.<br />

Bainbridge, N., & Myles, B. S. (1999). The use of priming to introduce toilet training<br />

to a child with autism. Focus on Autism and other Developmental Disabilities, 16 (2), 106-<br />

109.<br />

Baird, G., Chairman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Sweetenham, J., Wheelwright, S.<br />

& Drew, A. (2000). A screening instrument for autism at 18 month of age: A 6-year followup<br />

study. Journal of the American Academy Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 39 (6), 694-702.<br />

Baker, B. L., & Brightman, A. J. (1997). Steps to independence: Teaching everyday<br />

skills to children with special needs. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Co.<br />

Baker, B. S. (1992). The use of music with autistic children. Journal of Psychosocial<br />

Nursing Mental Health Service, 20 (4), 31-34.<br />

Barlow, D., & Hayes, S. (1984). Single-case experimental designs: Strategies for<br />

studying behavior change. New York: Pergamon Press.<br />

Barnett, W. S. (1998). Long-term effects on cognitive development and school<br />

success. In W. S. Barnett, & S. S. Boocock (Eds.), Early care and education for children in<br />

poverty: Promises, programs, and long-term results (pp. 11-44). Albany, NY: SUNY Press.<br />

279


280<br />

Barney & Friends (1992). Clean Up. On Barney’s Favorites Vol. 1. Compact Disc.<br />

Hollywood, CA: Lyons Partnership. (1993).<br />

Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.<br />

Benenzon, R. O. (1997). <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Theory and Manual: Contributions to<br />

the knowledge of nonverbal contexts (2 nd ed). Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas<br />

Publisher.<br />

Berard, G. (1993). Hearing equal behavior. Bowral, Australia: Keats Publishing.<br />

Berger, D. S. (2002). <strong>Music</strong> therapy, sensory integration and the autistic child.<br />

London, England: Jessica Kingsley publisher.<br />

Berk, L. E. (1997). Child Development (4 th ed). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and<br />

Bacon.<br />

Berk, L. E., & Winsler, A. (2002). Scaffolding children’s learning: Vygotsky and<br />

early childhood education. Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of<br />

Young Children.<br />

Berument, S. K., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Pickles, A., & Bailey, A. (1999). Autism<br />

screening questionnaire: Diagnostic validity. British Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 444-<br />

451.<br />

Blackstock, E. G. (1978). Cerebral asymmetry and the development of early<br />

infantile autism. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 339-353.<br />

Bondy, A. S., & Frost, L. A. (1994). The Picture Exchange Communication System.<br />

Focus on Autism, 9, 1-19.<br />

Boston, B. O. (2000). Start the music: A report from the Early Childhood <strong>Music</strong><br />

Summit. Retrieved April 3, 2003 from the Internet:<br />

http://www.menc.org/guides/startmusic/stmreport.htm.<br />

Boswell, S., & Gray, D. (2003). Applying structured teaching principles to toilet<br />

training. Retrieved January, 14, 2003 from the Internet: http://www.teacch.com/toilet.htm/.


Bornstein, H., Saulnier, K. L., & Hamilton, L.B. (1992). The comprehensive signed<br />

English dictionary. Gaullandet, Washington, D.C.: Gaullandet University Press.<br />

Bredekamp, S. (1991). Developmentally appropriate practice in early intervention<br />

childhood programs. Serving children from birth through age 8. Washington, DC: National<br />

Association for the Education of Young Children.<br />

Bredekamp, S., & Copple, C. (Eds.) (2002). Developmentally appropriate<br />

practice in early childhood programs (2 nd ed.). Washington, DC: National Association<br />

for the Education of Young Children.<br />

Bricker, D. (1978). A rational for the integration of handicapped and<br />

nonhandicapped preschool children. In M. J. Guralnick (Ed.), Early intervention and the<br />

integration of handicapped and nonhandicapped children (pp. 3-26). Baltimore, MD:<br />

University Press.<br />

Bricker, D., Pretti-Frontczak, K., & McComas, N. R. (1998). An active-based<br />

approach to early intervention (2 nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.<br />

Bridwell, N. (1997). Clifford’s spring clean up (Clifford). NY, NY: Cartwheel<br />

Books.<br />

Briggs, C. (1991). A model for understanding musical development. <strong>Music</strong><br />

<strong>Therapy</strong>, 10 (1), 1-21.<br />

Brown, S. M. K. (1994). Autism and music therapy: Is change possible and why<br />

music? Journal of British <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 8 (1), 15-25.<br />

Brownell, M. K. (2002). <strong>Music</strong>ally adapted social stories to modify behaviors in<br />

students with autism: Four case studies. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 39 (2), 117-144.<br />

Bruder, M. B. (1994). Working with members of other disciplines: Collaboration for<br />

success. In M. Wolery, & J. Wilbers (Eds.), Including children with special need in early<br />

childhood programs (pp. 45-70). Washington, DC: National Association for the Education<br />

of Young Children.<br />

Bruder, M. B. (1996). Interdisciplinary collaboration in service delivery. In R. A.<br />

McWilliam (Ed.), Rethinking pull-out services in early intervention: A professional resource<br />

(pp. 27-48). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.<br />

281


282<br />

Brunk, B. K., & Coleman, K. A. (2000). Development of a special education<br />

music therapy assessment process. <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Perspectives, 18 (1), 59-71.<br />

Brusica, K. E. (Ed.). (1996). Case studies in music therapy. Gilsum, NH:<br />

Barcelona Publishers.<br />

Bryan, L. C., & Gast, D. L. (2000). Teaching on-task and on-schedule behaviors to<br />

high-functioning children with autism via picture activity schedules. Journal of Autism<br />

Development Disorder, 30 (6), 553-567.<br />

Buffington, D. M., Krantz, P. J., McClannahan, L. E., & Poulson, C. L. (1998).<br />

Procedure for teaching appropriate gestural communication skills to children with autism.<br />

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28 (6), 535-545.<br />

Bunday, E. M. (1995). The effects of signed and spoken words taught with music<br />

on sign and speech imitation by children with autism. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 32 (3),<br />

189-202.<br />

Buysse, V. (1993). Friendship with preschoolers with disabilities in communitybased<br />

child care settings. Journal of Early Intervention, 17, 380-395.<br />

Buysse, V., Bernier, K., Skinner, D., & Wesley, P. (2000). Smart Start and<br />

qualitative child care in North Carolina: Study highlights. Chapel Hill, NC: University<br />

of North Carolina, Frank Porter Graham Child Development Center.<br />

Buysse, V., & Bailey, D. B. (1993). Behavioral and developmental outcomes in<br />

young children with disabilities in integrated and segregated settings: A review of<br />

comparative studies. Journal of Special Education, 26, 434-461.<br />

Buysse, V., Schulte, A. S., Pierce, P. P., & Terry, D. (1994). Models and styles of<br />

consultation: Preferences of professionals in early intervention. Journal of Early<br />

Intervention, 18, 302-310.<br />

Calculator, S., Fabry, D., Glennon, S., Prizant, B. M., & Schubert, A. (1995).<br />

Technical report on standards of practice on facilitated communication. Rockville, MD:<br />

American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.<br />

Campbell, D. (2000). The Mozart effect for children. NY, NY: Harper Collins<br />

Publisher.


Cambell, M., Schopler, E., Cueva, J., & Hallin, A. (1996). Treatment of autistic<br />

disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Children and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35<br />

(2), 134-143.<br />

Caine, R., & Caine, G. (1991). Making connections: Teaching and the human<br />

brain. New York: Addison-Wesley.<br />

Capps, L., Sigman M., & Mundy, P. (1994). Attachment security in children with<br />

autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 249-261.<br />

Carta, J. J. (1994). Development appropriate practice: Shifting the emphasis to<br />

individual appropriateness. Journal of Early Intervention, 18, 343-359.<br />

Carta, J. J. (1995). Development appropriate practice: A critical analysis as<br />

applied to children with disabilities. Focus on Exceptional Children, 27, 1-16.<br />

Carta, J. J., Greenwood, C. R., & Robinson, S. (1987). Application of an ecobehavioral<br />

approach to the evaluation of early intervention programs. In R. Prinz (Ed.),<br />

Advances in the behavioral assessment of children and families (Vol. 3) (pp. 123-155).<br />

Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.<br />

Casto, G., & Mastropieri, F. A. (1986). The efficacy of early intervention<br />

programs: A meta-analysis. Exceptional Children, 25, 417-424.<br />

Chester, K. K., Holmberg, T. K., Lawrence, M. P., & Thurmond, L. L. (1999). A<br />

program-based consultative music therapy model for public schools. <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong><br />

Perspectives, 17 (2), 82-91.<br />

Cicero, F. R., & Pfadt, A. (2002). Investigation of a reinforcement-based toilet<br />

training procedure for children with autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 23,<br />

319-331.<br />

Clarkson, G. (1991). <strong>Music</strong> therapy for a nonverbal autistic adult. In K. E. Bruscia<br />

(Ed.), Case studies in music therapy (pp. 373-385). Gilsum, NH: Barcelona Publishers.<br />

Cole, J., & Miller, M. (1989). Your new potty. NY, NY: Hapercollins Juvenile.<br />

283


284<br />

Cole, K. N., Harris, S. R., Eland, S. F., & Mills, P. E. (1989). Comparison of two<br />

service delivery models: In-class and out-class therapy approaches. Pediatric Physical<br />

<strong>Therapy</strong>, 1, 49-54.<br />

Cole-Currens, E. (1993). Smooth transitions for a smooth day. Texas Child Care, 17<br />

(3), 10-19.<br />

Coleman, K. A. (2002). <strong>Music</strong> therapy for learners with severe disabilities in a<br />

public school setting. In B. L. Wilson (Ed.), Models of music therapy interventions in<br />

school settings (2 nd ed) (pp. 197-209). Silver Spring, MD: The American <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong><br />

Association, Inc.<br />

Colwell, C. M., & Murrless, K. D. (2002). <strong>Music</strong> activities (singing vs. chanting) as a<br />

vehicle for reading accuracy of children with learning disabilities: A pilot study. <strong>Music</strong><br />

<strong>Therapy</strong> Perspectives, 20 (1), 13-19.<br />

Crocker, D. B. (1968). Clinical experience with emotional disturbed children. In E.<br />

T. Gaston (Ed.), <strong>Music</strong> in <strong>Therapy</strong> (pp. 202-207). NY, NY: Macmillan Publishing Co.<br />

Crosser, S. (1994). Making the most of water play. Young Children, 49 (5), 28-32.<br />

Cryer, D., Hurwitz, S., & Wolery, M. (1999). Continuity of care in the nation’s<br />

childcare programs. Presentation at the Annual Meeting of the National Association for<br />

the Education of Young Children. New Orleans, November 1999.<br />

Cullen, J. (1993). Preschool children’s use and perceptions of outdoor play areas.<br />

Early Child Development and Care, 89, 45-56.<br />

Cuvo, A. J., May, M. E., & Post, T. M. (2001). Effects of living room, Snoezelen<br />

room, and outdoor activities on stereotypic behavior and engagement by adults with<br />

profound mental retardation. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 22, 183-204.<br />

Dalldorf, J. S. (2002a). An instruction to the medical aspect of autism. Retrieved<br />

April, 23, 2003 from the Internet: http://www.teacch.com/medinfo.htm/.<br />

Dalldorf, J. S. (2002b). A pediatric view of the treatment options for autistic<br />

syndrome. Retrieved April 24, 2003 from the Internet:<br />

http://www.teacch.com/treatmnt.htm/.


Dalrymple, N. J., & Ruble, L. A. (1992). Toilet training behavior of people with<br />

autism: Parent views. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 22 (2), 265-275.<br />

Danko, C. D., & Buysee, V. (2002). Thank you for being a friend. Young<br />

Exceptional Children, 6 (1), 2-9.<br />

Darrow, A.-A., & Grohe, H. (2002). <strong>Music</strong> therapy for learners who are<br />

deaf/hard-of-hearing. In B. L. Wilson (Ed.), Models of music therapy interventions in<br />

school settings (2 nd ed) (pp. 291-317). Silver Spring, MD: The American <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong><br />

Association, Inc.<br />

Davies, P. L., & Gavin, W. J. (1994). Comparison of individual and<br />

group/consultation treatment methods for preschool children with developmental delays.<br />

The American Journal of Occupational <strong>Therapy</strong>, 48, 155-161.<br />

Davis, R. (1990). A model for the integration of music therapy within preschool<br />

classrooms for children with physical disabilities and language delays. <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong><br />

Perspectives, 8, 82-84.<br />

Davis, R. (2001). Taking first steps in preschool together: A hierarchical<br />

approach to group music therapy. Early Childhood Connections, 7 (2), 33-42.<br />

Dawson, G., & Osterling, J. (1997). Early intervention in autism. In M. J.<br />

Guralnick (Ed.), The effectiveness of early intervention (pp. 307-326). Baltimore, MD:<br />

P. H. Brookes Publisher Co.<br />

Dawson, G., & Watling, R. (2000). Interventions to facilitate auditory, visual,<br />

and motor integration in autism: A review of evidence. Journal of Autism and<br />

Developmental Disorders, 30 (5), 415-421.<br />

DeKlyen, M., & Odom, S. L. (1989). Activity structure and social interactions<br />

with peers in developmentally integrated play groups. Journal of Early Intervention, 13,<br />

342-352.<br />

Delacato, C. H. (1974). The ultimate stranger: The autistic child. Novato, CA:<br />

Academic <strong>Therapy</strong> Press.<br />

285


286<br />

Dempsey, I., & Foreman, P. (2001). A review of educational approaches for<br />

individuals with autism. International Journal of Disabilities, Development and Education,<br />

48 (1), 103-116.<br />

Dettmer, S., Simpson, R. L., Myles, B. S., & Ganz, J. B. (2000). The use of visual<br />

supports to facilitate transitions of students with autism. Focus on Autism and Other<br />

Developmental Disabilities, 15 (3), 163-169.<br />

DiSalvo, C. A., & Oswald, D. P. (2002). Peer-mediated interventions to increase the<br />

social interaction of children with autism: Consideration of peer expectancies. Focus on<br />

Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 17 (4), 198-207.<br />

Division of Early Childhood (DEC) Task Force on Recommended practices.<br />

(1993). DEC recommended practices: indicators of quality in programs for infants and<br />

young children with special needs and their families. Reston, VA: Council for<br />

Exceptional Children.<br />

Division of Early Childhood (DEC) (1998). Position on services for children<br />

birth to age eight with special needs and their families. Retrieved March 28, 2003 from<br />

the Internet: http://www.dec-sped.org/positions/birth08.html/.<br />

Division of Early Childhood (DEC) (2000). Position on inclusion. Retrieved<br />

March 28, 2003 from the Internet: http://www.dec-sped.org/positions/inclusio.html/.<br />

Doherty, K., McNally, P., & Sherrard, E. (2000). I have autism: What’s that?<br />

Belfast, Northern Ireland: Parents & Professionals & Autism.<br />

Donnellan, A. M., Mirenda, P. L., Mesaros, R. A., & Fassbender, L. L. (1984).<br />

Analyzing the communicative functions of aberrant behavior. Journal of the Associations of<br />

Persons with Severe Handicaps, 9, 201-212.<br />

Doyle, P. M., Wolery, M., Gast, D. L., Ault, M. J., & Wiley, K. (1990). Comparison<br />

of constant time delay and the system of less prompts in teaching preschoolers with<br />

developmental delays. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 11, 1-22.<br />

Dunn, W. (1990). A comparison of service provision models in school-based<br />

occupational therapy services: A pilot study. The Occupational <strong>Therapy</strong> Journal of<br />

Research, 10, 300-320.


Dunn, W. (1996). Occupational <strong>Therapy</strong>. In R. A. McWilliam (Ed.), Rethinking<br />

pull-out services in early intervention: A professional resource (pp. 267-313). Baltimore,<br />

MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.<br />

Dunst, C. T., Trivette, C. M., Boyd, K., & Hambey, D. (1996). Family oriented<br />

program models, help giving practices, and parental control appraisal. Exceptional Children,<br />

62, 237-248.<br />

Dworschak, M. (2003). Hupen in Fis. [Honking in F#]. Spiegel, 11, 164-167.<br />

Edgerton, C. L. (1994). The effect of improvisational music therapy on the<br />

communicative behavior of autistic children. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 31 (1), 31-62.<br />

Education <strong>World</strong> (2003). Germs ”Spread” into school curriculum: Handwashing<br />

saves the day! Retrieved January, 16, 2003 from the Internet: http://www.educationworld.com/.<br />

Elefant, C. (2001). Speechless yet communicative: Revealing the person behind the<br />

disability of Rett Syndrome through clinical research on song in music therapy. In D.<br />

Aldridge, G. DiFranco, E. Ruud, & T. Wigram (Eds.), <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> in Europe (pp. 113-<br />

128). Rome, Italy: ISMEZ Publisher.<br />

Enoch, A. (2001). Let’s do it again. All Together Now! (ATN), 7 (1), 5-7.<br />

Ermer, J, & Dunn, W. (1998). The sensory profile: A discriminate analysis of<br />

children with and without disabilities. Journal of Occupational <strong>Therapy</strong>, 52, 283-290.<br />

Fanjul, S. D., & Ball, P. M. (1995). Division of Child Development. Child Day Care<br />

Handbook. North Carolina Department of Human Recourses.<br />

Fenske, E. C., Zalenski, S., Krantz, P.J., & McClannahan, L. E. (1985). Age at<br />

intervention and treatment outcome for autistic children in a comprehensive intervention<br />

program. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 5 (1-2), 49-58.<br />

Fisher, R. (1991). Original song drawings in the treatment of a developmentally<br />

disabled, autistic adult. In K. E. Bruscia (Ed.), Case studies in music therapy (pp. 359-371).<br />

Gilsum, NH: Barcelona Publishers.<br />

287


288<br />

Frank Port Graham Child Development Institute (FPG) (2003a). Overview.<br />

Retrieved March 17, 2003 from the Internet: http://www.fpg.unc.edu/overview/index.htm<br />

Frank Porter Graham Child Development Institute (FPG) (2003b). The FPG day<br />

care center: About the FPG family and child care program. Retrieved April 29, 2003 from<br />

the Internet http://www.fpg.unc.edu/daycare/<br />

Frankel, A. (1999). Once upon a potty (boy). NY, NY: Harpercollins Juvenile.<br />

Frith, U. (1972). Cognitive mechanisms in autism: Experiments with color and tone<br />

sequence production. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 2, 160-173.<br />

Frost, L. A., & Bondy, A. (1994). PECS: The Picture Exchange Communication<br />

System. Cherry Hill, NJ: Pyramid Educational Consultants.<br />

Fujiki, M., Brinton, B., Isaacson, T., & Summers, C. (2001). Social behaviors of<br />

children with language impairment on the playground: A pilot study. Language, Speech and<br />

Hearing Service in Schools, 32, 101-113.<br />

Furman, A. G. (2001). Young children with autism spectrum disorder. Early<br />

Childhood Connections, 7 (2), 43-49.<br />

Furman, A. (2002). <strong>Music</strong> therapy for learners in a community early education<br />

public school. In B. L. Wilson (Ed.), Models of music therapy interventions in school<br />

settings (2 nd ed) (pp. 369-388). Silver Spring, MD: The American <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong><br />

Association, Inc.<br />

Gallender, D. (1980). Teaching eating and toileting skills to the multi-handicapped<br />

in the school setting. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.<br />

Galloway, H. F., & Bean M. F. (1974). The effects of action songs on the<br />

development of body-image and body-parts identification in hearing impaired preschool<br />

children. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 11 (3), 125-134.<br />

Garfinkle, A. N., & Schwartz, I. S. (2002). Peer imitation: Increasing social<br />

interactions in children with autism and other developmental disabilities in inclusive<br />

preschool classrooms. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 22, 26-38.


Geiger, B. F., Artz, L., Petri, C. L., Winnail, S. D., & Mason, J. W. (2000). Fun with<br />

handwashing education. Retrieved October, 25, 2002 from the Internet<br />

http://askeric.org/cgi-bin/printlessons.cgi/Virtual/Lessons/Health/Process_S<br />

Gervin, A. P. (1991). <strong>Music</strong> therapy compensatory techniques utilizing song lyrics<br />

during dressing to promote independence in the patient with brain injury. <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong><br />

Perspectives, 9, 87-90.<br />

Gfeller, K. E. (1983). <strong>Music</strong>al mnemonics as an aid to retention with normal and<br />

learning disabled students. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 20 (4), 179-189.<br />

Giles, M. M., Cogan, D., & Cox, C. (1991). A music and art program to promote<br />

emotional health in elementary school children. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 28, 135-148.<br />

Gillberg, C., & Coleman, M. (1996). Autism and medical disorders: A review of<br />

the literature. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 38, 191-202.<br />

Gillberg, C., Ehlers, S., Schaumann, H., Jakobsson, G., Dahlgren, S. O.,<br />

Lindblom, R., Bagenholm, A., Tjuus, T., & Blindner, E. (1990). Autism under age 3<br />

years: A clinical study of 28 cases referred for autistic symptoms in infancy. Journal of<br />

Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 21, 921-934.<br />

Gillberg, C., & Wing, L. (1999). Autism: Not an extremely rare disorder. Acta<br />

Psychiatric Scandinavia, 99, 399-409.<br />

Gladfelter N. D. (2002). <strong>Music</strong> therapy for learners with learning disabilities in a<br />

private school settings. In B. L. Wilson (Ed.), Models of music therapy interventions in<br />

school settings (2 nd ed) (pp. 277-317). Silver Spring, MD: The American <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong><br />

Association, Inc.<br />

Gold, S., Liepack, S., Scott, M., & Benjamin-Wise, S. (1999). Creating inclusive<br />

classrooms: A report from the field. Young Exceptional Children, 2 (2), 2-8.<br />

Goldstein, H. L. (1991). Physical environments: Planning a supportive environment.<br />

Kentucky Preschool Program Technical Assistance Paper, No. 4, Kentucky State Department<br />

of Education, Frankfort, KY (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 379102).<br />

289


290<br />

Goldstein, H. L., Kaczmarek, L., Pennington, R., & Shafer, K. (1992). Peermediated<br />

intervention: Attending to, commenting on, and acknowledging the behavior of<br />

preschoolers with autism. Journal of Applied Behaviour Analysis, 25, 289-305.<br />

Goldstein, H. L., Wickstrom, S., Hoyson, M., Jamieson, B., & Odom, S. L. (1988).<br />

Effects of sociodramatic play training on social and communicative interaction. Education<br />

and Treatment of Children, 11, 97-117.<br />

Gottschewski, K. (2001). Autismus aus der Innenperspektive und Musiktherapie<br />

[Autism from an inside-out perspective and music therapy]. In D. Aldridge (Ed.), Kairos V:<br />

Musiktherapie mit Kindern: Beitraege zur Musiktherapie in der Medizin (pp. 40-57). Bern;<br />

Goettingen; Toronto; Seattle: Verlag Hans Huber.<br />

Grandin, T. (1988a). Teaching tips from a recovered autistic. Focus on Autistic<br />

Behavior, 3 (1), 1-8.<br />

Grandin, T. (1988b). My experiences as an autistic child and review of selected<br />

literature. Journal of Orthomolecular Psychiatry, 13 (3), 144-174.<br />

Grandin, T. (1992). Calming effects of deep touch pressure in patients with<br />

autistic disorder, college students, and animals. Retrieved May 30, 2003 from the<br />

Internet: http://www.grandin.com/inc/squeeze.html<br />

Grandin, T. (1999). Social Problems: Understanding emotions and developing<br />

talents. Retrieved May 16, 2003 from the Internet:<br />

http://www.autism.org//temple/social.html/.<br />

Grandin, T. (2003). An inside view of autism. Retrieved April 24, 2003 from the<br />

Internet: http://www.autism.org//temple/inside.html/.<br />

Grandin, T., & Scariano, M. M. (1996). Emergence: Labeled autistic. NY, NY:<br />

Warner Books.<br />

Gray, C., & Garand, J. (1993). Social stories: Improving responses of students with<br />

autism with accurate social information. Focus on Autistic Behavior, 8, 1-10.<br />

Green, G. (1996). Early behavioral intervention for autism: What does research tell<br />

us? In C. Maurice (Ed.), Behavioral interventions for young children with autism: A manual<br />

for young children with autism (pp. 29-44). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.


Greenspan, S. I., & Wieder, S. (1997). Developmental patterns and outcomes in<br />

infants and children with disorders in relating and communicating: A chart review of 200<br />

cases of children with autistic spectrum diagnoses. Journal of Developmental and Learning<br />

Disorders, 1, 87-141.<br />

Gregory, D. (2002). Four decades of music therapy behavioral research designs: A<br />

content analysis of Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> articles. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 29 (1), 56-<br />

71.<br />

Griffin, C., & Rinn, B. (1998). Enhancing outdoor play with an obstacle course.<br />

Young Children, 53 (3), 18-23.<br />

Gunsberg, A. (1988). Improvised musical play: A strategy for fostering social<br />

play between developmentally delayed and nondelayed preschool children. Journal of<br />

<strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 25 (4), 178-191.<br />

Gunsberg, A. (1991). A method for conducting improvised musical play with<br />

children both with and without developmental delay in preschool classrooms. <strong>Music</strong><br />

<strong>Therapy</strong> Perspective, 9, 46-51.<br />

Gustorff, D. (1997). Sebastian- Annehmen und Verstehen in der Musiktherapie:<br />

Eine Fallstudie [Sebastian-Acceptance and understanding in music therapy: A case<br />

study]. In D. Aldridge (Ed.), Kairos I:Beitraege zur Musiktherapie in der Medizin (pp. 7-<br />

13). Bern; Goettingen; Toronto; Seattle: Huber.<br />

Gustorff, D., & Neugebauer, L. (1997). Musiktherapie mit Bahman, einem<br />

autistischen Jungen: Eine Fallstudie [<strong>Music</strong> therapy with Bahman, an autistic boy: A case<br />

study]. In D. Aldridge (Ed.), Kairos I:Beitraege zur Musiktherapie in der Medizin, 12-13.<br />

Bern; Goettingen; Toronto; Seattle: Huber.<br />

Hafner, W. (1998). Singend, spielend, lernend [Singing, playing, learning]. Kork,<br />

Germany: Author.<br />

Hafner, W. (1999). Neue Spiel-und Kinderlieder [New children’s play- songs].<br />

Kork, Germany: Author.<br />

Hales, S., Henderson, J., Strevens, A., & Warwick, A. (2002). From the outside<br />

looking in: The Oxfordshire Education Authority Team of <strong>Music</strong> Therapist. Paper<br />

presented at the 10 th <strong>World</strong> Congress of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, <strong>World</strong> Federation of <strong>Music</strong><br />

<strong>Therapy</strong>. Oxford, England, July, 2002.<br />

291


292<br />

Halle, J. W., Marshall, A. M., & Spradlin, J. E. (1979). Time delay: A techniques to<br />

increase language use and facilitate generalization in retarded children. Journal of Applied<br />

Behaviour Analysis, 12, 431-239.<br />

Hairston, M. J. P. (1990). Analysis of responses of mentally retarded autistic and<br />

mentally retarded nonautistic children to art and music therapy. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>,<br />

27 (3), 137-150.<br />

Handleman, J. S., & Harris, S. L. (Eds.) (2001). Preschool education programs<br />

for children with autism (2 nd ed). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.<br />

Hanser, S. B. (1995). Applied Behavior Analysis. In B. L. Wheeler (Ed.), <strong>Music</strong><br />

therapy research: Quantitative and qualitative perspectives (pp. 149-163). Phoenixville, PA:<br />

Barcelona Publishers.<br />

Harding, C., & Ballard, K. D. (1982). The effectiveness of music as a stimulus and as<br />

a contingent rewards in promoting the spontaneous speech of three physically handicapped<br />

preschoolers. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 19 (2), 86-101.<br />

Harms T., Clifford R. M., & Cryer D. (1998). Early Childhood Environment Rating<br />

Scale (Revised Edition). New York, NY: Teachers College Press.<br />

Harris, S. L., Handelman, J. S., Gordon, R., Kristoff, B., & Fuentes, F. (1991).<br />

Changes in cognitive and language functioning of preschool children with autism.<br />

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 21, 281-290.<br />

Hart, M. (1990). Drumming at the edge of magic. A journey into the spirit of<br />

percussion (2 nd ed.). Novato, CA: Grateful Dead Books.<br />

Heffner, G. J. (2002). Toilet Training. (BBB Autism, printable article #25).<br />

Retrieved Januray, 16, 2003 from the Internet: http://www.bbbautismus.com.<br />

Henninger, M. L. (1994). Planning for outdoor play. Young Children, 49 (4), 10-15.<br />

Hilliard, R. E. (2001). The effects of music therapy-based bereavement groups on<br />

mood and behavior of grieving children: A pilot study. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 38 (4),<br />

291-306.


Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1998). Hello and Goodbye: A study of social engagement<br />

in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28 (2), 117-127.<br />

Holcombe, A., Wolery, M., & Gast, D. L. (1994). Comparative single-subject<br />

research: Description of designs and discussion of problems. Topics in Early Childhood<br />

Special Education, 14, 119-145.<br />

Hollander, F. M., & Juhrs, P. D. (1974). Orff-Schulwerk, an effective treatment tool<br />

with autistic children. <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Today, 11 (1), 1-12.<br />

Horn, C. (1988). Use of music to increase verbal response and improve<br />

expressive language abilities of preschool language delayed children. Journal of <strong>Music</strong><br />

<strong>Therapy</strong>, 25 (2), 73-84.<br />

Hoshino, Y., Kumashiro, H., Yashima, Y., Tachibana, R., Watanabe, M., &<br />

Furukawa, H. (1982). Early symptoms of autistic children and its diagnostic significant.<br />

Folia Psychiatrica et Neurologica Japonica, 36, 367-374.<br />

Hoskins, C. (1988). Use of music to increase verbal response and improve<br />

expressive language abilities of preschool language delayed children. Journal of <strong>Music</strong><br />

<strong>Therapy</strong>, 25 (2), 73-84.<br />

Howard, B. J. (1991). It’s potty time (Video tape). (Available from Duke Family<br />

Services, Learning Through Entertainment, Inc.).<br />

Howat, R. (1995). Elisabeth: A case study of an autistic child in individual music<br />

therapy. In T. Wigram, B. Saperston & R. West (Eds.). The art and science of music<br />

therapy: A handbook (pp. 238-257). Chur, Switzerland: Harwood Academic Publishers.<br />

Hughes, J. E., Rice, B. J., DeBedout, J. K., & Hightower, L. M. (2002). <strong>Music</strong><br />

therapy for learners in comprehensive public school systems: Three district-wide models. In<br />

B. L. Wilson (Ed.). Models of music therapy interventions in school settings (2 nd ed) (pp.<br />

319-368). Silver Spring, MD: The American <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Association, Inc.<br />

Hughes J. E., Robbins, B. J., McKenzie, B. A., & Robb, S. S. (1990). Integrating<br />

exceptional and nonexceptional young children through music play: A pilot program.<br />

<strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Perspectives, 8, 52-56.<br />

293


294<br />

Hulsegge, J., & Verheul, A. (1998). Snoezelen: Eine andere Welt. [Snoezelen: A<br />

different world]. Marburg: Lebenshilfe-Verlag.<br />

Humpal, M. E. (1990). Early intervention: The implication for music therapy.<br />

<strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Perspectives, 8, 31-34.<br />

Humpal, M. E. (1991). The effects of an integrated early childhood music program<br />

on social interaction among children with handicaps and their typical peers. Journal of<br />

<strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 33 (1), 19-33.<br />

Humpal, M. E. (1998). Information Sharing: Song repertoire of young children.<br />

<strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Perspectives, 19, 37-38.<br />

Humpal, M. E. (2001). Annotated bibliography of music therapy articles related to<br />

young children: From music therapy journals (1990-2000). Early Childhood Connections, 7<br />

(2), 16-18.<br />

Humpal, M. E. (2002). <strong>Music</strong> therapy for learners in an early childhood<br />

community interagency setting. In B. L. Wilson (Ed.), Models of music therapy<br />

interventions in school settings (2 nd ed) (pp. 389-428). Silver Spring, MD: The American<br />

<strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Association, Inc.<br />

Humpal, M. E., & Wolf, J. (2003). <strong>Music</strong> in the inclusive environment. Young<br />

Children, 58 (2), 103-107.<br />

Hundert, J., & Hopkins, B. (1992). Training supervisors in a collaborative team<br />

approach to promote peer interaction of children with disabilities in integrated<br />

preschools. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 385-400.<br />

Hundert, J., Mahoney, W. J., Mundy, F., & Vernon, M. V. (1998). A descriptive<br />

analysis of developmentally appropriate and social gains of children with disabilities in<br />

segregated and inclusive preschools in southern Ontario. Early Childhood Research<br />

Quarterly, 13 (1) 49-65.<br />

Hutch-Rasmussen, C. (1995). Sensory Integration. Retrieved May 19, 2003 from<br />

the Internet: http://www.autism.org//temple/si.html/.


Hwang, B., & Hughes, C. (2000). The effects of social interaction training on<br />

early social communicative skills of children with autism. Journal of Autism and<br />

Developmental Disorders, 30 (4), 331-343.<br />

Jellison, J. (1985). An investigation of the factor structure of a scale for the<br />

measurement of children’s attitude toward handicapped peers within regular music<br />

environments. Journal of Research in <strong>Music</strong> Education, 32 (4), 243-264.<br />

Jellison, J., Brooks, B., & Huck, A. (1984). Structuring small groups and music<br />

reinforcement to facilitate positive interactions and acceptance of severely handicapped<br />

students in the regular music class. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> Education, 32, 243-264.<br />

Jellison, J. A., & Miller, N. I. (1982). Recall of digit and word sequences by<br />

musicians and nonmusians as a function of spoken and sung input a task. Journal of <strong>Music</strong><br />

<strong>Therapy</strong>, 19 (4), 102-13.<br />

Johnson, F. (2002). Models of service delivery and their relation to the IEP. In B. L.<br />

Wilson (Ed.), Models of music therapy interventions in school settings (2 nd ed) (pp. 83-107).<br />

Silver Spring, MD: The American <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Association, Inc.<br />

Joint Commission on Health Education Standards (1995). National Health Education<br />

Standard. Washington, D.C.: Author.<br />

Jonsdottir, V. (2002). <strong>Music</strong>king in early intervention: Early intervention as a<br />

framework for music therapy with caretakers and their special-needs infants. In Voices: A<br />

<strong>World</strong> Forum for <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Vol. 2 (2) 2002. Retrieved October, 30, 2002 from the<br />

Internet: http://www.voices.no/mainissues/Voices2(2)jonsdottir.html.<br />

Kaiser, A. P., Yoder, P., & Keetz, A. (1992). Evaluating milieu therapy. In S. F.<br />

Warren, & J. Reichle (Eds.), Causes and effects in communication and language<br />

intervention (pp. 9-47). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publisher Co.<br />

Kaiser, A. P., Ostrosky, M. M., Alpert, C. L. (1993). Training teachers to use<br />

environmental arrangement and milieu teaching with nonvocal preschool children.<br />

Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 18 (3), 188-199.<br />

Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2,<br />

217-250.<br />

295


296<br />

Kaplan, M., Edelson, S. M., & Seip, J.-A. L. (1998). Behavioral changes in autistic<br />

individuals as a result of wearing ambient transitional prism lenses. Child Psychiatry and<br />

Human Development, 27 (2), 81-91.<br />

Kazdin, A. E. (1982). Single case research designs: Methods for clinical and applied<br />

settings. New York: Oxford University Press.<br />

Keen, D., Siagfoos, J., & Woodyatt, G. (2001). Replacing prelinguistic behaviors<br />

with functional communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31 (4),<br />

385-398.<br />

Kemmis, B. L., & Dunn, W. (1996). <strong>Collaborative</strong> consultation: The efficacy of<br />

remedial and compensatory interventions in school context. The American Journal of<br />

Occupational <strong>Therapy</strong>, 59, 709- 717.<br />

Kern, P., Marlette, S., & Snyder A. (2002). Sounds on the playground. All Together<br />

Now! (ATN), 8 (2), 3-5.<br />

Kern, P., & Wolery, M. (2001). Participation of a preschooler with visual<br />

impairments on the playground: Effects of musical adaptations and staff development.<br />

Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 38 (2), 149-164.<br />

Kern, P., & Wolery, M. (2002). The Sound Path: Adding music to a childcare<br />

playground. Young Exceptional Children, 5 (3), 12-20.<br />

Koegel, R. L., Dyer, K., & Bell, L. K. (1987). The influence of childprefered<br />

activities on autistic chidlren’s social behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,<br />

20, 243-252.<br />

Koegel, R. L., & Koegel, L. K. (1995). Teaching children with autism: Strategies<br />

for initiating positive interactions and improving learning opportunities. Baltimore, MD:<br />

P. H. Brookes Publishing Co.<br />

Koegel, R. L., Koegel, L., K., & Carter, C. M. (1999). Pivotal teaching<br />

interactions for children with autism. School Psychology Review, 28 (4), 576-594.<br />

Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., Hurley, C., & Frea, W. (1992). Improving social<br />

skills and disruptive behaviors in children with autism. Journal of Autism and<br />

Developmental Disorders, 25, 341-353.


Koegel, R. L., O’Dell, C., & Koegel, L. K. (1987). A natural language teaching<br />

paradigm for nonverbal autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental<br />

Disorders, 17, 187-200.<br />

Koska, M. J. (1993). A comparison of selected behaviors of students with autism<br />

in special education and regular music classes. <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Perspectives, 11, 57-60.<br />

Kramer, S. A. (1978). The effects of music as a cue in maintaining handwashing in<br />

preschool children. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 15 (3), 136-144.<br />

Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. (1993). Teaching children with autism to initiate<br />

to peers: Effects of a script-fading procedure. Journal of Applied Behaviour Analysis, 26,<br />

121-132.<br />

Kraus, K. (2002). Treatments for autism: Many choices- not enough answers.<br />

Retrieved May 16 th , 2003 from the Internet: http://www.commtx.com/times.autism.html/.<br />

Krout, R. (1986). <strong>Music</strong> therapy in special education: Developing and<br />

maintaining social skills necessary for mainstreaming. St. Louis: MMB <strong>Music</strong>, Inc.<br />

Krout, R. (1987). <strong>Music</strong> therapy with multi-handicapped students: Individualized<br />

treatment within a group setting. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 24 (1), 2-13.<br />

Ladd, G. W., & Price, M. (1986). Play styles of peer-accepted and peer-rejected<br />

children on the playground. In C. H. Hart (Ed.), Children on Playgrounds: Research<br />

perspectives and Applications (pp. 130-161). Albany, NY: State University of New York<br />

Press.<br />

Lamorey, S., & Bricker, D. (1993). Integrated programs: Effects on young<br />

children and their parents. In C. A. Peck, S. L. Odom, & D. Bricker (Eds.), Integrating<br />

young children with disabilities into community programs: Ecological perspectives on<br />

research and implementation (pp. 249-270). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing<br />

Co.<br />

Lauritsen, M. B., & Ewald, H. (2001). The genetics of autism. Acta Psychiatric<br />

Scandinavia, 103, 411-427.<br />

297


298<br />

Lazar, I., & Darlington, R. (1982). Lasting effects of early education: A report<br />

from the consortium for longitudinal studies. Monographs of the Society for Research in<br />

Child Development, 47 (2-3). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.<br />

Lecourt, E. (1991). Off-beat music therapy: A psychoanalytic approach to autism. In<br />

K.E. Bruscia (Ed.), Case studies in music therapy (pp. 73-98). Gilsum, NH: Barcelona<br />

Publishers.<br />

Loewy, J. V. (1995). The musical stage of speech: A developmental model of preverbal<br />

sound making. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 13 (1), 47-73.<br />

Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism Diagnostic Observation<br />

Schedule (ADOS). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.<br />

Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal educational intellectual<br />

functioning in young autistic children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,<br />

55, 3-9.<br />

Lovaas, O. I., & Smith, T. (1989). A comprehensive behavioral theory of autistic<br />

children: Paradigm for research and treatment. Journal of Behavioral <strong>Therapy</strong> and<br />

Experimental Psychiatry, 20, 45-68.<br />

Mackey, S., & McQueen, J. (1997). Exploring the association between integrated<br />

therapy and inclusive education. British Journal of Special Education, 25 (1), 22-27.<br />

Mahlberg, M. (1973). <strong>Music</strong> therapy in the treatment of an autistic child. Journal<br />

of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 10 (4), 189-193.<br />

Marcus, L., Schopler, E., & Lord, C. (2001). TEACCH Services for preschool<br />

children. In J. S. Handelman & S. L. Harris (Eds.), Preschool education programs for<br />

children with autism (2 nd ed) (pp. 215-232). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.<br />

Marohn, S. (2002). The natural medicine guide to autism. Charlottesville, VA:<br />

Hampton Roads Publishing Company.<br />

Martin, S. S., Brandy, M. P., & Williams, R. E. (1991). Effects of toys on the<br />

social behavior of preschool children in integrated and nonintegrated groups:<br />

Investigations of a setting event. Journal of Early Intervention, 15, 152-161.


Mayer-Johnson, R. (1992). The Picture Communication Symbols. Solana Beach,<br />

CA: Mayer-Johnson, Co.<br />

McGee, G.G., Almeida, M. C., Sulzer-Azaroff, B., & Feldman, R. S. (1992).<br />

Promoting reciprocal interactions via peer incidental teaching. Journal of Applied<br />

Behaviour Analysis, 25 (1), 117-126.<br />

McGee, G. G., Morrier, M. J., & Daly, T. (2001). The Walden Early Childhood<br />

Programs. In J. S. Handelman and S. L. Harris (Eds.), Preschool education programs<br />

for children with autism (2 nd ed) (pp. 157-190). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.<br />

McGee, G. G., Morrier, M. J., & Daly, T. (1999). An incidental teaching<br />

approach to early intervention for toddlers with autism. Journal of the Association for<br />

the Severely Handicapped, 24, 133-146.<br />

McGee, G. G., Paradis, T., & Feldman, R. S. (1993). Free effects on integration<br />

on levels of autistic behaviors. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 13 (1), 57-<br />

67.<br />

McIntosh, H. (1999). Researches unearth new treatments for autism. In Monitor,<br />

30 (8 ) (online), September, available at<br />

http://www.apa.org/monitor/sep99/homepage.html/.<br />

McConnell, S. R. (2002). Interventions to facilitate social interaction for young<br />

children with autism: Review of available research and recommendations for educational<br />

intervention and further research. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32 (5),<br />

351-372.<br />

McGuire, K. M. (2001). The use of music on Barney & Friends: Implication in music<br />

therapy practice and research. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 38 (2), 114-148.<br />

McWilliam, R. A. (1992). Family-centered intervention planning: A routinebased<br />

approach. Tucson, AZ: Communication/<strong>Therapy</strong> Skill Builders.<br />

McWilliam, R. A. (1995). Integration of therapy and consultative special education:<br />

A continuum in early intervention. Infants and Young Children, 7 (4), 29-38.<br />

McWilliam, R. A. (Ed.). (1996). Rethinking pull-out services in early<br />

intervention: A professional resource. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.<br />

299


300<br />

McWilliam, R. A. (2000). Recommended practice in interdisciplinary models. In S.<br />

Sandall, M. E. McLean, & B. J. Smith (Eds.), DEC: Recommended practices in early<br />

intervention/early childhood special education (pp. 47-52). Longmont, CO: Sopris West.<br />

McWilliam, R. A., & Bailey, D. B. (1994). Predictors of service delivery models in<br />

center-based early intervention. Exceptional Children, 61, 56-71.<br />

McWilliam, R. A., Tocci, L., & Harpin, G. H. (1998). Family-centered services:<br />

Service providers’ discourse and behavior. Topics in Early Childhood Special<br />

Education, 18, 206-221.<br />

Mehl-Matrona, L. (2003). Autism treatments: Craniosacral therapy. Retrieved<br />

May, 20, 2003 from the Internet: http://www.healingarts.org/children/craniosacral.htm#wethink<br />

Meisels, S. J., & Shonkoff, J. P. (2000). Early childhood intervention: A continuing<br />

evolution. In J. P. Shonkoff, & S. J. Meisels (Eds.), Handbook of early childhood<br />

intervention (2 nd ed.) (pp. 3-31). NY, NY: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Mellard, E. (2000). Impact of federal policy on services for children and families<br />

in early intervention programs and public schools. In W. Dunn (Ed.), Best practice<br />

occupational therapy in community services with children and families (pp. 147-156).<br />

Thorofare, NJ: Slack Incorporated.<br />

Mesibov, G. B., Adams, L., & Klinger, L. (1997). Autism: Understanding the<br />

disorder. NY, NY: Plenum Press.<br />

Mesibov, G. B., & Shea, V. (2003). The culture of autism. Retrieved April, 24,<br />

2003 from the Internet: http://www.autismuk.com/index3sub1.htm/.<br />

Michel, D. (1976). <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.<br />

Ministry of Education (1990). The universal playground. Exceptional Parent, 20<br />

(26), 28-29.<br />

Morgan, M. (1994). Mutant Message Down Under. NY, NY: Haper Collins<br />

Publisher.


Morrison, K., & Rosales-Ruiz, J. (1997). The effect of object performance on task<br />

performance and stereotypy in a child with autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities,<br />

18, 127-137.<br />

Mulvihill, B. A., Shearer, D., & Van Horn, M. L. (2002). Training, experiences, and<br />

child care providers’ perceptions of inclusion. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 17,<br />

197-215.<br />

Myles, B. S., Simpson, R. L., Ormsbee, C. K., & Erickson, C. (1993). Integrating<br />

preschool children with autism with their normally developing peers: Research findings and<br />

best practice recommendations. Focus on Autistic Behavior, 8, 1-18.<br />

Nabors, L., & Badawi, M. (1997). Playground interactions for preschool-aged<br />

children with special needs. Physical & Occupational <strong>Therapy</strong> in Pediatrics, 17 (3), 21-31.<br />

Nabors, L., Willoughby, J., Leff, S., & McMenamin, S. (2001). Promoting Inclusion<br />

for young children with special needs on playgrounds. Journal of Developmental and<br />

Physical Disabilities, 13 (2), 179-190.<br />

National Association for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC) (1996).<br />

Position statement: Developmentally appropriate practice in early childhood programs<br />

serving children from birth through age 8. Retrieved March 28, 2003 from the Internet:<br />

http://www.naeyc.org/recources/positions_statements/dap1.html/<br />

National Information Center for Children and Youth with Disabilities (NICHCY)<br />

(2003). Autism and pervasive developmental disorder: Fact sheet 1 (FS1). Retrieved<br />

April, 23, 2003 from the Internet: http://www.nichcy.org/pub/factshe/fs1txt.htm/.<br />

National Center of Birth Defects and Developmental Disabilities (CDC) (2003).<br />

About Autism. Retrieved May, 17, 2003 from the Internet:<br />

http://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/dd/aic/about/default.htm/.<br />

National Institute of Neurological Disorder and Stroke (NINDS) (2003). Autism<br />

fact sheet. Retrieved May, 17, 2003 from the Internet:<br />

http://www.ninds.nih.gov/health_and_medical/pubs/autism.htm.<br />

National Research Council (2001). Educating children with autism. Committee<br />

on educational interventions for children with autism. In C. Lord & J. P. McGee (Eds.),<br />

Division of Behavioral and Social Science and Education. Washington, DC: National<br />

Academy Press.<br />

301


302<br />

Neelly, L., Kenney, S., & Wolf, J. (2000). Start the music strategies. Reston, VA:<br />

MENC.<br />

Nelson, D. I., Anderson, V. G., Gonzales, A. D. (1984). <strong>Music</strong> activities for children<br />

with autism and other pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 21 (3),<br />

100-116.<br />

Neugebauer, L. (2001). Kann Musiktherapie körpelich behinderten Kindern helfen?<br />

[Can music therapy help the physical handicapped child?]. In D. Aldridge (Ed.), Kairos V:<br />

Musiktherapie mit Kindern: Beitraege zur Musiktherapie in der Medizin (pp. 4-11). Bern;<br />

Goettingen; Toronto; Seattle: Huber.<br />

Nicholas, M. J., & Boyle, M. E. (1983). An annotated bibliography of single case<br />

experimental research in music therapy. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> publications 1966-1982.<br />

Journal <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 20 (3), 156-63.<br />

Nordoff, P., & Robbins, C. (1968a). Clinical experiences with autistic children. In E.<br />

T. Gaston (Ed.), <strong>Music</strong> in therapy (pp.191-193). NY, NY: The McMillian Company.<br />

Nordoff, P., & Robbins C. (1968b). The second book of children’s play-songs. Bryn<br />

Mawr, PA: Theodore Presser Company.<br />

Nordoff, P., & Robbins, C. (1977). Creative <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>: Individual treatment for<br />

the handicapped child. NY, NY: John Day Books in Special Education.<br />

Nordoff, P., & Robbins, C. (1983). <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> in special education (2 nd ed.).<br />

Saint Louis, MO: MMB <strong>Music</strong>, Inc.<br />

Nordoff, P., & Robbins, C. (1992). <strong>Therapy</strong> in music for handicapped children (3 rd<br />

print). London, England: Victor Gollancz.<br />

Nordoff, P. & Robbins, C. (1995). Greetings and goodbyes: A Nordoff-Robbins<br />

collection for the classroom use. Bryn Mawr, PA: Theodore Presser Company.<br />

OchsLabs (2002). Flexyx: Development, research and improving the flexibility<br />

of human functioning through use of the Flexyx Neurotherapy System (FNS). Retrieved<br />

June, 1, 2003 from the Internet: http://www.flexyx.com/articles/index.html.


Odom, S. L. (2002). Narrowing the question: Social integration and characteristics of<br />

children with disabilities in inclusion settings. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 17,<br />

167-170.<br />

Odom, S. L., Chandler, L. K., Ostrosky, M., McConnell, S. R., & Reaney, S.<br />

(1992). Fading teacher prompts from peer-initiation interventions for young children<br />

with disabilities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25 (2), 307-317.<br />

Odom, S. L., & Diamond, K. E. (1998). Inclusion of young children with special<br />

needs in early childhood education: The research base. Early Childhood Research Quarterly,<br />

13 (1), 3-25.<br />

Odom, S. L., Hoyson, M., Jamieson, B., & Strain, P. S. (1985). Increasing<br />

handicapped preschoolers’ peer social interactions: Cross-setting and component<br />

analysis. Journal of Applied Behaviour Analysis, 19, 59-71.<br />

Odom, S. L., & Strain, P. S. (1986). A comparison of peer-initiation and teacherantecedent<br />

interventions for promoting social interactions of autistic preschoolers.<br />

Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 3, 89-103.<br />

Osborn, K., & Osborn, D. (1981). Discipline and classroom management. Athens,<br />

GA: Education Association.<br />

Palsha, S., & Wesley, P. (1998). Improving quality in early childhood environments<br />

through on-side consultation. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 18 (4), 243-253.<br />

Peck, C. A., Carlson, P., & Helmstetter, E. (1992). Parent and teacher perceptions of<br />

outcomes for typically developing children enrolled in early childhood programs: A<br />

statewide survey. Journal of Early Intervention, 16, 53-63.<br />

Peck, C. A., Odom, S. L., & Bricker, D. (1993). Integrating young children with<br />

disabilities into community programs: Ecological perspectives on research and<br />

implementation. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.<br />

Piaget, J. (1951). Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. NY, NY: Norton.<br />

Preschool Education (2003). <strong>Music</strong> & Songs: Circle Time-Clean up. Retrieved<br />

January, 16, 2003 from the Internet: http://www.preschooleducation.com/scleanup.shtml/.<br />

303


304<br />

Prizant, B., & Wetherby, A. (1998). Understanding the continuum of discretetrial<br />

traditional behavior to social-pragmatic developmental approaches in<br />

communication enhancement for young children with autism/PDD. Seminars in Speech<br />

and Language, 19, 329-353.<br />

Pollard, N. L. (1998). Development of social interaction skills in preschool children<br />

with autism: A review of the literature. Child and Family Behavior <strong>Therapy</strong>, 20 (2), 1-16.<br />

Quenville, P. (1980). Teaching a young autistic boy how to wash his hands and face.<br />

Journal of Special Education, 4 (1), 87-93.<br />

Quill, K. A. (2000). Do-Watch-Listen-Say: Social and Communication Intervention<br />

for Children with Autism. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publisher Co.<br />

Rainforth, B., & Roberts, P. (1996). Physical therapy. In R. A. McWilliam (Ed.).<br />

Rethinking pull-out services in early intervention: A professional resource (pp. 243-265).<br />

Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.<br />

Redefer, L. A., & Goodman, J. F. (1989). Pet-facilitated therapy with autistic<br />

children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19 (3), 461-467.<br />

Register, D. (2001). The effects of an early intervention music curriculum on<br />

prereading/writing. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 38 (3), 239-248.<br />

Register, D. (2002). Collaboration and consultation: A survey of board certified<br />

music therapists. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 39 (4), 305-321.<br />

Reuer, B. (1996). Posturing for the changing world: Consulting as a career option.<br />

<strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Perspectives, 14, 16-20.<br />

Ricciarelli, A. (2003). The guitar in palliative music therapy for cancer patients. In<br />

<strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Today, 4 (2) 2003. Retrieved August, 11, 2003 from the Internet:<br />

http://www.musictherapyworld.net.<br />

Rogers, F., & Judkins, J. (1997). Going to the potty. NY, NY: Paper Star.


Rogers, S. J., & Lewis, H. (1989). An effective day treatment model for young<br />

children with pervasive developmental disorder. Journal of the American Academy of<br />

Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 207-214.<br />

Rogers, S. J., Oszonoff, S., & Maslin-Cole, C. (1993). Developmental aspects of<br />

attachment behavior in young children with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of<br />

the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36 (6), 1274-1282.<br />

Ross, T. (2000). Wash your hands! Brooklyn, NY: Kane/Miller Book.<br />

Rumfelt-Wright, C. S. (2001). Keeping it clean. All Together Now! (ATN), 7 (2), 5-<br />

7. All Together Now.<br />

Ruttenberg, B. A., Dratman, M. L., Fraknoi, J., & Wenar, C. (1966). An<br />

instrument to evaluate autistic children. Journal of Child Psychiatry, 5 (3), 453.<br />

Salisbury, C. L. (1991). Mainstreaming during the early childhood years.<br />

Exceptional Children, 58, 146-155.<br />

Saint-Exupéry, A. (1943). The little prince. London, England: Harcourt, Inc.<br />

Sandall, S., McLean, M. E., & Smith, B. J. (2000). DEC: Recommended practices in<br />

early intervention/early childhood special education. Longmont, CO: Sopris West.<br />

Sandall, S., & Schwartz, I. (2002). Building blocks for teaching preschoolers with<br />

special needs. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.<br />

Sandler, A. (1997). Physical and occupational therapy services: Use of a consultative<br />

therapy model in the schools. Preventing School Failure, 41 (4), 164-165.<br />

Salz, J. (2000). The way of adventure: Transforming your life and work with<br />

spirit and vision. NY, NY: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.<br />

Saperston, B. (1973). The use of music in establishing communication with an<br />

autistic mentally retarded child. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 10, 184-188.<br />

Scar, S. (1998). American child care today. American Psychologist, 58, 95-108.<br />

305


306<br />

Schmid, W., & Janssen, W. (2001). Gruppen-Musik-Therapie [Group music<br />

therapy]. In D. Aldridge (Ed.), Kairos V: Musiktherapie mit Kindern: Beitraege zur<br />

Musiktherapie in der Medizin, 12-24. Bern; Goettingen; Toronto; Seattle: Huber.<br />

Schmidt, J. A. (1984). Structural analysis of clinical music: An important tool for<br />

music therapy and research. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 4 (1), 18-28.<br />

Schmidt, D. C., Franklin, R., & Edwards, J. S. (1997). Reinforcement of autistic<br />

children to music. Psychological Reports, 39 (2), 571-577.<br />

Schmit, J., Alper, S., Raschke, D., & Ryndak, D. (2000). Effects of using a<br />

photographic cueing package during routine school transitions with a child who has autism.<br />

Mental Retardation 38 (2) 131-37.<br />

Schnur-Ritholz, M., & Robbins, C. (1999). Themes for <strong>Therapy</strong>. New York, NY:<br />

Carl Fischer.<br />

Schopler, E. (1997). Implementation of TEACCH philosophy. In D. J. Cohen, &<br />

F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (pp.<br />

767-795). NY, NY: Wiley.<br />

Schopler, E., Brehm, S. S., Kinsbourne, M., & Reichler, R. J. (1971). Effect of<br />

treatment structure in autistic children. Archives of General Psychiatry, 24, 415-421.<br />

Schopler, E., Mesibov, G. B., & Hearsey, K. (1995). Structured teaching in the<br />

TEACCH system. In E. Schopler, & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), Learning and Cognition in<br />

Autism (pp. 243-268). NY, NY: Plenum Press.<br />

Schopler, E., Reichler, R., Bashford, A., Lansing, M., & Marcus, L. (1990).<br />

Psychoeducational Profile-revised (PEP-R). Austin, TX: Pro-ED.<br />

Schopler, E., Reichler, R., & Renner, B. (1988). The childhood autism rating<br />

scale (CARS). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological.<br />

Schreibman, L. (2000). Intensive behavioral/psychoeducational treatments for<br />

autism: Research needs and future directions. Journal of Autism and Developmental<br />

Disorders, 30 (5), 373-378.


Schuhmacher, K. (1994). Musiktherapie mit autistischen Kindern [<strong>Music</strong> therapy<br />

with autistic children]. Stuttgart, Germany: Gustav Fischer Verlag.<br />

Schuhmacher, K. (1999). Musiktherapie und Säuglingsforschung. [<strong>Music</strong><br />

therapy in infant research]. Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Peter Lang GmbH.<br />

Schweinhart, L. J., & Weikart, D. P. (1996). Lasting differences: The High/Scope<br />

preschool curriculum comparison study through age 23. Monographs of the High/Scope<br />

Educational Research Foundation, 12. Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press.<br />

Scott, J., Clark, C., & Brady, M. (2000). Students with autism: Characteristics<br />

and instruction programming. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group.<br />

Scott, M. S., McWilliam, R. A., & Mayhew, M. S. (1999). Integrating therapies<br />

into the classroom. Young Exceptional Children, 2 (3), 15-24.<br />

Sewell, T. J., Collins, B. C., Hemmeter, M. L., & Schuster, J. W. (1998). <strong>Using</strong><br />

simultaneous prompting within an activity-based format to teach dressing skills to<br />

preschoolers with developmental delays. Journal of Early Intervention, 21, 132-145.<br />

Shane, H. (Ed.) (1994). Facilitated Communication: The clinical and social<br />

phenomenon. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group.<br />

Shapiro, T., Sherman, M., Calamari, G., & Koch, D. (1987). Attachment in<br />

autism and other developmental disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child<br />

and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26 (4), 480-484.<br />

Sherwin, A. C. (1953). Reactions to music of autistic (schizophrenic) children.<br />

American Journal of Psychiatry, 109, 823-831.<br />

Shonkoff, J. P. & Meisels, S. J. (Eds.). (2000). Handbook of early childhood<br />

intervention (2 nd ed.). NY, NY: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Siegel, B. (1996). The world of the autistic child. Understanding and treating<br />

Autistic Spectrum Disorder. NY, NY: Oxford University Press.<br />

Silver, B. (2003). Making music: Native American <strong>Music</strong>. Retrieved July 29, 2003<br />

from the Internet: http://www.sbgmusic.com/html/teacher/reference.html.<br />

307


308<br />

Simpson, J. (2003). Your action matters. <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Matters, 6 (1), 3-5.<br />

Skille, O. (1989). Vibroacoustic <strong>Therapy</strong>. <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 8 (1), 61-77.<br />

Smith, D. S., & Hairston, M. J. (1999). <strong>Music</strong> therapy in school settings: Current<br />

practice. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 36 (4), 274-292.<br />

Smith, P. (1979). A comparison of different methods of toilet training the mentally<br />

handicapped. Behaviour Research & <strong>Therapy</strong>, 17, 33-34.<br />

Smith, T. (1999). Outcome of early intervention for children with autism.<br />

Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 6 (1), 33-49.<br />

Smith, T., Donahoe, P. A., & Davis, B. J. (2001). The UCLA Young Autism<br />

Project. In S. L. Harris, & J. S. Handleman (Eds.), Preschool education programs for<br />

children with autism (2 nd ed). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.<br />

Snell, A. M. (2002). <strong>Music</strong> therapy for learners with autism in a public school<br />

setting. In B. L. Wilson (Ed.), Models of music therapy interventions in school settings (2 nd<br />

ed) (pp. 211-275). Silver Spring, MD: The American <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Association, Inc.<br />

Soap and Detergent Association (1990). Hooray for handwashing: A program for<br />

early childhood settings. NY, NY: Author.<br />

Society for the Autistically Handicapped (2003a). Autism. Retrieved May, 16,<br />

2003 from the Internet: http://www.autismuk.com<br />

Society for the Autistically Handicapped (2003b). Treatments. Retrieved May,<br />

16, 2003 from the Internet: http://www.autismuk.com/index13.htm/.<br />

Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cichetti, D. C. (1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior<br />

Scales. Circler Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.<br />

Spears, J. A. (1998). Soap and water does the trick. Texas Child Care, 22 (3), 24-36.<br />

Squibb, B., & Yardley, K. (1999). Playing healthy, staying healthy: A prevention<br />

program for contagious disease. Early Childhood Education Journal, 26 (3), 143-147.


Standley, J. M., & Hughes, J. E. (1996). Documenting developmentally appropriate<br />

practice objectives and benefits of a music therapy program for early intervention: A<br />

behavioral analysis. <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Perspectives, 14 (2), 87-94.<br />

Standley, J. M., & Hughes, J. E. (1997). Evaluation of an early intervention music<br />

curriculum for enhancing prereading/writing skills. <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Perspectives, 15, 79-85.<br />

Steele, A. L. (1985). The music therapy level system: A manual of principles and<br />

applications. Cleveland, OH: The Cleveland <strong>Music</strong> School Settlement.<br />

Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. NY, NY: Basic Books.<br />

Stevens, E., & Clark, F. (1969). <strong>Music</strong> therapy in the treatment of autistic<br />

children. Journal of <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 6, 98-104.<br />

Stile, S. W. (1988). Single-subject research in early childhood special education: A<br />

survey of seven selected journals—1977-1986. Paper presented at the conference of the Rocky<br />

Mountain Education Research Association. La Cruces, NM, October 25, 1988.<br />

Stine, S. (1997). Designing landscapes for learning: Transforming school grounds<br />

into “special places.” In C. Wagner (Ed.), Proceedings: American Society of Landscape<br />

Architects (ERIC Document No. ED 432 879).<br />

Stone W. L., Coonrod, E. E., & Ousley, O. Y. (2000). Brief report: Screening<br />

tool for autism in two-year-olds (STAT): Development and preliminary data. Journal of<br />

Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30 (6), 607-612.<br />

Stone, W. L., Lee, E. B., Ashford, L., Brissie, J., Hepburn, S. L., Coonrod, E. E.,<br />

& Weiss, B. (1999). Can autism be diagnosed accurately in children under three years?<br />

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 219-226.<br />

Strain, P. S. (1983). Generalization of autistic children’s social behavior change:<br />

Effects of developmentally integrated and segregated settings. Analysis and Intervention in<br />

Developmental Disabilities, 3, 23-34.<br />

Strain, P.S., Kerr, M. M., & Ragland, E. U. (1979). Effects of peer-mediated social<br />

initiations and prompting/reinforcement procedures on the social behavior of autistic<br />

children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9 (1), 41-54.<br />

309


310<br />

Tawney, J. W., & Gast, D. L. (1984). Single subject research in special<br />

education. Columbus: Merrill.<br />

Taylor, S. I., & Morris, V. G. (1996). Outdoor play in early childhood education<br />

settings: Is it safe and healthy for children? Early Childhood Education Journal, 23 (3),<br />

153-157.<br />

Thaut, M. H. (1984). A music therapy treatment model for autistic children.<br />

<strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Perspectives, 1 (4), 7-13.<br />

Thaut, M. H. (1987). Visual versus auditory (musical) stimulus preferences in<br />

autistic children: A pilot study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17 (3),<br />

425-432.<br />

Thaut, M. H. (1988). Measuring musical responsiveness in autistic children: A<br />

comparative analysis of improvised musical tone sequences of autistic, normal and mentally<br />

retarded individuals. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18 (4), 561-571.<br />

The American Red Cross, Orange County Chapter. (1996). Don’t get sick, wash up<br />

quick. Santa Ana, CA: Author.<br />

The National Autistic Society. (2001). Fact Sheet: Daily Life therapy-Higashi.<br />

Retrieved May, 30, 2003 from the Internet:<br />

http://www.nas.org.uk/pubs/factsheet/docs/dailife.pdf<br />

The National Autistic Society. (2002). Fact Sheet: <strong>Music</strong> therapy. Retrieved<br />

May, 30, 2003 from the Internet:<br />

http://www.nas.org.uk/pubs/factsheet/docs/musictherapy.pdf<br />

Toigo, D. A. (1992). Autism: Integrating a personal perspective with music therapy<br />

practice. <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Perspectives, 10, 13-20.<br />

The Tomatis Method (2003). Autism. Retrieved May, 20, 2003 from the Internet:<br />

http://www.tomatis.com/English/Articles/autism.htm/.<br />

Treatment and Education of Autistic and related Communication handicapped<br />

Children (TEACCH). (1999). Autism Primer: Twenty questions and answers (3 rd<br />

edition). Retrieved April, 23, 2003 from the Internet:<br />

http://www.teacch.com/20ques.htm/.


Trillingsgaard, A. (1999). The script model in relation to autism. European Children<br />

Adolescence Psychiatry, 8 (1), 45-49.<br />

Turnbull, H. R., Wilcox, B. L., & Stowe, M. (2002). A brief overview of special<br />

education law with focus on autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32<br />

(5), 479-493.<br />

U.S. Consumer Product Safety Commission (1997). Handbook for public playground<br />

safety (Publication No. 325). Washington, DC: U.S. Consumer Product Safety Commission.<br />

Venn, M. L., Wolery, M., Werts, M. G., Morris, A., DeCesare, L. D., & Cuffs, M. S.<br />

(1993). Embedding instruction in art activities to teach preschoolers with disabilities to<br />

imitate their peers. Early Childhood Quarterly, 8, 277-294.<br />

Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in Society: The development of higher psychological<br />

progresses. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.<br />

Wakeford, L. (2002). Making transition easier…. Unpublished Paper. FPG<br />

Newsletter, December, 2002.<br />

Wallace, N. E. (2001). Count down to clean up. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.<br />

Warlick, K. R. (2000). Letter for Andrea H. Farbman as reprinted in <strong>Music</strong><br />

<strong>Therapy</strong> Matters, 3 (2), 29-32.<br />

Warwick, A. (1995). <strong>Music</strong> therapy in the education service: Research with<br />

autistic children and their mother. In T. Wigram, B. Saperston, & R. West (Eds.), The<br />

art and science of music therapy: A handbook (pp. 209-225). Chur, Switzerland:<br />

Harwood Academic Publisher.<br />

Watson, L., Lord, C., Schaffer, B., & Schopler, E. (1988). Teaching spontaneous<br />

communication to autistic and developmentally handicapped children. NY, NY:<br />

Irvington Press.<br />

Welch, M. (1988). Holding time. London, England: Century.<br />

Wheeler, M. (1998). Toilet training for individuals with autism and related<br />

disorders. Arlington, TX: Future Horizons.<br />

311


312<br />

Wigram T. (1995). A model of assessment and differential diagnosis of handicap<br />

in children through the medium of music therapy. In T. Wigram, B. Saperston, & R.<br />

West (Eds.), The art and science of music therapy: A handbook (pp. 181-193). Chur,<br />

Switzerland: Harwood Academic Publisher.<br />

Wigram, T. (2000). A method of music therapy assessment for the diagnosis of<br />

autism and communication disorders in children. <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Perspectives, 18 (1),<br />

13-22.<br />

Wigram, T. A. (2002). Diagnostische Evaluierung durch Musiktherapie: Evaluierung<br />

und Diagnostizierung von Kommunikationsstörungen und Autismus durch Analyse<br />

musikalischer Materials [Diagnostic evaluation of music therapy: Evaluation and diagnostic<br />

of communication disorders and autism by analyzing musical material]. In D. Aldridge, &<br />

M. Dembski (Eds.), <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> <strong>World</strong>: Musiktherapie Diagnostik und Wahrnehmung,<br />

72-106. Witten/Herdecke, Germany: Private Universitate Witten/Herdecke gGmbH.<br />

Wilcox, M. J., Kouri, T. A., & Caswell, S. B. (1991). Early language intervention: A<br />

comparison of classroom and individual treatment. American Journal of Speech-Language<br />

Pathology, 1, 49-61.<br />

Wilcox, M. J., & Shannon, M. S. (1996). Integrated early intervention practices<br />

in speech-language pathology. In R. A. McWilliam (Ed.), Rethinking pull-out services in<br />

early intervention: A professional resource (pp. 217-242). Baltimore, MD: Paul H.<br />

Brookes Publisher Co.<br />

Willer, B., Hofferthy, S., Kisker, E., Divine-Hawkins, P., Farguhar, E., & Glantz,<br />

F. (1991). The demand and supply of child care in 1990. Washington, DC: National<br />

Association for the Education of Young Children.<br />

Williams, D. (1992). Nobody nowhere: The extraordinary autobiography of an<br />

autistic. MY, NY: Times Books.<br />

Williams, D. (1996). Autism: An inside- out approach. London, Bristol, PA: Jessica<br />

Kingsley.<br />

Wilson, B. L. (Ed.). (2002). Models of music therapy interventions in school<br />

settings (2 nd ed). Silver Spring, MD: The American <strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong> Association, Inc.


Wimpory, D. Chadwick, P., & Nash, S. (1995). Brief report: <strong>Music</strong>al Interaction<br />

<strong>Therapy</strong> for children with autism: An evaluative case study with two-year follow-up.<br />

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25 (5), 541-552.<br />

Wolery, M. (1992). Teaching students with moderate and severe disabilities: Use<br />

of response prompting strategies. White Plains, NY: Longman.<br />

Wolery, M. (1996). Early childhood special and general education. In R. A.<br />

McWilliam (Ed.), Rethinking pull-out services in early intervention: A professional<br />

resource (pp. 185-216). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.<br />

Wolery, M. (2000). Behavioral and educational approaches to early intervention.<br />

In J. P. Shonkoff & S. J. Meisel (Eds.), Handbook of early childhood intervention (pp.<br />

179-203). NY, NY: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Wolery, M., Anthony, L., Caldwell, N. K., Snyder, E. D., & Morgante, J. D.<br />

(2002). Embedding and distributing constant time delay in circle time and transitions.<br />

Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 22, 14-25.<br />

Wolery, M., Bailey, D. B., & Sugai, G. M. (1988). Effective teaching: Principles<br />

and procedures of applied behavior analysis with exceptional students. Boston, MA:<br />

Allyn & Bacon.<br />

Wolery, M., & Bredecamp, S. (1994). Developmental appropriate practice and young<br />

children with special needs: Contextual issues in the discussion. Journal of Early<br />

Intervention, 18, 331-341.<br />

Wolery, M., & Garfinkel, A. N. (2002). Measuring in intervention research with<br />

young children who have autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32<br />

(5), 463-478.<br />

Wolery, M., Strain P. S., & Bailey, D. B. (1992). Reaching potential of children with<br />

special needs. In S. Bredekamp, & T. Rosegrant (Eds.), Reaching potentials: Appropriate<br />

curriculum and assessment for young children (Vol. 2) (pp. 92-111). Washington, D.C.:<br />

National Association for the Education of young Children.<br />

Wolery, M., Watson, L., Garfinkel, A. N., Marcus, L., & Coburn, J. (2001).<br />

Replication Manual: Center-Based Early Intervention Demonstration Project for<br />

Children with Autism. Unpublished Manual.<br />

313


314<br />

Wolery, M., & Wilbers, J. (Eds.) (1994). Including children with special need in<br />

early childhood programs. Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of<br />

Young Children.<br />

Wolfe, D., & Horn, C. (1993). Use of melodies as a structural prompt for<br />

learning and retention of sequential verbal information by preschool students. Journal of<br />

<strong>Music</strong> <strong>Therapy</strong>, 30 (2), 100-118.<br />

<strong>World</strong> Health Organization (WHO) (1993). Classification of mental and<br />

behavioral disorders: Diagnostic criteria for researchers. Geneva, Switzerland: Author.<br />

Wortham, S. C., & Wortham, M. R. (1989). Designing creative play<br />

environments: Infant/toddler development and play. Childhood Education, 65 (5), 295-<br />

299.<br />

Young, R. L., & Nettelbeck, T. (1995). The ability of a musical savant and his<br />

family. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25 (3), 231-248.


Appendix A: Data Sheet Experiment I.<br />

Name: ___________________________<br />

Date: ____________________________<br />

Observer: _________________________<br />

Condition: ________________________<br />

Start Time: _______________________<br />

Stop Time: _______________________<br />

Total Time: _______________________<br />

APPENDICES<br />

Data Sheet<br />

Step Adult Child<br />

Behavior Behavior<br />

1. Phillip enters the classroom + - I R NR E X<br />

2. Phillip says “Hello” with symbol<br />

(or word) to a teacher or peer<br />

+ - I R NR E X<br />

3. Phillip says “Hello” with symbol<br />

(or word) to a teacher or peer<br />

+ - I R NR E X<br />

4. Phillip waves good bye to Mom + - I R NR E X<br />

5. Phillip picks up a toy or material + - I R NR E X<br />

+ = Prompt<br />

- = No prompt<br />

I = Independent Response<br />

R = Prompted Response<br />

NR = No response (to request, routine or prompt)<br />

E = Error<br />

X = Inappropriate response<br />

FIELD NOTES:<br />

315


316<br />

Name: _________________________<br />

Date: __________________________<br />

Observer: _______________________<br />

Condition: ______________________<br />

Start Time: _____________________<br />

Stop Time: _____________________<br />

Total Time: _____________________<br />

Data Sheet<br />

Step Adult Child<br />

Behavior Behavior<br />

1. Ben enters the classroom + - I R NR E X<br />

2. Ben says “Hello” with symbol<br />

or word to a teacher or peer<br />

+ - I R NR E X<br />

3. Ben says “Hello” with symbol<br />

or word to a teacher or peer<br />

+ - I R NR E X<br />

4. Ben waves or says good bye to Krista + - I R NR E X<br />

5. Ben picks up a toy or material<br />

Number of peers greeting Ben<br />

+ - I R NR E X<br />

+ = Prompt<br />

- = No prompt<br />

I = Independent Response<br />

R = Prompted Response<br />

NR = No response (to request, routine or prompt)<br />

E = Error<br />

X = Inappropriate response<br />

FIELD NOTES:


Appendix B: Data Sheet Experiment II.<br />

Name:<br />

Date:<br />

Observer:<br />

Task #1: Hand washing<br />

Condition:<br />

STEPS<br />

Turn on water<br />

Wet hands<br />

Get soap<br />

Rub hands<br />

Rinse soap<br />

Turn water off<br />

Dry hands<br />

Did it Did not<br />

do it<br />

Task #2: Toilet Training<br />

Condition:<br />

STEPS<br />

Get up<br />

Walk to toilet<br />

Climb step<br />

Pants down<br />

Sit down<br />

Pee<br />

Get up<br />

T-shirt up<br />

Pants up<br />

Flush<br />

Did it Did not<br />

do it<br />

Task #3: Clean up<br />

Condition:<br />

STEPS<br />

Get up<br />

Pick up s.th.<br />

Put s.th. away<br />

Pick up s.th.<br />

Put s.th. away<br />

Go where<br />

directed by<br />

teacher<br />

Did it Did not<br />

do it<br />

FIELD NOTES:<br />

Data Evaluation Sheet<br />

Did it with Negative Verbalizations<br />

prompts YES NO<br />

Did it with Negative Verbalizations<br />

prompts YES NO<br />

Did it with Negative Verbalizations<br />

prompts YES NO<br />

Escapes Skipped<br />

the part<br />

Escapes Skipped<br />

the part<br />

Escapes Skipped<br />

the part<br />

317


318<br />

Appendix C: Data Sheet Experiment III.<br />

Date ______________ Children ________________ /_______________ Observer_________________ Condition_____________<br />

Adults<br />

Interaction of peer Interaction of child with autism Play and<br />

Engagement<br />

Interval<br />

Interaction<br />

Supported<br />

by Adults<br />

With Material<br />

and<br />

Equipment<br />

Stays in the<br />

MH<br />

Positive<br />

Interaction<br />

Initiates<br />

Interaction<br />

Stays in the<br />

MH<br />

Positive<br />

Interaction<br />

Initiates<br />

Interaction<br />

1.1<br />

1.2<br />

1.3<br />

1.4<br />

2.1<br />

2.2<br />

2.3<br />

2.4<br />

3.1<br />

3.2<br />

3.3<br />

3.4<br />

4.1<br />

4.2<br />

4.3<br />

4.4<br />

5.1<br />

5.2<br />

5.3<br />

5.4


319<br />

Adults<br />

Interaction of peer Interaction of child with autism Play and<br />

Engagement<br />

Interval<br />

Interaction<br />

Supported<br />

by Adults<br />

With Material<br />

and<br />

Equipment<br />

Stays in the<br />

MH<br />

Positive<br />

Interaction<br />

Initiates<br />

Interaction<br />

Stays in the<br />

MH<br />

Positive<br />

Interaction<br />

Initiates<br />

Interaction<br />

Sub-Total #<br />

6.1<br />

6.2<br />

6.3<br />

6.4<br />

7.1<br />

7.2<br />

7.3<br />

7.4<br />

8.1<br />

8.2<br />

8.3<br />

8.4<br />

9.1<br />

9.2<br />

9.3<br />

9.4<br />

10.1<br />

10.2<br />

10.3<br />

10.4<br />

Total #


320<br />

Condition C: Teacher’s Tasks Following (Behaviors)<br />

Tasks Yes<br />

1. Enter <strong>Music</strong> Hut with the dyad<br />

2. Initiate to play and include both children on the children’s level<br />

3. Sing the song with the dyad<br />

4. Model tasks of song: verbalization, body contact, play together, turn taking,<br />

choose instrument, guide to other activities in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut<br />

5. Continue to play in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut (total length of 10 minutes)<br />

Total #<br />

Condition D: Peer’s Task Following (Behaviors)<br />

Tasks Yes<br />

1. Enter the <strong>Music</strong> Hut together with the child with autism<br />

2. Initiate to play and include child with autism<br />

3. Sing the song with the child with autism<br />

4. Model tasks of song: verbalization, body contact, play together, turn taking,<br />

choose instrument, guide to other activities in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut<br />

5. Continue to play in the <strong>Music</strong> Hut (total length of 10 minutes)<br />

Total #<br />

Field Notes:


LEBENSLAUF<br />

________________________________________________________________<br />

PETRA KERN GEBURTSDATUM 28.9.1968<br />

STAATSANGEHÖRIGKEIT Deutsch<br />

GEBURTSORT Aalen<br />

SCHULBILDUNG<br />

1975 - 1979 Grundschule, Aalen<br />

1979 - 1985 Schubart-Gymnasium, Aalen<br />

1985 - 1988 Haushalts- und Ernährungswissenschaftliches Gymnasium, Aalen<br />

VORPRAKTIKUM<br />

1988 - 1989 Siftung „Haus Lindenhof“, Schwäbisch-Gmünd, Einrichtung<br />

für behinderte und alte Menschen<br />

STUDIUM<br />

1989 Studium der Medizin an der Eberhard-Karls-Universität Tübingen<br />

1989 - 1994 Studiengang Sozialwesen, Schwerpunkt Musiktherapie an der Fachhochschule<br />

Würzburg-Schweinfurt<br />

1997 Musiktherapeutin BVM; Anerkennung durch den Berufsverband der<br />

Musiktherapeutinnen und Musiktherapeuten in Deutschland e.V.<br />

2001 <strong>Music</strong> Therapist, MT-BC; Anerkennung durch das American Certification<br />

Board for <strong>Music</strong> Therapists, Inc., USA.<br />

BERUFLICHER WERDEGANG<br />

1994 - 1995 Musiktherapeutin im Kreis-Alten- und Pflegeheim, Würzburg<br />

1995 Musiktherapeutin im Kuratorium „Wohnen im Alter“, Albstift Aalen<br />

1996 - 1997 Musiktherapeutin in der Stiftung „Haus Lindenhof“, Schwäbisch-Gmünd<br />

1996 - 1998 Musiktherapeutin an der Interdisziplinären Frühförderstelle,<br />

Sozialpädagogisches Zentrum St. Canisius, Schwäbisch-Gmünd<br />

Seit 1999 Research Scholar am Frank Porter Graham Child Development Institute,<br />

University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, USA<br />

LEHR-UND FORTBILDUNGSTÄTIGKEITEN<br />

1991 - 1992 Instrumentallehrerin für Klavier an der Musikschule Oberkochen<br />

1996 - 1998 Kursleiterin des Familienbildungswerks, Aalen<br />

1996 - 1999 Lehrbeauftragte an der Berufsfachschule für Altenpflege, DAG Aalen<br />

1996 - 2000 Referentin bei der Fortbildungsdozentur Süd, Fortbildungsmaßnahmen für<br />

Einrichtungen der Behindertenhilfe, Schwäbisch Hall<br />

1996-2001 Lehrbeauftragte bei der Ganzheitlichen Bildung, Weiterbildungszweig<br />

Altentherapeut/Altentherapeutin, Nürnberg<br />

Seit 2001 Lehrbeauftragte bei Pluspunkt, Weiterbildung in der Altenpflege, Nürnberg

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!