29.08.2013 Views

Minimality Effects in Syntax · The MLC and Derivational Economy ...

Minimality Effects in Syntax · The MLC and Derivational Economy ...

Minimality Effects in Syntax · The MLC and Derivational Economy ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

94 Gisbert Fanselow<br />

Two remarks are <strong>in</strong> order before we can discuss possible analyses for (35).<br />

First, it is often hard to determ<strong>in</strong>e whether a language tolerates superiority<br />

violations or not. When I asked 22 Dutch l<strong>in</strong>guists via the <strong>in</strong>ternet to rate<br />

(36), five accepted it <strong>and</strong> seven found it questionable, while ten speakers<br />

rejected the sentence. It not very plausible that this judgment pattern lends<br />

support to the claim that there is a categorial difference between, say, Dutch<br />

<strong>and</strong> German with respect to superiority. Likewise, it is not obvious what the<br />

marg<strong>in</strong>ality of (37) implies for the status of superiority <strong>in</strong> French.<br />

(36) Dutch superiority<br />

#ik weet niet wat wie gekocht heeft<br />

I know not what who bought has<br />

“I do not know who has bought what”<br />

(37) French superiority<br />

?Je me dem<strong>and</strong>e à qui a parlé qui<br />

I me wonder to whom has talked who<br />

“I wonder who has talked to whom”<br />

Instead of forc<strong>in</strong>g (36) <strong>and</strong> (37) <strong>in</strong>to one or the other category, the graded<br />

nature of such <strong>MLC</strong> violations should figure <strong>in</strong> the analysis of the construction.<br />

3 This is particularly true <strong>in</strong> the light of experimental f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs concern<strong>in</strong>g<br />

judgements by l<strong>in</strong>guistically naive <strong>in</strong>formants. We compared structures such<br />

as (38a) <strong>and</strong> (38b) <strong>in</strong> a questionnaire study <strong>and</strong> found a highly significant<br />

difference between multiple questions that respect the <strong>MLC</strong> <strong>and</strong> those that<br />

do not. Structures violat<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>MLC</strong> were rated worse than those respect<strong>in</strong>g<br />

it (4.8 vs. 2.34) on a 1-6 scale (1: perfect, 6: completely ungrammatical) by<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guistically naive <strong>in</strong>formants.<br />

(38) a. Wer besucht wen <strong>in</strong> der Villa? 2.34<br />

who visited whom <strong>in</strong> the villa<br />

b. wen besucht wer <strong>in</strong> der Villa? 4.80<br />

Given that the syntax literature states more or less unanimously that German<br />

lacks simple superiority effects, such f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are a bit surpris<strong>in</strong>g at first<br />

glance, but they are <strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>e with those obta<strong>in</strong>ed by Featherston (2002a,b),<br />

<strong>and</strong> they reappeared <strong>in</strong> a very similar shape <strong>in</strong> our questionnaire studies<br />

concern<strong>in</strong>g Polish <strong>and</strong> Russian.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!