10.07.2015 Views

Special Issue Survey of Cetaceans in Captive Care

Special Issue Survey of Cetaceans in Captive Care

Special Issue Survey of Cetaceans in Captive Care

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

2. Whales, Dolph<strong>in</strong>s, and Porpoises: Presentation <strong>of</strong> the <strong>Cetaceans</strong> 295<strong>of</strong> 500 Hz to 100 kHz, whereas humans hear wellfrom about 200 Hz to 17 kHz. The majority <strong>of</strong>odontocetes have peak sensitivities <strong>in</strong> the ultrasonicranges, although most have moderate sensitivityfrom 1 kHz to 20 kHz and no acute hear<strong>in</strong>gbelow 500 Hz (< 80 dB re 1 µPa). Models <strong>in</strong>dicatethat the mysticetes’ functional hear<strong>in</strong>g range commonlyextends down to 20 Hz. Several species areexpected to hear well <strong>in</strong>to <strong>in</strong>frasonic frequencies,from 20 Hz to 30 kHz (Ketten, 1998).The odontocete larynx does not possess vocalcords. Instead, sounds are produced by nasalsacs <strong>in</strong> the blowhole region and controlled byorgans called monkey lips (Cranford et al., 1997;Degollada et al., 1998). The fatty melon abovethe skull acts as an acoustical lens focus<strong>in</strong>g andamplify<strong>in</strong>g these sounds <strong>in</strong>to a beam. Receivedsounds are transmitted to the <strong>in</strong>ner ear by thebones and fatty channels <strong>of</strong> the lower jaw. Manyodontocetes communicate with whistles andburst-pulse sounds. Whistles are cont<strong>in</strong>uous, frequency-modulatedpure tones, vary<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>tensityand pattern, with one or more harmonics. The frequency<strong>of</strong> bottlenose dolph<strong>in</strong> whistles is generallybetween 4 and 24 kHz (Herman, 1980). Dolph<strong>in</strong>scan whistle and echolocate at the same time. Burstpulsesounds are another type <strong>of</strong> communicationsounds. They are broadband signals resembl<strong>in</strong>gmoans, trills, grunts, squeaks, and creak<strong>in</strong>g doors.Sperm whales, members <strong>of</strong> the porpoise family,and members <strong>of</strong> the river dolph<strong>in</strong> family appearto produce only clicks and bursts <strong>of</strong> clicks, whichmay function for both echolocation and communication(Evans, 1987). Acoustical <strong>in</strong>terference canhave a dramatic effect on the behaviour and physiology<strong>of</strong> captive animals. Loud, human-made,underwater noises with<strong>in</strong> their hear<strong>in</strong>g frequenciescan adversely affect cetaceans (Richardson et al.,1995). Acoustical <strong>in</strong>terference, which cetaceanscannot escape <strong>in</strong> captivity, can cause endocr<strong>in</strong>echanges, <strong>in</strong>creased aggression, decreased appetite,and irreversible hear<strong>in</strong>g loss (Stoskopf & Gibbons,1994). The quality <strong>of</strong> their acoustical environment<strong>in</strong> human care is critical and <strong>of</strong>ten has beenneglected. It is important to suppress human-madebackground noise, and to design a habitat thatreduces sound reverberation (see Chapter 5 fordetails).CognitionSenses are used by animals to understand their environment.The cognitive characteristics <strong>of</strong> dolph<strong>in</strong>sand whales are reflected <strong>in</strong> how the <strong>in</strong>formation isselected, encoded, stored, analysed, and retrievedby various sensory receptors. Little is known aboutbaleen whales’ cognitive abilities, but those <strong>of</strong> thebottlenose dolph<strong>in</strong>s have been studied for manyyears. The bottlenose dolph<strong>in</strong> is a quick learner <strong>in</strong>most types <strong>of</strong> auditory tasks, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g those hav<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>tricate conceptual demands. Its auditory memoryis very faithful (Herman, 1980). The dolph<strong>in</strong> iscapable <strong>of</strong> form<strong>in</strong>g and generalis<strong>in</strong>g concept rules.Dolph<strong>in</strong>s also seem pr<strong>of</strong>icient at imitative behaviours,and they are able to perform both vocal andmotor mimicry, demonstrat<strong>in</strong>g that they can learn byobservation. Despite the fact that attempts to demonstratethe existence <strong>of</strong> a natural language havefailed, dolph<strong>in</strong>s may be capable <strong>of</strong> comprehend<strong>in</strong>ga simple artificial language. Gestural language comprehensionseems to be better if the visual <strong>in</strong>formationmanipulated is dynamic (Herman, 1980, 1986,1989). The extensive development <strong>of</strong> the dolph<strong>in</strong>’sbra<strong>in</strong> and the result<strong>in</strong>g cognitive skills have evolvedfrom the demands <strong>of</strong> social liv<strong>in</strong>g, and accord<strong>in</strong>gto Herman (1989), they require “the acquisitionand use <strong>of</strong> knowledge to facilitate an exchange <strong>of</strong><strong>in</strong>formation with<strong>in</strong> a mutually dependant social network.”In other words, cetaceans may have developedtheir complex cognitive abilities because <strong>of</strong>the need for high flexibility and “communality” <strong>in</strong>their social organisation and behaviour (Johnson &Norris, 1986).Social Life and BehaviourWhales and dolph<strong>in</strong>s are essentially social animals.Group size varies greatly among species, be<strong>in</strong>ganywhere from two to several thousand <strong>in</strong>dividuals;however, common social patterns and behaviourshave been identified. Liv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> groups hasseveral advantages: it maximises forag<strong>in</strong>g, helpswith defence aga<strong>in</strong>st predators, br<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>dividualstogether for reproduction, and <strong>in</strong>creases the efficiency<strong>of</strong> calf rear<strong>in</strong>g (Evans, 1987). Some species,such as the killer whale, show stronger social cohesionthan others. <strong>Cetaceans</strong> do not form stable longtermmale-female bonds, as wolves do for example.The basis <strong>of</strong> the social unit is the reproductivefemale; most <strong>of</strong> the stable and long-last<strong>in</strong>g social<strong>in</strong>teractions that are ongo<strong>in</strong>g with<strong>in</strong> a group arebetween adult females and their <strong>of</strong>fspr<strong>in</strong>g (Sweeney,1990). In bottlenose dolph<strong>in</strong>s, multigenerationalfemale bands tend to be composed <strong>of</strong> females whoare related or who share long histories <strong>of</strong> associationwith<strong>in</strong> a common home range. Females at thesame stage <strong>of</strong> their reproductive cycle tend to swimtogether. Female cetaceans care for their <strong>of</strong>fspr<strong>in</strong>guntil they are weaned. Bottlenose dolph<strong>in</strong> calvestypically rema<strong>in</strong> with their mother for three to sixyears (Wells & Scott, 1999). A mother and youngpair may rema<strong>in</strong> together on an <strong>in</strong>termittent basisfor long periods. Bottlenose dolph<strong>in</strong>s may return toeach other <strong>in</strong> times <strong>of</strong> stress many years after the<strong>of</strong>fspr<strong>in</strong>g have reached adulthood (Norris & Dohl,1980). Mothers cont<strong>in</strong>ue to care for their younguntil the follow<strong>in</strong>g pregnancy and some even longer.Other females sometimes assist the mother dur<strong>in</strong>g

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!