13.07.2015 Views

the psychology of learning and motivation - Percepts and Concepts ...

the psychology of learning and motivation - Percepts and Concepts ...

the psychology of learning and motivation - Percepts and Concepts ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Applying <strong>the</strong> Science <strong>of</strong> Learning to Multimedia Instruction 93performed better on transfer tests when <strong>the</strong>y learned from a narratedanimation explaining how some system works than from <strong>the</strong> same narratedanimation along with concurrent on-screen text that was identicalto <strong>the</strong> words in <strong>the</strong> narration. The median effect size was d = 0.72 favoring<strong>the</strong> nonredundant lessons (i.e., narration <strong>and</strong> animation without onscreentext). For example, students learned better with a narratedanimation on lightning formation than from <strong>the</strong> same narrated animationwith concurrent on-screen text (Mayer, Heiser, et al., 2001,Experiments 1 <strong>and</strong> 2; Moreno & Mayer, 2002a, Experiment 2).Similarly, students learned better from an on-screen agent in a botanycomputer game when <strong>the</strong> agent narrated an animation ra<strong>the</strong>r than when<strong>the</strong> agent narrated an animation with concurrent on-screen captions(Moreno & Mayer, 2002b, Experiments 2a <strong>and</strong> 2b). There is somepreliminary evidence that <strong>the</strong> redundancy principle is strongest when<strong>the</strong> on-screen text is long <strong>and</strong> placed far from <strong>the</strong> graphic (Mayer &Johnson, 2008) or when <strong>the</strong> spoken text is presented before <strong>the</strong> printedtext (Kalyuga, Ch<strong>and</strong>ler, & Sweller, 2004). Sweller (2005b) has a broaderdefinition <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> redundancy principle in multimedia <strong>learning</strong>, but reportssimilar findings.4.2.4. Spatial Contiguity PrincipleConsider a lesson consisting <strong>of</strong> graphics <strong>and</strong> printed text, presented ei<strong>the</strong>ras an annotated narration on a computer screen or a series <strong>of</strong> annotatedframes in a book. Common practice is to place <strong>the</strong> words at <strong>the</strong> bottom <strong>of</strong><strong>the</strong> screen or figure as a caption or to present <strong>the</strong> words in a paragraph withdirections to ‘‘see <strong>the</strong> figure.’’ According to <strong>the</strong> cognitive <strong>the</strong>ory <strong>of</strong>multimedia <strong>learning</strong>, this can create an extraneous overload situation inwhich <strong>the</strong> learner must visually scan back <strong>and</strong> forth between <strong>the</strong> printedwords <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> corresponding part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> graphic. The required scanning isa form <strong>of</strong> extraneous processing that leaves less cognitive capacity foressential <strong>and</strong> generative processing. A solution to this kind <strong>of</strong> extraneousoverload situation is to move segments <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> printed text next to <strong>the</strong> part<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> corresponding part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> graphic, so <strong>the</strong> need for visual scanning isreduced.The spatial contiguity principle is that people learn better from a multimedialesson when corresponding words <strong>and</strong> pictures are placed nearra<strong>the</strong>r than far from one ano<strong>the</strong>r on <strong>the</strong> screen or page. In five out <strong>of</strong> fiveexperimental comparisons, students learned better from integrated lessons(with words placed near <strong>the</strong> part <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> graphic <strong>the</strong>y described) than fromseparated lessons (with words placed as captions or in paragraphs far from <strong>the</strong>graphic). The median effect size was d = 1.12 favoring <strong>the</strong> integrated lessons.For example, students learned better from an annotated animation onlightning when <strong>the</strong> on-screen text was placed within <strong>the</strong> graphic next to<strong>the</strong> element it described ra<strong>the</strong>r than at <strong>the</strong> bottom <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> screen as a caption

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!