13.07.2015 Views

the psychology of learning and motivation - Percepts and Concepts ...

the psychology of learning and motivation - Percepts and Concepts ...

the psychology of learning and motivation - Percepts and Concepts ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

292 Jose P. Mestre et al.acknowledge that identifying <strong>and</strong> justifying principles is a higher levelproblem solving skill that is difficult for students, <strong>and</strong> students needappropriate scaffolding to learn how to do this. A promising addition to<strong>the</strong> approach includes using syn<strong>the</strong>sizing concept diagrams to emphasize<strong>the</strong> main ideas learned in a course (Bagno & Eylon, 1997).5. CONCLUDING REMARKSWe have reviewed different implementations <strong>of</strong> CPS with university<strong>and</strong> high school students both in carefully controlled laboratory studies aswell as in <strong>the</strong> messy environment <strong>of</strong> real college <strong>and</strong> high school classrooms.The common feature across all <strong>of</strong> our implementations <strong>of</strong> CPSwas <strong>the</strong> emphasis on conceptual analyses <strong>of</strong> problems, in particularattempting to illustrate how conceptual knowledge is used to solve problems<strong>and</strong> to make more explicit some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> tacit knowledge used byexperts in solving problems. For example, justifying why a particularprinciple could be applied to solve a problem by examining <strong>the</strong> problem’sstory line (question asked, context, objects in <strong>the</strong> problem <strong>and</strong> attributespresent such as friction) is something that experts do naturally but is a skillthat is not overtly taught in traditional instruction.In all three implementations reviewed, students who practiced CPSshowed advantages in conceptual measures as well as in problem solving.Also heartening is <strong>the</strong> fact that <strong>the</strong>re is an element <strong>of</strong> robustness in CPS;despite <strong>the</strong> relatively short intervention in <strong>the</strong> three high schools (abouteight 50-min classes over a 4-month period), <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> wide variation inteachers’ implementation, CPS students displayed benefits from <strong>the</strong> intervention.Additional good news is that CPS can be ‘‘blended’’ into a coursewithout major curricular changes, redesign, or disruptions. However,CPS is an approach that dem<strong>and</strong>s more work from students than is typicalin traditional physics classes (e.g., deep thinking about concepts; morewriting <strong>of</strong> prose about abstract concepts <strong>and</strong> how <strong>the</strong>y apply to problemsolving—both uncommon in physics classes), which is likely to meet withmore resistance from students than traditional instructional approaches toproblem solving.An interesting final question to entertain is What next? This, webelieve, is a more meaningful question at <strong>the</strong> high school level than at<strong>the</strong> college level. Our early work exploring ways to help students gainconceptual underst<strong>and</strong>ing as well as similar early work by Larkin, Reif <strong>and</strong><strong>the</strong>ir collaborators led many o<strong>the</strong>rs to apply results from physics educationresearch to <strong>the</strong> design <strong>of</strong> curricular reforms in introductory physics instruction.Ano<strong>the</strong>r catalyst to this reform was <strong>the</strong> availability <strong>of</strong> concept inventories(Hestenes, Wells, & Swackhamer, 1992; Thornton & Sokol<strong>of</strong>f,1998) that measured students’ underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> basic physics concepts

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!