02.04.2013 Views

The psychopathology of everyday art: a quantitative Study - World ...

The psychopathology of everyday art: a quantitative Study - World ...

The psychopathology of everyday art: a quantitative Study - World ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>The</strong> <strong>psychopathology</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>everyday</strong> <strong>art</strong>:<br />

a <strong>quantitative</strong> study<br />

by<br />

Suzanne Hacking<br />

November 1999<br />

A dissertation submitted<br />

in fulfilment for the degree <strong>of</strong><br />

Doctor <strong>of</strong> Philosophy<br />

Dep<strong>art</strong>ment <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry<br />

School <strong>of</strong> Postgraduate Medicine<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Keele<br />

i


Abstract<br />

Analysis <strong>of</strong> psychiatric <strong>art</strong>work has been largely based on its content or verbal reports <strong>of</strong> its meaning. This<br />

thesis presents an alternative approach to <strong>psychopathology</strong> <strong>of</strong> paintings, in the development <strong>of</strong> a new<br />

instrument, the Descriptive Assessment for Psychiatric Artwork (DAPA). This thesis reports the<br />

steps taken to validate the DAPA: through examination <strong>of</strong> the literature, initial study, reliability study and<br />

the main comparison <strong>of</strong> 86 patients with disabling psychiatric disorder and controls.<br />

<strong>The</strong> surface distribution <strong>of</strong> 12 operationally defined characteristics <strong>of</strong> paintings was recorded: 10<br />

formal (red, yellow, green, brown, blue, black, intensity, painted and drawn line, and space covered by<br />

media) and 2 content characteristics (subjective emotional tone and dominant form) using a grid <strong>of</strong> twenty<br />

rectangles placed over the picture. <strong>The</strong>se scores were collapsed to produce average scores for each painting.<br />

All paintings within subject were also collapsed to produce a subject pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong> 12 continuous scales.<br />

Reliability assessment between six independent raters and the author were excellent. Intraclass<br />

correlations were 0.86-0.99 and Cronbach's alpha 0.91-0.99.<br />

Design: Subjects were classified using ICD-10 diagnostic classifications for research (depression,<br />

schizophrenia, personality disorder, substance abuse). 1-way ANOVAs were performed with post-hoc<br />

comparisons using the Duncan procedure. Discriminant analysis predicted patient/control classification<br />

and controlled interactions between variables.<br />

Results: ANOVA showed highly significant differences (p


Table <strong>of</strong> Contents<br />

Page Number<br />

Chapter One: Introduction<br />

1. Organisation <strong>of</strong> thesis<br />

1<br />

2. <strong>The</strong> argument for another <strong>art</strong> assessment 2<br />

Chapter Two: Literature Review<br />

1. Is there a way through the jungle? What kind <strong>of</strong> literature is<br />

relevant to this study?<br />

Literature search methods and exclusion criteria 30<br />

Impressionistic and theoretical studies 35<br />

Artists: Are they special cases? 42<br />

2. Research Studies<br />

Case and series studies 47<br />

Expression <strong>of</strong> feelings 49<br />

Changes or signs in content or form <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>work 54<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapeutic Relationship 63<br />

Summary <strong>of</strong> case studies 65<br />

Controlled studies 69<br />

Analysis 71<br />

Summary 87<br />

Validity analysis <strong>of</strong> the literature 88<br />

Summary and conclusion 93<br />

Reliability analysis <strong>of</strong> the literature 95<br />

Summary <strong>of</strong> reliability study 136<br />

3. Conclusion and Summary 141<br />

Chapter Three: Positive Thinking: What are the common<br />

psychiatric characteristics <strong>of</strong> paintings?<br />

Meta Analysis <strong>of</strong> reliable studies identified by the literature 143<br />

Conclusion for meta analysis 158<br />

Summary <strong>of</strong> findings from the common results 158<br />

Chapter Four: <strong>The</strong> Descriptive Assessment for Psychiatric Art<br />

<strong>The</strong> ideal characteristics for a new test: Development <strong>of</strong> the DAPA 162<br />

<strong>The</strong> DAPA test<br />

Illustration <strong>of</strong> scoring procedure and rating guide 171-2<br />

Methods Section 173<br />

1. <strong>The</strong> Main <strong>Study</strong> 173<br />

Sample 174<br />

Instruments 178<br />

iv


Analyses 182<br />

Procedures 189<br />

2. Reliability <strong>Study</strong> 193<br />

3. <strong>Study</strong> 1 206<br />

Chapter Five: Results Section<br />

1. Results <strong>of</strong> the statistical tests<br />

Preparation <strong>of</strong> the data 217<br />

Comparison <strong>of</strong> groups<br />

221<br />

2. Interpretation <strong>of</strong> the figures<br />

Specific differences from the ANOVA 223<br />

Discriminant analysis 236<br />

3. Subordinate analyses 241<br />

Chapter Six: Discussion 245<br />

1. Expectations and anomalies<br />

Colour 250<br />

Structure 262<br />

Conclusion and summary from discussion <strong>of</strong> the results 273<br />

2. Relation <strong>of</strong> results from <strong>Study</strong> 1 to <strong>Study</strong> 2 278<br />

3. Discriminatory power between controls and patients 284<br />

(i) Is the DAPA a better assessment than the other tests<br />

reviewed in Chapter 2? 284<br />

(ii) Which variables discriminate between patients and<br />

controls? 286<br />

Strengths and weaknesses <strong>of</strong> the study 288<br />

Effectiveness <strong>of</strong> the DAPA 291<br />

Chapter Seven: Conclusion 293<br />

Is the DAPA better than other <strong>art</strong> assessments? 299<br />

Where do we go from here? 300<br />

<strong>The</strong> future for measures <strong>of</strong> Psychopathology 302<br />

Appendices<br />

Descriptions <strong>of</strong> terms from analyses <strong>of</strong> the literature Appendix<br />

1<br />

Tables <strong>of</strong> data relating to the Chapters 2 and 5 Appendix 2<br />

Table <strong>of</strong> authorities Appendix 3<br />

Development <strong>of</strong> the instrument (the DAPA) Appendix 4<br />

Permission and informed consent for experiments with patients Appendix 5<br />

Bibliography<br />

v


List <strong>of</strong> Illustrations, Figures and Tables<br />

Chapter 2.<br />

Impressionistic studies<br />

Table 1: Analysis <strong>of</strong> 253 impressionistic and theoretical papers by diagnoses<br />

according to the theory <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> value to therapy. 37<br />

Table 2a: Environmental description for 253 impressionistic and theoretical papers<br />

according to explanation <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapeutic value. 39<br />

Table 2b: Commonly claimed benefits for 253 impressionistic and theoretical papers<br />

according to explanation <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapeutic value. 40<br />

Table 3: Summaries <strong>of</strong> impressionistic studies <strong>of</strong> or about <strong>art</strong>ists. Appendix 2<br />

Case studies<br />

Table 1: Diagnostic groups for case studies by levels <strong>of</strong> benefit where the primary<br />

purpose <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>making was the expression <strong>of</strong> feelings.<br />

51<br />

Table 2: Descriptions <strong>of</strong> 67 case studies showing consistency <strong>of</strong> the research by<br />

levels <strong>of</strong> benefit from expression <strong>of</strong> feelings. 52<br />

Table 3: Chi square results for association between method <strong>of</strong> study, form <strong>of</strong><br />

expression and benefits for 67 studies which claimed 'expression <strong>of</strong><br />

feelings' as the main benefit for the use <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> with psychiatric patients.<br />

53<br />

Table 4: Summaries <strong>of</strong> papers reporting changes or signs in the theme/content <strong>of</strong><br />

the <strong>art</strong>work. Appendix 2<br />

Table 5: Summaries <strong>of</strong> papers reporting general signs <strong>of</strong> psychiatric disturbance.<br />

Appendix 2<br />

Table 6: Summaries <strong>of</strong> papers reporting changes or signs in the form <strong>of</strong> the<br />

<strong>art</strong>work. Appendix 2<br />

Table 7: Summaries <strong>of</strong> papers reporting the most important feature <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy<br />

was the therapeutic relationship. Appendix 2<br />

Table 8: Summaries <strong>of</strong> papers reporting behaviour changes for case studies.<br />

Appendix 2<br />

Contingency tables for Chi square calculation to indicate association between method <strong>of</strong><br />

study, form <strong>of</strong> expression and benefits for categorised commonalities <strong>of</strong> the 67<br />

'expression <strong>of</strong> feelings as main benefit for <strong>art</strong> therapy' studies:<br />

crosstabulation 1: Orientation by method <strong>of</strong> study. Appendix 2<br />

crosstabulation 2: Orientation by form <strong>of</strong> expression. Appendix 2<br />

crosstabulation 3: Orientation by benefit. Appendix 2<br />

crosstabulation 4: Method <strong>of</strong> study by benefit. Appendix 2<br />

crosstabulation 5: Form <strong>of</strong> expression by method <strong>of</strong> study. Appendix 2<br />

crosstabulation 6: Form <strong>of</strong> expression by benefit. Appendix 2<br />

Controlled Studies<br />

vi


Table 1: Descriptive statistics for 51 controlled studies. 73<br />

Table 2: Frequency and percentage <strong>of</strong> demographic variables for 51<br />

controlled studies by levels <strong>of</strong> result. 73<br />

Table 3a: Diagnostic groups 1-4. Frequency and percentage for<br />

whole study. 75<br />

Table 3b: Diagnostic group 1. Frequency and percentage by levels <strong>of</strong> result. 75<br />

Table 4: t-tests performed to compare the means <strong>of</strong> ordinal demographic<br />

variables by year <strong>of</strong> study. 76<br />

Table 5: Non-parametric tests performed to compare the ranks <strong>of</strong> categorical study<br />

variables by year <strong>of</strong> study. 76<br />

Table 6: Significant variables identified from the Mann-Whitney non-parametric<br />

association test. Frequency and percentage for Group 1, early studies<br />

1973-1977; Group 2, late studies 1992-1996. Changes in methods and<br />

measurements. 78<br />

Table 7: Frequency and percentage <strong>of</strong> test variables for whole sample <strong>of</strong><br />

controlled studies by results. 79<br />

Table 8a: Analysis <strong>of</strong> variance compared 5 demographic variables<br />

by 7 study factors. 83<br />

Table 8b: Analysis <strong>of</strong> variance performed to compare the means <strong>of</strong> 5<br />

ordinal demographic variables by 7 study factors. 84<br />

Table 9: Frequency and percentage for measurement form by no. <strong>of</strong><br />

criterion measures. 85<br />

Table 10: Frequency and percentage for test derivation and results by<br />

no. <strong>of</strong> judges. 86<br />

Validity Analysis<br />

Table 1: Frequency and percentage <strong>of</strong> criterion variables for 70 studies<br />

by result. 91<br />

Table 2: Diagnostic groups for 70 studies with criterion measures. Frequency<br />

and percentage by levels <strong>of</strong> result. 92<br />

Table 3: Non parametric test to show differences between study factors for<br />

findings <strong>of</strong> association and non-association <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>-test with criterion<br />

measure. Mann-Whitney U-Wilcoxian Rank Sum Test.<br />

92<br />

Reliability Analysis<br />

Table 1: Reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong><br />

theme. 105<br />

Table 2: Reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong><br />

content. 107<br />

Table 3: Reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong> body<br />

detail in picture. 110<br />

Table 4: Reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong><br />

vii


quality. 113<br />

Table 5: Reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong><br />

line quality. 115<br />

Table 6: Reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong><br />

shape. 118<br />

Table 7: Reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong><br />

colour. 120<br />

Table 8: Reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong><br />

reality 123<br />

Table 9: Reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong><br />

space. 126<br />

Table 10: Reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong> energy.<br />

128<br />

Table 11: Reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong> pictorial<br />

detail. 130<br />

Table 12: Reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong><br />

complexity. 132<br />

Table 13: Reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong><br />

control.<br />

133<br />

Table 14: Reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong><br />

composition. 134<br />

Chapter 3<br />

Meta Analysis<br />

Table 1: Results <strong>of</strong> the aggregated combination <strong>of</strong> all variables from tabulated areas<br />

for patients and any type <strong>of</strong> control 155<br />

Table 2: All variables for 11 studies from the tabulated 15 different areas <strong>of</strong><br />

drawing measurement. 155<br />

Table 3: Aggregated results for patients/all controls by form or content<br />

variables. 157<br />

Chapter 4<br />

Development <strong>of</strong> the DAPA<br />

Table 1: <strong>The</strong> simplified positive findings from case and control studies reviewed<br />

relating to content <strong>of</strong> picture. 164<br />

Table 2: Table <strong>of</strong> expected characteristics from the literature. 169<br />

Methods<br />

Table 1: Demographics for all experimental groups. 175<br />

Table 2: Category definition <strong>of</strong> case assignment to 4 types: Affective disorder,<br />

Schizophrenia, Personality Disorder, Substance Abuse. 190<br />

Table 3: 21 patients who were excluded from the study. 190<br />

viii


Reliability <strong>Study</strong><br />

Tables showing scores given by 6 raters and author for 7 pictures, no. <strong>of</strong> exact<br />

agreements, mean rating for 6 raters and difference to score 7.<br />

Figures showing plot <strong>of</strong> differences in interrater score with author score for Mean<br />

Raters+Author plotted against average, Rater score+author score/2<br />

Table/figure 1: Red Appendix 2<br />

Table/figure 2: Yellow Appendix 2<br />

Table/figure 3: Orange Appendix 2<br />

Table/figure 4: Purple Appendix 2<br />

Table/figure 5: Green Appendix 2<br />

Table/figure 6: Blue Appendix 2<br />

Table/figure 7: Brown Appendix 2<br />

Table/figure 8: White Appendix 2<br />

Table/figure 9: Black Appendix 2<br />

Table/figure 10: Intensity Appendix 2<br />

Table/figure 11: Painted Line Appendix 2<br />

Table/figure 12: Drawn Line Appendix 2<br />

Table/figure 13: Space Appendix 2<br />

Table/figure 14: Emotional Tone Appendix 2<br />

Table/figure 15: Form Appendix 2<br />

Table 16: Standard deviation <strong>of</strong> the differences between the mean <strong>of</strong> the 6 trainee<br />

raters and the author ratings over 7 paintings. 198<br />

Table 17: Means and standard deviations <strong>of</strong> rater scores for each variable over the<br />

sample <strong>of</strong> 7 pictures. 202<br />

Table 18: Ordering <strong>of</strong> DAPA variables for reliability. 201<br />

Table 19: Inter-rater reliability showing Cronbach Alpha score for internal<br />

consistency <strong>of</strong> scores between raters; and correlation coefficient for linear<br />

association between scores <strong>of</strong> raters. 202<br />

<strong>Study</strong> 1<br />

Table 1: Demographics for experimental groups. 207<br />

Table 2: Diagnosis: Assignment to type. 211<br />

Table 3: Multiple ranges: group means significantly different at the 5% level.<br />

Duncan procedure. 214<br />

Chapter 5<br />

Results<br />

Boxplots, spreadplots and tables showing distribution for each diagnostic group against<br />

whole population for variables from the DAPA test with heterogeneous distribution<br />

before transformation.<br />

Table, boxplot and spreadplot 1a (red). Appendix 2<br />

Table, boxplot and spreadplot 1b (yellow). Appendix 2<br />

Table, boxplot and spreadplot 1c (orange). Appendix 2<br />

Table, boxplot and spreadplot 1d (purple). Appendix 2<br />

ix


Table, boxplot and spreadplot 1e (brown). Appendix 2<br />

Table, boxplot and spreadplot 1f (white). Appendix 2<br />

Table, boxplot and spreadplot 1g (painted line). Appendix 2<br />

Table, boxplot and spreadplot 1h (drawn line). Appendix 2<br />

Table, boxplot and spreadplot 1i (form). Appendix 2<br />

Boxplots, spreadplots and tables for variables which were kept in their original state<br />

because they showed homogeneity in their distribution from the Levene test, or the<br />

transformation did not improve their distribution pattern.<br />

Table, boxplot and spreadplot 2a (green). Appendix 2<br />

Table, boxplot and spreadplot 2b (blue). Appendix 2<br />

Table, boxplot and spreadplot 2c (black). Appendix 2<br />

Table, boxplot and spreadplot 2d (intensity).<br />

Appendix 2<br />

Table, boxplot and spreadplot 2e (space). Appendix 2<br />

Table, boxplot and spreadplot 2f (emotional tone). Appendix 2<br />

Mean scores for variables from the DAPA test showing distribution norms <strong>of</strong> over 109<br />

subjects after transformation.<br />

Table, boxplot and spreadplot 3a (red). Appendix 2<br />

Table, boxplot and spreadplot 3b (yellow). Appendix 2<br />

Table, boxplot and spreadplot 3c (brown). Appendix 2<br />

Table, boxplot and spreadplot 3d (painted line). Appendix 2<br />

Table, boxplot and spreadplot 3e (drawn line). Appendix 2<br />

Table, boxplot and spreadplot 3f (form). Appendix 2<br />

Table 4: Projected transformations for data based on computations designed to<br />

verify the assumptions <strong>of</strong> the ANOVA test. 218<br />

Table 5: Transformed data based on computations designed to verify the ANOVA<br />

test. 218<br />

Figures 6, standard differences from mean <strong>of</strong> each diagnostic group.<br />

6a, plot <strong>of</strong> red Appendix 2<br />

6b, plot <strong>of</strong> yellow Appendix 2<br />

6c, green Appendix 2<br />

6d, blue Appendix 2<br />

6e, brown Appendix 2<br />

6f, black Appendix 2<br />

6g, intensity Appendix 2<br />

6h, painted line Appendix 2<br />

6i, drawn line Appendix 2<br />

6j, space Appendix 2<br />

6k, emotional tone Appendix 2<br />

6l, dominant form Appendix 2<br />

Figures 7, standard differences from mean <strong>of</strong> diagnostic groups: abnormal distribution.<br />

x


7a, orange Appendix 2<br />

7b, purple Appendix 2<br />

7c, white Appendix 2<br />

Table 8: Results <strong>of</strong> analysis <strong>of</strong> variance for the purpose <strong>of</strong> identifying diagnostic<br />

grouping variables between 4 groups <strong>of</strong> psychiatric patients and one<br />

control group on 13 formal measures <strong>of</strong> their paintings. 222<br />

Table 9: Main Analysis: multiple comparisons, Duncan procedure. 222<br />

Specific Differences for each variable<br />

Figures showing confidence intervals and means by diagnostic type for DAPA variables:<br />

Figure 10a, red 224<br />

Figure 10b, yellow 225<br />

Figure 10c, green 226<br />

Figure 10d, blue 226<br />

Figure 10e, brown 227<br />

Figure 10f, black 228<br />

Figure 10g, intensity 229<br />

Figure 10h, painted line 230<br />

Figure 10i, drawn line 231<br />

Figure 10j, space 232<br />

Figure 10k, subjective emotional tone 234<br />

Figure 10l, dominant form 235<br />

Table 11: Discriminant analysis to classify controls or patients. Classification<br />

functions from the first sample are used to classify the second sample.<br />

Repeated 5 times with different p<strong>art</strong>itions <strong>of</strong> the data set. 238<br />

Table 12: t-test results for significant differences between means <strong>of</strong> variables<br />

measured from paintings by patients against non-patients as though they<br />

were independent. 239<br />

Table 13a: Separate effect size for each variable from the DAPA test. 240<br />

Table 13b: Separate effect size for each variable from the DAPA test. 240<br />

Table 14: Results <strong>of</strong> 2-way ANOVA: diagnostic groups by number <strong>of</strong> paintings<br />

from each patient.<br />

242<br />

Table 15: Correlations between number <strong>of</strong> pictures within each group with each<br />

variable. 243<br />

Table 16: Correlation for DAPA variables measured from the paintings <strong>of</strong><br />

schizophrenics - a) Colours b) structure. Appendix 2<br />

Table 17: Correlation for DAPA variables measured from the paintings <strong>of</strong> substance<br />

abusers - a) Colours b) structure. Appendix 2<br />

Table 18: Correlation for DAPA variables measured from the paintings <strong>of</strong><br />

depressives - a) Colours b) structure. Appendix 2<br />

xi


Table 19: Correlation for DAPA variables measured from the paintings <strong>of</strong> controls<br />

- a) Colours b) structure. Appendix 2<br />

Table 20: Correlation for DAPA variables measured from the paintings <strong>of</strong><br />

personality disorder - a) Colours b) structure.<br />

Appendix 2<br />

Chapter 6<br />

Discussion<br />

Table 1: Summary table <strong>of</strong> results. 249<br />

Table 2: Interpretations <strong>of</strong> the use <strong>of</strong> colour in <strong>art</strong>istic production, taken from<br />

S.P.Amos. 251<br />

Figures 3a-e: Map <strong>of</strong> associations between colour variables measured between<br />

diagnostic groups from the results <strong>of</strong> the DAPA test.<br />

252<br />

Figures 4a-e: Map <strong>of</strong> association <strong>of</strong> structural variables measured between diagnostic<br />

groups using the DAPA test. 262<br />

Illustration: example <strong>of</strong> use <strong>of</strong> grid system with marked score for 'red'. 171<br />

xii


List <strong>of</strong> Abbreviations<br />

Am. Assn. Art <strong>The</strong>rapists American Association <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapists (AAAT)<br />

Am. American<br />

Am. Educational Research J. American Educational Research Journal<br />

Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy<br />

Am. J. Psychotherapy American Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychotherapy<br />

Art Psychotherapy <strong>The</strong> Arts in Psychotherapy<br />

B. J. Psychiatry British Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry<br />

Bull. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Bulletin <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapy<br />

British Assn. Art <strong>The</strong>rapists (BAAT) British Association <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapists<br />

Canadian Psychol. Canadian Psychologist<br />

CDAT Comparitive Description <strong>of</strong> Artwork Test<br />

DAPA Descriptive Assessment for Psychiatric Artwork<br />

DAP Draw-A-Person (used in drawing tests)<br />

DDS Diagnostic Drawing Series<br />

HFD Human Figure Drawing<br />

HTP House-Tree-Person (used in drawing tests)<br />

Int. International<br />

J. Journal<br />

J. <strong>of</strong> the Am. Academy <strong>of</strong> Psychoanalysis Journal <strong>of</strong> the American Academy <strong>of</strong><br />

Psychoanalysis<br />

J. Am. Psychoanalytic Assessment Journal <strong>of</strong> American Psychoanalytic Assessment<br />

J. Clin. Psychol. Journal <strong>of</strong> Clinical Psychology.<br />

J. Exp.l Psychol. Journal <strong>of</strong> Experimental Psychology<br />

J. Gen. Psychol. Journal <strong>of</strong> General Psychology.<br />

J. Nervous Mental Dis. Journal <strong>of</strong> Nervous and Mental Disease<br />

J. Pers. Assess. Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality Assessment<br />

J. Sch. Psychol. Journal <strong>of</strong> School Psychology<br />

Percep. Motor Skills Perceptual and Motor Skills<br />

Psycholog. Bull. Psyhological Bulletin<br />

Psychol. Mon. Psychological Monographs.<br />

WHO <strong>World</strong> Health Organisation<br />

xiii


Acknowledgements, thanks and foreword<br />

This thesis comes as a culmination <strong>of</strong> questions that have arisen, mostly unformulated,<br />

over many years <strong>of</strong> involvement in <strong>art</strong>, which st<strong>art</strong>ed casually through community<br />

projects, continued during a degree in Fine Art, and subsequent pr<strong>of</strong>essional involvement<br />

with mental health groups. During the last decade, I touched on the same sort <strong>of</strong> question<br />

under the auspices <strong>of</strong> academic psychology during my MA., but finally the opportunity<br />

to research through psychiatry has led me to approach the subject from a far different<br />

angle than I had originally envisiaged. I have not become a multidisciplinary expert, I<br />

have tried to understand what I have applied, but no doubt shortcomings can be found<br />

in it. <strong>The</strong> course <strong>of</strong> this research, however, has changed me. It has given me new and<br />

useful skills, and thought structures, but it has also taken away some aspects I was not<br />

fully aware <strong>of</strong>; I am still synthesising the widely different contexts it covered.<br />

I am aware this thesis may read as an attack on the pr<strong>of</strong>ession <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapists,<br />

although this is far from the intention. It is a sad picture <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy research that<br />

emerges, and I have related its failings rather than the clinical achievements <strong>of</strong> this<br />

dedicated pr<strong>of</strong>ession for which I have the greatest respect and whose research work is<br />

<strong>of</strong>ten constrained by lack <strong>of</strong> funding and low status.<br />

This research was made possible by the interest and generous help and<br />

encouragement I have received from some who gave their valuable time and went out <strong>of</strong><br />

their way to assist this novice. I should like to record outstanding debts <strong>of</strong> gratitude to<br />

Dr. David M<strong>art</strong>in Foreman, Senior Lecturer, Psychiatry Dept. School <strong>of</strong> Postgraduate<br />

Medicine at the University <strong>of</strong> Keele, who supervised this research through the depths <strong>of</strong><br />

depression to its highlights and whose unfailing encouragement, useful comments,<br />

criticism and suggestions most motivated me over the six years it took. I would like to<br />

specially thank also Mike Fletcher, Head <strong>of</strong> Dept., School <strong>of</strong> Computing, Staffordshire<br />

University, who provided specialist knowledge and supervised the application <strong>of</strong> the<br />

discriminant analysis technique and who kindly checked over the first draft <strong>of</strong> the results<br />

section.<br />

I thank these Consultant psychiatrists from N. Staffs. Hospitals, who allowed me<br />

access to their patients: Dr. K. Bloore, Dr. K. Barrett, Dr. F. MacMillan, Dr. J. Crisp,<br />

Dr. Ward, D. Gee, Dr. Slade, Dr. J. Boardman, Dr. Myers and special thanks to Dr. K.<br />

Barrett, Clinical Director, Bucknall Hospital and Senior Lecturer in Psychiatry, Post<br />

Graduate School <strong>of</strong> Medicine, Keele University whose interest, encouragement and<br />

supervision <strong>of</strong> the initial stages for the first DAPA study was instrumental in<br />

development <strong>of</strong> the test theory.<br />

John Belcher <strong>of</strong> the Dept. <strong>of</strong> Mathematics, Keele University did his best to<br />

instruct me in basic statistics in the early stages <strong>of</strong> this research; Pr<strong>of</strong>essor Terry Shave,<br />

<strong>of</strong> the Dept. <strong>of</strong> Fine Art at Staffordshire University allowed me to recruit his students<br />

as test-raters; Dr. Linda Gantt, <strong>of</strong> Alexandria sent me her Ph.D thesis from the USA and<br />

provided some useful contacts.<br />

Finally, without the unceasing support and encouragement <strong>of</strong> my p<strong>art</strong>ner David,<br />

I should never have been able to finish what has seemed at times an impossible task,<br />

14


especially since the birth <strong>of</strong> our daughter Ellie 3 years ago. His support, love and<br />

understanding is my greatest resource.<br />

Chapter One: Introduction<br />

Organisation <strong>of</strong> thesis: <strong>The</strong> first three chapters introduce the background to the study,<br />

the need for systematic assessment <strong>of</strong> painted <strong>art</strong>work and the development <strong>of</strong> a novel<br />

approach to the assessment <strong>of</strong> drawing categories. Chapter one provides an overview,<br />

points out that conventional interpretive evaluative methods do not identify what is<br />

specifically psychiatric about <strong>art</strong>work produced by patients and reveals the neglect <strong>of</strong><br />

formal measures. Chapter two reviews the development <strong>of</strong> assessment <strong>of</strong> psychiatric<br />

<strong>art</strong>work over the past 20 years in two sections. Section 1 introduces a number <strong>of</strong> popular<br />

views about how <strong>art</strong> therapy works and Section 2 concentrates on the information from<br />

case studies and controlled studies; common findings, conclusions and changes in<br />

assessment methods. <strong>The</strong>se results are then critically discussed with p<strong>art</strong>icular reference<br />

to methodology. In chapter three a meta-analysis is performed on studies from the<br />

review to identify which groups <strong>of</strong> variables are central to reliable and systematic<br />

assessments <strong>of</strong> psychiatric paintings. <strong>The</strong> summary gathers the knowledge from the<br />

literature review to show the kinds <strong>of</strong> measurement which have the best repeatability and<br />

validity for new instruments.<br />

Chapter 4 describes the development <strong>of</strong> a novel instrument, the Descriptive<br />

Assessment for Psychiatric Art (the DAPA) and introduces the steps taken to validate<br />

it: the main experiment, inter-rater reliability and the initial study. Chapters 5 presents<br />

the results <strong>of</strong> the main study. Chapter 6 considers the support for and discrepancies<br />

from the DAPA results with previous work. Finally, Chapter seven summarises and<br />

15


concludes on the effectiveness <strong>of</strong> the instrument with some thoughts on how the<br />

approach exemplified by the DAPA extends previous practice in this field.<br />

Introduction<br />

<strong>The</strong> Argument for Another Art Assessment<br />

It has long been remarked upon that people with mental illness can sometimes produce<br />

quite surprisingly communicative and highly organised <strong>art</strong>, the explanation <strong>of</strong> which has<br />

had a number <strong>of</strong> theoretical approaches 1 . <strong>The</strong> general task has been to describe and<br />

explain the phenomena <strong>of</strong> psychotic <strong>art</strong> in relation to the behaviour and experience <strong>of</strong><br />

mental illness. This issue has been discussed within many disciplines, without producing<br />

final solutions, but the psychological aspects have been, to some extent detached and<br />

investigated on an empirical level. <strong>The</strong> subject is too complex to be handled as a whole<br />

and it has been divided into many different problems for research. MacGregor (1989) and<br />

Waller (1991) have made recent extensive reviews <strong>of</strong> the history <strong>of</strong> ideas in the field <strong>of</strong><br />

psychiatric <strong>art</strong> 2 . <strong>The</strong>se two sources alone provide a comprehensive historical<br />

background, but Winner 3 adds a useful analytical framework and the diversity <strong>of</strong><br />

1<br />

For summaries see Tessa Dalley and Caroline Case (1992), Handbook <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , London:<br />

Routledge, probably gives the most readable account <strong>of</strong> the diversity <strong>of</strong> theoretical approaches; T. Dalley<br />

and A. Gilroy, eds. (1989), Pictures at an Exhibition , London: Routledge, explores the range <strong>of</strong><br />

psychoanalytic explanations thought to be related to <strong>psychopathology</strong> in <strong>art</strong>; Marion Liebmann (1990),<br />

Art in Practice , London: Tavistock, describes the variety <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy practised with different client<br />

groups in one area; and V. Lusebrink (1990), Imagery and Visual Expression in <strong>The</strong>rapy , New York:<br />

Plenum, identifies most <strong>of</strong> the different practical approaches to <strong>art</strong> therapy.<br />

2<br />

Diane Waller's (1991) Ph.D. based Becoming a Pr<strong>of</strong>ession: the history <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy 1940-1982 , London:<br />

Routledge, gives a useful and informative in-depth study <strong>of</strong> the roots <strong>of</strong> current theoretical practice. John<br />

Monroe MacGregor (1989), <strong>The</strong> Discovery <strong>of</strong> the Art <strong>of</strong> the Insane , New Jersey and Oxford: Princetown<br />

University Press, also developed from his Ph.D. research, provides a detailed interdisciplinary history <strong>of</strong><br />

the earlier empirical and case studies, personalities and work <strong>of</strong> insane <strong>art</strong>ists.<br />

3<br />

Ellen Winner (1982) Invented Worl ds: the psychology <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong>s , Cambridge, Mass: Harvard U.P.<br />

16


contributors to the exploration <strong>of</strong> emotion through <strong>art</strong> have been comprehensively<br />

discussed elsewhere 4 . <strong>The</strong> foci <strong>of</strong> investigation fall between two domains, closely<br />

interrelated: those describing the process <strong>of</strong> creation and therapeutic benefit and those<br />

describing the product.<br />

This chapter introduces a number <strong>of</strong> popular views about the explanation and<br />

investigation <strong>of</strong> pictures produced by psychiatric patients. When we talk about <strong>art</strong><br />

therapy, we refer to a belief that <strong>art</strong> is somehow expressive in itself or even healing 5 . Art<br />

therapists are not united in their subscription to any explanatory theory but most agree<br />

on these principles: disturbed people, p<strong>art</strong>icularly schizophrenics, produce <strong>art</strong>work<br />

which is communicative in a way that their language is not; the thoughts or feelings <strong>of</strong> the<br />

patient are literally re-presented in concrete form; connections are made cognitively or<br />

visually which approximate insight experiences; the paintings are described as powerful<br />

and disturbing, <strong>of</strong>ten filled with bizarre imagery. <strong>The</strong>se features have been related to the<br />

<strong>art</strong>ist's mental or emotional state. <strong>The</strong> commonly understood advantage in using <strong>art</strong> with<br />

people who may not be able to fully express themselves in language is that as the painting<br />

4<br />

see, for example: Tessa Dalley (1984), Art as <strong>The</strong>rapy , London: Tavistock; T. Dalley et al. (1987), eds.,<br />

Images <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , London: Tavistock, giving a range <strong>of</strong> perspectives on the therapeutic aspects <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>art</strong>; Joy Schaverien (1992) gives a very readable psychoanalytic breakdown <strong>of</strong> the relationship <strong>of</strong> affect and<br />

transference and the development <strong>of</strong> the therapeutic relationship, <strong>The</strong> Revealing Image: analytical <strong>art</strong><br />

psychotherapy in theory and pra ctice , London: Routledge.<br />

5<br />

Joan Woddis (1992) comments on the belief <strong>of</strong> many <strong>art</strong> therapists in a "mythology concerning the<br />

intrinsic healing properties <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong>-making process" (Art <strong>The</strong>rapy: new problems, new solutions in Diane<br />

Waller and Andrea Gilroy, eds., Art <strong>The</strong>rapy: A Handbook , Bristol: O.U.P. p.39). For example, to use<br />

a standard text on <strong>art</strong> therapy, Dalley and Case (1992, Handbook, op.cit. p.54) acknowledge the split<br />

between <strong>art</strong> therapists on this issue and also on the relevance <strong>of</strong> psychoanalytic theory. Whilst Dalley<br />

generally emphasises the vital importance <strong>of</strong> the therapist in access to the image, elements <strong>of</strong> the healing<br />

function <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> do enter pervade into her writing: She explains her personal view <strong>of</strong> the function <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong><br />

therapist as "providing a setting in which healing can occur". Another 'setting' argument: "<strong>The</strong> activity<br />

<strong>of</strong> painting sets up a relationship between client and the paper, which can be exclusive <strong>of</strong> the therapist, but<br />

the therapist... holds the safety <strong>of</strong> the scene, like the mother ever attentive to her infant" (p.59). Recent<br />

papers which <strong>of</strong>fer this view as an explanation for the therapeutic process are reviewed in Chapter 2.<br />

17


or drawing progresses, relationships and reinforcement develop between the p<strong>art</strong>s <strong>of</strong> the<br />

composition, which can be monitored by the therapist, or manipulated to produce<br />

specific reactions, as with a conversation.<br />

This overview does not provide a historical survey, which would both step<br />

outside the conceptual boundary <strong>of</strong> this thesis and require more space. <strong>The</strong> background<br />

is only discussed as far as it affects this thesis. It introduces two kinds <strong>of</strong> assessment<br />

which informed the study <strong>of</strong> psychiatric <strong>art</strong>: case studies, which are split into four areas;<br />

and projective testing. Although limited, this background is important here because many<br />

<strong>of</strong> the assumptions underlying the theoretical base used to analyse content, especially in<br />

impressionistic studies, hark back to speculations that were published at the turn <strong>of</strong> the<br />

century but have been subject to serious criticism ever since 6 ; for example, the confusion<br />

<strong>of</strong> relations between insanity and <strong>art</strong>istic creativity. This section also introduces a few<br />

<strong>of</strong> the terms and jargon commonly used in studies <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> but not all <strong>of</strong> them since many<br />

are based on theoretical assumptions which do not concern this study 7 .<br />

6<br />

But not from writers on psychopathological <strong>art</strong>. D. Waller (1991) op.cit. gives an account <strong>of</strong> the<br />

development <strong>of</strong> current theory but there is little personal comment. Most writers on <strong>art</strong> therapy rather than<br />

tackle arguments try to embrace a range <strong>of</strong> apparent contradictions by modifying or integrating theory;<br />

Waller explains "<strong>art</strong> therapy is a term which has been used to describe a collection <strong>of</strong> diverse practices, held<br />

together fundamentally by their practitioners' belief in the healing value <strong>of</strong> image-making", Waller and<br />

Gilroy (1992) introduction, op.cit. Dalley and Case (1992), Handbook, op.cit. write that <strong>art</strong> activity is<br />

both a conscious process (p.51, 98) and an unconscious process (p.64), point out that images are complex<br />

and take time to understand both for the therapist and the <strong>art</strong>ist and need discussion (p.52, 64) and Dalley<br />

goes on to interpret a series <strong>of</strong> paintings left by a client who never spoke to her (p.118). Notable<br />

exceptions are David Maclagan (1989) who includes acerbic comments in his <strong>art</strong>icles concerned with the<br />

relation <strong>of</strong> verbal psychotherapy to the image produced, see for example, <strong>The</strong> Aesthetic Dimension <strong>of</strong> Art<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy: luxury or necessity, Inscape , Spring: 10-13; John Birchtnell (1981) Is Art <strong>The</strong>rapeutic?, Inscape ,<br />

V(I).p.10 and J. Champernowe (1971), Art and <strong>The</strong>rapy: an uneasy p<strong>art</strong>nership? Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy ,<br />

April, X(3):131-143 which gives the bones <strong>of</strong> the arguments.<br />

7 Dalley and Case (1992) op.cit., p.60-3 gives a useful glossary and explanation <strong>of</strong> the main analytic terms<br />

and interpretations, but there is no generally agreed standardisation.<br />

18


Another reason that little time is spent here is because before the 1950s concept<br />

change in psychiatry, there was a totally different approach to diagnosis so we can't<br />

really be sure that previous descriptions are comparable with today's psychiatric<br />

categorisations. Institutionalisation may have accounted for much <strong>of</strong> the previous<br />

findings <strong>of</strong> global differences between patients and controls. During the late 1960s,<br />

psychiatric diagnostic criteria was standardised between the U.S. and the U.K., especially<br />

in operational definitions <strong>of</strong> specific categories.<br />

<strong>The</strong> need for more research in <strong>art</strong> assessment<br />

<strong>The</strong> two most commonly recognised uses <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> with psychiatric patients other than as<br />

a diversionary activity, are in therapy for expression <strong>of</strong> emotional issues and for<br />

psychological assessment, as an aid to diagnosis or treatment. <strong>The</strong>re is pressure on <strong>art</strong>-<br />

therapy to control and assess its therapeutic input for the latter two categories if <strong>art</strong><br />

activity is not to be relegated to the first. <strong>The</strong> descriptive literature points to obvious and<br />

felt benefits from clients' experience <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy, but there is a marked paucity <strong>of</strong><br />

criticism <strong>of</strong> the theoretical bases <strong>of</strong> the practices and a lack <strong>of</strong> investigative research into<br />

the contribution <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong>work to <strong>psychopathology</strong> 8 .<br />

Investigation into the <strong>psychopathology</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>: the background<br />

8<br />

Recognised by the majority <strong>of</strong> contemporary writers in this field, for example: David Edwards (1987),<br />

Evaluation in Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, in Derek Milne, ed., Evaluation in Mental Health Practice , Beckingham:<br />

Croom Helm, pp.53-69; Andrea Gilroy (1992) Research in Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, in Waller and Gilroy, Handbook,<br />

op.cit. pp.229-247; Shaun McNiff (1986), Freedom <strong>of</strong> Research and Artistic Inquiry, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , V.13: 279-84; comments from Linda Gantt and Gladys Agell (1994) in, R. Goodman,<br />

G. Agell, L. Gantt and K. Williams, 'Are there Doctors in the House? Does Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Need a Cure?'<br />

Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.33, p.3-13.<br />

19


<strong>The</strong> link between genius as a product <strong>of</strong> madness and creativity, which was first<br />

proposed by Lombroso, Italian psychiatrist and criminologist, in 1891 9 , has survived in<br />

popular culture and to some extent in learned circles, as have two <strong>art</strong>icles written by Paul-<br />

Max Simon (1876 and 1888) 10 . Lombroso and Simon separately describe the use <strong>of</strong><br />

characteristic features as a means <strong>of</strong> psychiatric diagnosis. Simon's conclusions, from<br />

clinical observation, were based on only 14 creative individuals and Lombroso's <strong>of</strong>fer only<br />

his own speculations. Nevertheless they were a great influence on the psychiatric<br />

approach to <strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the late 19th century. MacGregor describes the refinement <strong>of</strong> Simon's<br />

work in the more systematic experimental approach <strong>of</strong> Fritz Mohr (1874-1966), derived<br />

from the school <strong>of</strong> Kraepelin (1856-1926) 11 . Mohr's methods were adopted by students<br />

<strong>of</strong> the subject 12 and led to the development <strong>of</strong> standardised tests on the one hand, and on<br />

the other hand, the descriptive case study. Both methods subsequently developed<br />

separately, each strand evolving its own literature, techniques and theory. Prinzhorn<br />

(1886-1933), a psychiatrist as well as an <strong>art</strong> historian, studied the Heidelberg collection<br />

<strong>of</strong> psychiatric <strong>art</strong>. His book, Artistry <strong>of</strong> the Mentally Ill (1922) 13 presents and does not<br />

interpret the work <strong>of</strong> p<strong>art</strong>icularly talented but selected individuals, commenting on the<br />

9<br />

Cesare Lombroso (1891), Man <strong>of</strong> Genius , London: Scott.<br />

10 see MacGregor (1989) op.cit., Chapter 7, pp.103-115 for a description.<br />

11<br />

Emil Kraepelin discussed the drawings <strong>of</strong> dementia praecox patients (his own category for schizophrenia)<br />

in his Lehrbuch der Psychiatrie (1883) distinguishing several subtypes, although his involvement with<br />

patient <strong>art</strong> was rather superficial (MacGregor (1989, op.cit.):188-9).<br />

12 Although he never got further than descriptive studies himself. Most <strong>of</strong> his followers contributed little<br />

to his original findings with a few notable exceptions (e.g. Herman Rorschach, 1884-1922 and Karl<br />

Jaspers, 1883-1969).<br />

13<br />

Hans Prinzhorn (1922), Artistry <strong>of</strong> the Mentally Ill , Berlin: Springer Verlag.<br />

20


style and communication value. Prinzhorn ridiculed the diagnostic use <strong>of</strong> drawings:<br />

"Anyone unable to make a diagnosis without the drawings will certainly not have an<br />

easier time with them" (p.3). This work is still held as the most complete treatment <strong>of</strong><br />

the field at the time, but there is no doubt that some <strong>of</strong> his 'schizophrenics' would not be<br />

so diagnosed today. By the turn <strong>of</strong> the century, psychiatry 14 acknowledged the<br />

systematic drawing test as a useful aid to diagnosis. Illustrated case studies with<br />

attempts to link characteristics from drawing and painting to psychiatric symptoms were<br />

published, although with few exceptions these were poor quality: their material was<br />

selective, they confused observation and interpretation, were unreliable in their<br />

reporting 15 . By mid-century, there were hundreds <strong>of</strong> descriptive case studies, some book<br />

length 16 . Speculative attempts to relate psychiatric symptoms to images were mostly<br />

discredited when the confounding influences <strong>of</strong> intelligence, <strong>art</strong> experience, skill in<br />

patients were demonstrated to influence raters' judgement <strong>of</strong> normality in the 1970s 17 ,<br />

14<br />

Macgregor (1989, op.cit., pp.243-244) describes the interest <strong>of</strong> Karl Jaspers in the comparison <strong>of</strong><br />

aesthetics in paintings by <strong>art</strong>ists with a mental illness with those <strong>of</strong> psychiatric patients from the Prinzhorn<br />

collection, (K. Jaspers, Strindberg un d Van Gogh ); Henri F. Ellenberger (1970), <strong>The</strong> Discovery <strong>of</strong> the<br />

Unconscious - <strong>The</strong> History and Evolution <strong>of</strong> Dynamic Psychiatry , Harmondsworth: Penguin Press, gives<br />

a good account <strong>of</strong> the gradual medical recognition that drawings by the mentally ill functioned as<br />

externalisation <strong>of</strong> their delusional preoccupations and that specifically disturbance <strong>of</strong> speech was related<br />

to disturbance <strong>of</strong> pictorial rendering.<br />

15<br />

A. Anastasi & J. Foley (1941), A Survey <strong>of</strong> the literature on <strong>art</strong>istic behavior in the abnormal: 1.<br />

Historical & <strong>The</strong>oretical Background, J. <strong>of</strong> Gen. Psychol. , V.25:111-142; for later criticism <strong>of</strong> the<br />

substandard quality <strong>of</strong> much <strong>of</strong> the research material <strong>of</strong> the past, see: S. Russell-Lacy, V. Robinson, J.<br />

Benson, J. Cranage (1979), An Experimental <strong>Study</strong> <strong>of</strong> Pictures Produced by Acute Schizophrenic<br />

Subjects, British Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry , V.134:195-200; Harriet Wadeson (1980), Art Psychotherapy,<br />

New<br />

York: Wiley; Cathy Malchiodi (1993), Introduction to special issue on Art <strong>The</strong>rapy and Pr<strong>of</strong>essionalism,<br />

Is there a crisis in Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Education? Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.10(3):122.<br />

16<br />

Artist, A. Hill (1945), Art Versus Illness , and (1951) Painting out Illness , London: Allen Unwin.<br />

Books now considered minor classics by psychiatrists: R. Pickford (1967), Studies in Psychiatric A rt<br />

London, Tavistock; F. Reitman (1950) Psychotic Art , London: R and KP, and J. H. Plokker (1964),<br />

Artistic Self-Expression in Mental Disease , London; Littlebrown, while E. Cunningham-Dax (1953), made<br />

a more objective and controlled study, Experimental Stu dies in Psychiatric Art , London: Faber Faber.<br />

17 R. Langevin, M. Raine, D. Day and K. Waxer (1975), Art experience, intelligence and formal features<br />

21


again by a multidisciplinary team who tested different diagnostic groups against non-<br />

patients in 1979 18 and more recent studies have confirmed these findings 19 .<br />

Art <strong>The</strong>rapy and Psychoanalytic Interpretation<br />

It is not really possible to discuss this section generally, so it is necessarily limited in<br />

scope because <strong>of</strong> the enormous complexity <strong>of</strong> the understanding <strong>of</strong> psychoanalytic<br />

theory and also because <strong>of</strong> the limited application to this thesis. Nevertheless, some<br />

aspects <strong>of</strong> the contribution <strong>of</strong> psychoanalysis to <strong>art</strong> therapy research must be<br />

summarised, since the bulk <strong>of</strong> the literature reviewed in this thesis, and hence the<br />

direction and questions come from therapists, whose formal training includes some<br />

breakdown <strong>of</strong> psychoanalytic theory 20 , trying to share the sense <strong>of</strong> the imagery they face<br />

in their clinical practice.<br />

in psychotics' paintings, Arts in Psychotherapy , Fall V.2(2):149-158; Frank A. Johnson and Roger P.<br />

Greenberg (1978), Quality <strong>of</strong> Drawing as a factor in the interpretation <strong>of</strong> figure drawings, Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

Personality Assessment , V.42(5):489-495; R. Cressen (1975), Artistic quality <strong>of</strong> drawing and judges<br />

evaluations <strong>of</strong> the DAP, Journal <strong>of</strong> Personality Assessent , V.39:132-137.<br />

18 S. Russell-Lacy et al (1979, op.cit.).<br />

19<br />

M. Miljkovitch and G.M. Irvine (1982), Comparison <strong>of</strong> drawing performances <strong>of</strong> schizophrenics, other<br />

psychiatric patients, and normal schoolchildren on a draw-a-village task, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.9:203-<br />

16; G.C. Cupchik and R.J. Gebotys (1988), <strong>The</strong> search for meaning in <strong>art</strong>: interpretive styles and<br />

judgement <strong>of</strong> quality, Visual Arts Research , V.14:138-50; J. Sims, R.H. Dona and B. Bolton (1983), <strong>The</strong><br />

validity <strong>of</strong> the DAP as an anxiety measure, J. Pers. Assess ment , V.47:250-7; see review by F.F. Kaplan<br />

(1991), Drawing assessment and <strong>art</strong>istic skill, Art Psychotherapy , V.18:347-52.<br />

20<br />

<strong>The</strong> qualification for <strong>art</strong> therapists in Britain is an <strong>art</strong> degree and a postgraduate one year training course,<br />

which may not even include experience <strong>of</strong> personal therapy, in <strong>art</strong> therapy run by specialist centres, so<br />

much <strong>of</strong> their information is simplistic and self-taught; recent application <strong>of</strong> psychoanalytic theory to <strong>art</strong><br />

therapy is covered in a special issue devoted to transference and countertransference in the creative <strong>art</strong>s<br />

therapies, Arts in Psychotherapy (1992) V.19(15).<br />

22


Others have given summaries <strong>of</strong> the immense influence <strong>of</strong> Freud and Jung on <strong>art</strong><br />

evaluation 21 , both need no introduction here. Freud never used drawings in his work, but<br />

his model <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong>ist as a borderline psychiatric case became the public view <strong>of</strong> the<br />

'unconscious made visible' <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong>ist's work. Freud's deconstruction <strong>of</strong> the work <strong>of</strong><br />

Leonardo and Michelangelo 22 was probably his influential contribution to the technique<br />

<strong>of</strong> interpretation 23 . Jung painted and sculpted and encouraged his patients to do the same<br />

"in order to escape the censure <strong>of</strong> the unconscious mind" 24 . He rejected Freud's negative<br />

view <strong>of</strong> phantasy as a source <strong>of</strong> unconscious or as sublimations <strong>of</strong> infantile conflicts. He<br />

made a distinction between personal and collective unconscious, and based his<br />

interpretations and assumptions concerning archetypes 25 and universal symbols 26 on a<br />

21<br />

MacGregor (1989) op.cit., pp.245-270 gives a perspective <strong>of</strong> Freud and Jung's limited involvement in<br />

true psychotic <strong>art</strong> and the development <strong>of</strong> method and theory from Kris; Joy Schaverien (1992) op.cit.,<br />

takes p<strong>art</strong>icular aspects from psychoanalysis to develop her own methods for <strong>art</strong> therapy and evaluation,<br />

but gives a very clear explanation <strong>of</strong> transference and counter transference; Ellenberger (1970) op.cit.,<br />

shows the development from the theories <strong>of</strong> Freud to today's 'dynamic psychiatry'; Waller (1991) op.cit.,<br />

presents a perspective <strong>of</strong> the synthesis <strong>of</strong> methods in psychoanalysis for<br />

interpretation <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> in therapy in Britain from 1940, and Maxine Borrowsky Junge and Paige Pateracki<br />

Asawa (1994), A History <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapy in the United States , Mundelein: Am. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Assn., review<br />

the <strong>art</strong> therapy literature which summarises the contributions <strong>of</strong> first and second generation <strong>art</strong> therapists<br />

in America but is generally less theoretically orientated. For a more in-depth analysis <strong>of</strong> psychoanalytic<br />

methods in <strong>art</strong>, E. Kris (1964), Psychoanalytic Explorations in Art , New York: Intl. U. P., and R.<br />

Wollheim (1964), Art and its Objects Cambridge (reprint 1980): Writers Readers, probably give the most<br />

understandable explanations. Books on <strong>art</strong> therapy generally are too simplistic and use a synthesis <strong>of</strong><br />

different methods with no overall context.<br />

22<br />

Laurie Schneider Adams (1993) Art and Psychoanalysis , New York: Harper Collins gives a perspective<br />

on Sigmund Freud (1910), Leonardo Da Vinci - a memory <strong>of</strong> his childhood, Art and Literature, Pelican<br />

Freud Library , V.XIV, Harmondsworth: Penguin, mostly derived from his childhood memories and<br />

speculations on the infancy <strong>of</strong> Leonardo. It was in <strong>The</strong> Moses <strong>of</strong> Michelangelo, ibid., (1914) that he<br />

explored expression in the <strong>art</strong>work.<br />

23 But he made a number <strong>of</strong> assumptions based on faulty translation <strong>of</strong> Italian histories.<br />

24<br />

C.G. Jung and R. Wilhelm (1931), <strong>The</strong> Secret <strong>of</strong> the Golden Flower , London: Macmillan, p.94;<br />

Schaverien (1992), op.cit., gives a good description <strong>of</strong> the concept <strong>of</strong> the 'unconscious' and its application<br />

to modern day <strong>art</strong> therapy pp.22-25.<br />

25<br />

Archetypes in <strong>art</strong>; inherited unconscious images that are component p<strong>art</strong>s <strong>of</strong> the 'collective unconscious'<br />

shared by all.<br />

26 Universal symbol: a symbol that is assumed to represent the same referent universally. Such symbols<br />

23


theoretical foundation relating <strong>art</strong> products to innate and inherited personal and universal<br />

potentialities.<br />

Psychoanalytic interpretation <strong>of</strong> content has thrown the only major light on<br />

access to <strong>art</strong> and <strong>art</strong>ists through their paintings. <strong>The</strong>re have been accusations <strong>of</strong> obscurity<br />

due to the metaphoric language. However, there has been limited involvement with true<br />

psychotic <strong>art</strong>, and more concentration on the psychoanalysis <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong>ist. Followers<br />

who did involve themselves have largely abandoned Freud's categorisation <strong>of</strong> primary<br />

(primitive) and secondary (rational) processes and derived their methods from dream<br />

analysis techniques with theoretical excerpts from Jung.<br />

<strong>The</strong> psychiatrist, Schilder, working with the <strong>art</strong>ist, Levine, in 1942, describes the<br />

development <strong>of</strong> this thesis for <strong>art</strong> therapy:<br />

Drawings which are <strong>of</strong>fered during the analysis can be handled in the<br />

same way as dream material irrespective <strong>of</strong> whether they have contents in<br />

the common sense or whether they are to be classified as abstract <strong>art</strong>. <strong>The</strong><br />

drawing corresponds to the manifest content <strong>of</strong> a dream and also abstract<br />

forms are basically the expression <strong>of</strong> human problems and conflicts. <strong>The</strong><br />

material at hand allows the conclusion that in abstract forms p<strong>art</strong>icularly,<br />

primitive and important drives make their appearance. <strong>The</strong>ir study is<br />

therefore revealing, not only from the point <strong>of</strong> view <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> but also from the<br />

point <strong>of</strong> view <strong>of</strong> therapy 27 .<br />

reflect basic components <strong>of</strong> the human psyche.<br />

27<br />

Paul Schilder (1942) Mind: perception and thought in their constructive aspects , Oxford U.P reprint<br />

1981, p.10.<br />

24


Simplistically, <strong>art</strong> is understood to access the unconscious processes between two<br />

people, and the theory is based around the process <strong>of</strong> projective identification, sometimes<br />

with the image and sometimes with the therapist. <strong>The</strong> purpose <strong>of</strong> the analysis is to<br />

mediate. Bringing the unconscious in a controlled way to consciousness is deemed to<br />

release the emotions (catharsis): universal images may be interpreted as archetypal<br />

symbols, i.e. they <strong>of</strong>ten mean the same things to different people.<br />

A number <strong>of</strong> prominent analysts were seriously involved with <strong>art</strong> besides<br />

Winnicott 28 ; Ernst Kris, the Freudian psychoanalyst and <strong>art</strong> historian, contributed a<br />

study <strong>of</strong> a psychotic sculptor 29 and developed the crucial explanation <strong>of</strong> the creative<br />

process as "regression in the service <strong>of</strong> the ego" 30 . No analyst has succeeded in<br />

penetrating the world <strong>of</strong> the psychotic <strong>art</strong>ist and the image as far as Marion Milner,<br />

whose book In <strong>The</strong> Hands <strong>of</strong> <strong>The</strong> Living God (1969) detailed her work (1943-1959) with<br />

Susan, a schizophrenic girl, which has had a great influence on the theoretical<br />

development <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy 31 . Milner wrote this book as an inditement on insensitive<br />

treatment <strong>of</strong> patients in hospital, <strong>of</strong> the isolation <strong>of</strong> the schizoid and the need to make<br />

contact. She described the tendency for some patients to produce spontaneous drawings,<br />

28<br />

for others see: H. F. Ellenberger (1968), <strong>The</strong> Concept <strong>of</strong> Creative Illness, Psychoanalytic Review , 55,<br />

pp.442-56; MacGregor 1989, op.cit. gives a very detailed analysis; D.W. Winnicott (1971), Playing and<br />

Reality London: Tavistock.<br />

29 Ernst Kris (1952), study <strong>of</strong> Messerschmidt, a psychotic <strong>art</strong>ist (trans. chap 4), in<br />

explorations , op.cit. p.128-50.<br />

25<br />

Psychoanalytic<br />

30<br />

R. Shafer (1958) Regression in the service <strong>of</strong> the ego: the relevance <strong>of</strong> a psychoanalytic concept for<br />

personality assessment in G. Lindzey (ed.), Assessment <strong>of</strong> Human Motives , London: Grove Press, pp.119-<br />

148, explores in more detail the idea presented by Kris (1952, op.cit) that creation or enjoyment <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong>s<br />

involves a controlled use <strong>of</strong> the capacity to shift quickly from mature cognitive activity to less mature<br />

forms.<br />

31<br />

for example, authors who acknowledge her influence: Dalley and Case (1992) op.cit.; Schaverien (1992)<br />

op.cit.; Waller and Gilroy (1992) op.cit.


the content <strong>of</strong> which was seemingly dependent on the pathological depths <strong>of</strong> the patient,<br />

and the more contact the patient had, the more the need to draw diminished 32 . Among<br />

other influential case studies was that <strong>of</strong> a woman who could draw but not speak about<br />

her painful emotional experiences, by the Australian psychiatrist, Meares 33 , and another<br />

by Jungian psychiatrist, Baynes 34 , paralleling the clinical techniques <strong>of</strong> therapeutic <strong>art</strong><br />

practice in the 1940-50s. Recent traditional psychoanalytic works have not dep<strong>art</strong>ed<br />

from this type <strong>of</strong> careful in-depth study 35 and there is further promise <strong>of</strong> development<br />

from new theoretical structures, such as from the writings <strong>of</strong> Lacan 36 .<br />

<strong>The</strong> overriding agreement in all serious psychoanalytic writing is that it is time-<br />

consuming, p<strong>art</strong>icularly in the development <strong>of</strong> a relationship, and there can be few<br />

immediate results, but this important qualification is disregarded by many <strong>art</strong> therapy<br />

perspectives <strong>of</strong> these techniques 37 , another qualification from psychoanalytic literature,<br />

32 An observation which has generally been ignored by her later followers.<br />

33<br />

Ainslie Meares (1958), <strong>The</strong> Door <strong>of</strong> Serenity .<br />

34<br />

H. G. Baynes (1940), Mythology <strong>of</strong> the Soul London: Tindall Cox.<br />

35<br />

A recent example is the 20 year analysis <strong>of</strong> a female patient "recovering a hidden <strong>art</strong>istic talent" and the<br />

discussion <strong>of</strong> theories arising from the paintings; Margaret I. Little (1997), Miss Alice M. and her Dragon ,<br />

New York: Binghampton.<br />

36 Lacan's esoteric ideas have made a massive impact on most aspects <strong>of</strong> psychoanalysis, especially his<br />

reinterpretations <strong>of</strong> Freud. When it is possible to assess how Lacan's later view <strong>of</strong> the image, as a separate<br />

category, fits in with his theory <strong>of</strong> signifiers, it may <strong>of</strong>fer much to the study <strong>of</strong> <strong>psychopathology</strong> through<br />

systems other than language, J. Lacan (1988) <strong>The</strong> Topic <strong>of</strong> the Imaginary, in J.A. Miller (ed.), <strong>The</strong><br />

Seminar <strong>of</strong> Jacques Lacan , Book 1 (trans. Forrester), Cambridge U.P.; Bruce Fink (1995), <strong>The</strong> Lacanian<br />

Subject , Oxford: Princeton U.P.<br />

37 see these examples from collected essays edited by well-respected <strong>art</strong>-therapy authors and lecturers (who<br />

do not expound these views themselves): Sheila McClelland (1992), Brief Art <strong>The</strong>rapy in Acute States:<br />

a process oriented approach, in Waller and Gilroy op.cit., pp.189-207, <strong>The</strong> author claims to draw on the<br />

work <strong>of</strong> Dr. Mindell on process science and psychological interventions in psychiatry and the personal<br />

construct work <strong>of</strong> G.A. Kelly (1955). She interprets many <strong>of</strong> the typical features <strong>of</strong> work presented by<br />

psychotic and neurotic referrals and recommends 1. rapid establishment <strong>of</strong> rapport, 2. maintaining a focus,<br />

3. Active assertive therapist style and 4. anticipating the ending, but qualifies that considerable training<br />

is needed. This method is described as "a minimal intervention as it aims to draw forth what is already<br />

26


neglected as a matter <strong>of</strong> course now for <strong>art</strong>-therapy 'referrals', is that the <strong>art</strong> must elicit<br />

spontaneously from the patient 38 . MacGregor makes the point that he discusses little<br />

<strong>of</strong> the contribution <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapists to the history <strong>of</strong> investigation <strong>of</strong> the image making<br />

<strong>of</strong> psychotic individuals, "<strong>The</strong> interference <strong>of</strong> individuals with minimal training in either<br />

psychiatry or psychotherapy cannot be seen as an advantage" 39 , although he goes on to<br />

point out that a full psychoanalytic training with <strong>art</strong> skills makes a valuable contribution<br />

to the therapeutic milieu, giving the example <strong>of</strong> Margaret Naumberg, psychoanalyst and<br />

generally acknowledged as the inventor <strong>of</strong> American psychodynamic <strong>art</strong> therapy 40 .<br />

Demonstrations <strong>of</strong> the therapeutic ineffectiveness <strong>of</strong> psychoanalytic treatment<br />

have led to a loss <strong>of</strong> confidence in the theory, but it at least <strong>of</strong>fers a framework <strong>of</strong><br />

happening"; Paola Luzzatto (1989), Drinking problems and short-term <strong>art</strong> therapy: working with images<br />

<strong>of</strong> withdrawal and clinging, in Andrea Gilroy and Tessa Dalley, eds., op.cit., pp.207-219 discusses<br />

treatment <strong>of</strong> 2 patients who were also attending group therapy, through interpretation "linking behaviour<br />

and feelings - past and present" and confrontation "allowing, or even encouraging, the exploration <strong>of</strong><br />

alternative ways <strong>of</strong> defence" in only 11 hours <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy by "stimulating free associations to the image".<br />

She assumes transference to the imagery and supports her views with a variety <strong>of</strong> noncontextual references,<br />

assumes a therapeutic relationship, and none <strong>of</strong> the interpretations <strong>of</strong> imagery or counselling relates to the<br />

patient's drinking, but to "underlying problems". She discusses the anger <strong>of</strong> her second patient who "had<br />

<strong>art</strong> therapy chosen for him". <strong>The</strong> images were recognised by the patients as symbolic <strong>of</strong> their own attitude.<br />

38<br />

Margaret Naumberg (1947) Studies <strong>of</strong> the free <strong>art</strong> expression <strong>of</strong> behaviour disturbed children as a means<br />

<strong>of</strong> diagnosis and therapy , New York: J.Nervous Mental Dis. Monographs, Cooleridge Foundn.; (1950)<br />

Schizophrenic Art, Its meaning in Psychotherapy New York: Grune Stratton; Art <strong>The</strong>rapy: its scope and<br />

function in E.F. Hammer, ed. (1958), Clinical Applications <strong>of</strong> Projective D rawing , Springfield: Thomas;<br />

(1966) Dynamically Orientated Art <strong>The</strong>rapy: Its Principles and Practices New<br />

York: Grune Stratton; and Marion Milner 1969, op.cit., were both very specific that the patient should<br />

come spontaneously to <strong>art</strong> and not be given standard exercises.<br />

39 MacGregor (1989), op.cit. p.311.<br />

40<br />

Margaret Naumburg, (1947, 1950, 1958, 1966, op.cit.); Kris was p<strong>art</strong>icularly conscious <strong>of</strong> the<br />

psychoanalytically orientated form <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy developed by Naumburg and refers to it as providing<br />

detailed case material "I believe that future students <strong>of</strong> this question will be strongly impelled to draw on<br />

the material so carefully presented by Naumburg. In no other similar publication <strong>of</strong> which I know is there<br />

for instance, an equal opportunity to compare graphic and verbal productions <strong>of</strong> one patient", Kris (1953),<br />

Review <strong>of</strong> Schizophrenic Art, by Margaret Naumberg Psychoanalytic Qu<strong>art</strong>erly V.22: 98-101. Kris also<br />

makes reference to H.G. Baynes (1940) op.cit. as providing an example <strong>of</strong> the Jungian approach to the<br />

same problem.<br />

27


approach. Some <strong>of</strong> the major criticisms <strong>of</strong> psychoanalytic theory 41 would more than<br />

equally apply to their <strong>art</strong> derivatives: the metaphorical language, vague theoretical<br />

concepts and the assumed correspondence between concept and behaviour. Farrell 42<br />

notes that belief and attitude towards psychoanalytic interpretation <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> depend on<br />

previous disposition and training and the weight attached to Freudian theories. Freud<br />

himself admitted much about the weakness <strong>of</strong> psychoanalytic methods and serious<br />

criticisms have been made at all levels 43 . Art therapy is not psychoanalysis, but some<br />

techniques have been absorbed, albeit sometimes in a non-contextual and piecemeal way.<br />

Dalley recognises that <strong>art</strong> therapists are not agreed as to what actually constitutes <strong>art</strong><br />

therapy 44 . Most <strong>art</strong> therapy explanations <strong>of</strong> how psychoanalytic interpretation and<br />

theory applies to <strong>art</strong> therapy are necessarily abbreviated to fit in with the general context<br />

<strong>of</strong> a book or a paper for clinical practice. <strong>The</strong>y give the impression <strong>of</strong> confusion,<br />

subjectivity, lack <strong>of</strong> context and provide a synthesis <strong>of</strong> sometimes incompatible theory 45 .<br />

41<br />

Ernst Nagel (1959), Methodological issues in psychoanalytic theory, in S. Hook, ed., Psychoanalysis:<br />

Scientific Method and Philosophy: A symposium , London: Grove Press, pp.38-56; Brian Anthony Farrell<br />

(1981), <strong>The</strong> Standing <strong>of</strong> Psychoanalys is , Oxford (originally 1955): OUP<br />

42 Farrell (1955/1981) op.cit. p.82-84 and Freudian theory p.71.<br />

43<br />

For a review see H. J. Eysenck (1961), <strong>The</strong> Effects <strong>of</strong> Psychotherapy, in H. J. Eysenck, ed., Handbook<br />

<strong>of</strong> Abnormal Psychology , London: Basic Books, pp.697-725.<br />

44<br />

Diane Waller and Tessa Dalley (1992), Art <strong>The</strong>rapy: a theoretical perspective, in Waller and Gilroy<br />

op.cit. p.1.<br />

45<br />

For example, Dalley and Case (1992) op.cit. Chapter 4 provides simplistic combinations <strong>of</strong> theories<br />

from different analysts working at different times. Quotes from <strong>art</strong> historian, Peter Fuller ( Art and<br />

Psychoanalysis , 1980), relating to <strong>art</strong>ists, support their deconstruction <strong>of</strong> Freud and others; <strong>The</strong> published<br />

papers are discussed in more detail in Chapter 2; <strong>The</strong>re are numerous examples <strong>of</strong> confusing language but<br />

even the more respected practitioners are culpable Schaverien (1992), op.cit. in attempting to explain her<br />

methods <strong>of</strong> analytical <strong>art</strong> psychotherapy uses psychoanalytic terms, mixed with her own derivatives, states<br />

that "mythical thought processes are evident within the pictures" (p.41) and speaks <strong>of</strong> "sympathetic magic"<br />

as a technique and explanation. Further explanation by association to psychoanalysis: "In analytical <strong>art</strong><br />

psychotherapy as in psychoanalysis, the scientific and the mythical modes <strong>of</strong> thought are both <strong>of</strong><br />

28


Many <strong>art</strong> therapists are deeply uncomfortable about the use made <strong>of</strong> psychoanalytic<br />

techniques 46 . Criticism <strong>of</strong> the application does not necessarily imply the theoretical<br />

assumptions are invalid, but that these hypotheses require pro<strong>of</strong> <strong>of</strong> their relevance.<br />

Neither Freud, Jung, nor others have ever produced sufficient evidence.<br />

concern."; By Jung (1946, op.cit. p.12) "<strong>The</strong> patient, by bringing an activated unconscious to bear on the<br />

doctor constellates the corrupting unconscious material in him, owing to the inductive effect which always<br />

emanates from projection in greater or lesser degree. Doctor and patient thus find themselves in a<br />

relationship founded on mutual unconsciousness." This image from Dalley and Case (1992, usually clear)<br />

on how the client takes an active p<strong>art</strong> in the therapy "<strong>The</strong> healthy p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the client aids the sick p<strong>art</strong>,<br />

feeding it and, in doing so, feeds itself".<br />

46<br />

Dalley and Case (1992) op.cit., p.53-54; J. Champernowne (1971), Art and <strong>The</strong>rapy: an uneasy<br />

p<strong>art</strong>nership. op.cit.; Shaverien (1992) op.cit., develops her own theories about the role <strong>of</strong> the image as<br />

reflective <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong> therapy process. R.W. Moore (1983), Art <strong>The</strong>rapy with Substance Abusers: a review<br />

<strong>of</strong> the literature, <strong>The</strong> Arts in Psychotherapy,<br />

V.10:251-60 identified 20 papers with a range <strong>of</strong> orientations,<br />

all using structured sessions dominated by <strong>art</strong> techniques rather than psychotherapeutic. Many <strong>art</strong><br />

therapists have adopted systemised protocols <strong>of</strong> assessment such as the DDS (B.M. Cohen, J. Hammer,<br />

S. Singer (1988) <strong>The</strong> Diagnostic Drawing Series (DDS): a systematic approach to <strong>art</strong> therapy evaluation<br />

and research, Arts in Psychotherapy V.15(1):11-21) in direct opposition to recommended principles from<br />

Naumberg and lending support to her unease about the relationship between Freudian analysis and <strong>art</strong><br />

therapy (Naumberg 1966, op.cit.); David Maclagan aired concerns that some therapeutic methods were<br />

incompatible with pictorial imagery, that therapy is assumed to be inherent in the process <strong>of</strong> creating the<br />

pictorial image and that the verbal discourse takes place in a different and more explicit frame <strong>of</strong> mind<br />

(1989) op.cit. this argument, p.10. Maclagan is not arguing against psychoanalysis but is suspicious <strong>of</strong><br />

attempts to decode the picture. He goes on to state that "If there is an '<strong>art</strong>' in this analytic work, then it<br />

is all to <strong>of</strong>ten a devious, detective <strong>art</strong>, concerned with un-doing what the pictorial image is composed <strong>of</strong><br />

and weaving into it a web <strong>of</strong> its own devising"; Janet Lee Bachant and Elliot Adler (1997) Transference:<br />

Co-constructed or brought to the interaction? J. Am. Ps ychoanalytic Assessment , V.45(4):1097-1120<br />

detail the evolution <strong>of</strong> the transference controversy.<br />

29


Gestalt Analysis<br />

Rudolph Arnheim has been a seminal influence on the development <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy 47 . His<br />

was also the monumental figure who presented the most comprehensive and unifying<br />

framework for perceptual, emotional, expressive and cognitive-development for the<br />

interpretation <strong>of</strong> the visual <strong>art</strong>s, Gestalt theory. <strong>The</strong> principles <strong>of</strong> Gestalt, therefore,<br />

pervade much present day thinking about the assessment <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>works by <strong>art</strong> therapy<br />

clients 48 and must therefore be mentioned.<br />

<strong>The</strong> meaning <strong>of</strong> visual data was seen at three distinctive and individual levels: the<br />

representational and recognisable; in symbol systems; and the abstract understructure,<br />

the form <strong>of</strong> everything we see 49 . Any visual event is a form with content but the content<br />

is highly influenced by the significance <strong>of</strong> the constituent p<strong>art</strong>s, such as colour, tone,<br />

texture, dimension, proportion and their compositional relationships to meaning.<br />

47<br />

see <strong>The</strong> Arts in Psychotherapy 1994, V.21(4) passim. Shaun McNiff and Bruce Moon, two prolific and<br />

influential recent authors <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy texts and discussions, acknowledge their debt to Arnheim as do<br />

others (McNiff, Celebrating the Life and Work <strong>of</strong> Rudolf Arnheim, p.247-8, and Rudolf Arnheim: A<br />

Clinician <strong>of</strong> Images, p.249-260; Moon, What Kind <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, p.295-298). Arnheim himself gives<br />

an interesting brief perspective on his interest in expression, <strong>The</strong> Thoughts That Made Me Move, p.245-6.<br />

Arnheim has been on the editorial board <strong>of</strong> the journal, <strong>The</strong> Arts in Psychotherapy since the 1970s.<br />

48<br />

see Dalley and Case, 1992, op.cit. Chapter 6, traces the development <strong>of</strong> psychoanalytic understanding<br />

(from the point <strong>of</strong> view <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy) to the theories <strong>of</strong> A. Ehrenzweig (1967, <strong>The</strong> Hidden Order <strong>of</strong> Art ),<br />

dealing with the unconscious structure <strong>of</strong> the work, actually at odds with the surface constructions <strong>of</strong><br />

Gestalt theory but are here integrated and seen as the predecessors <strong>of</strong> writers such as Adrian Stokes, painter<br />

and aesthetician, with such statements as: "Stokes saw the work <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> itself as an individual separate<br />

object, differentiated, yet made <strong>of</strong> undifferentiated material" (p.133). <strong>The</strong> influence <strong>of</strong> Arnheim can be seen<br />

in much <strong>of</strong> the description <strong>of</strong> assessment <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy, for example, in the reasons for rejection <strong>of</strong><br />

molecular analysis for the global assessment <strong>of</strong> the whole picture (for example in Wadeson, 1980 op.cit.)<br />

and in descriptions <strong>of</strong> how the client experiences <strong>art</strong> in therapy. A recent example <strong>of</strong> the new 'empathetic'<br />

research approach appears in the description <strong>of</strong> a client's experience <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy: Judith Quail and R.W.<br />

Peavy (1994) A phenomenological research study <strong>of</strong> a client's experience in <strong>art</strong> therapy, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , V.21(1): 45-57.<br />

49<br />

Donis A. Dondis (1973), A Primer <strong>of</strong> Visual Literacy Cambridge: MIT Press, p.13.<br />

30


This is not all, however, there is also an emotional reaction, especially to <strong>art</strong>, <strong>of</strong><br />

the kind described by Ralph Ross 50 , which,<br />

Yields an experience <strong>of</strong> the kind we call aesthetic, an experience most <strong>of</strong> us<br />

have in the presence <strong>of</strong> beauty, which gives deep satisfactions. Exactly<br />

why we have these satisfactions has puzzled philosophers for centuries,<br />

but it seems clear that they depend somehow on the qualities and<br />

organization <strong>of</strong> a work <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> including its meanings, not on meanings in<br />

isolation.<br />

Sensing and interpreting were seen to be only a p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the total process <strong>of</strong><br />

perception, varying with expectations, desires and emotional attitudes. Gestalt was<br />

always a theoretical model and many <strong>of</strong> its wider precepts have since been challenged 51 .<br />

Very few <strong>of</strong> Arnheim's hypotheses were ever subjected to experimental pro<strong>of</strong>, p<strong>art</strong>ly<br />

because most <strong>of</strong> the theory is untestable 52 .<br />

Criticism <strong>of</strong> Gestalt theory began early in its development and continued 53 . <strong>The</strong><br />

main focus has been that the definition <strong>of</strong> a criterion for a true Gestalt is the<br />

unpredictability <strong>of</strong> its effect from a knowledge <strong>of</strong> its p<strong>art</strong>s and their relations. This idea<br />

is important to this thesis, because it <strong>of</strong>fers the explanation that a picture is more than<br />

the sum <strong>of</strong> its constituents and so to the belief that pictures can only be assessed on a<br />

50<br />

Ralph Ross (1963) Symbol Systems and Civilisation , New York: Harcourt Brace.<br />

51<br />

For a quick summary <strong>of</strong> the criticism and experiments on the Gestalt position see James Hogg (1969),<br />

Some Psychological <strong>The</strong>ories and the Visual Arts, in Hogg, ed., Psychology and the Visual Arts ,<br />

Harmondsworth: Penguin, p.78-81.<br />

52 J. Hogg (1969), op.cit. makes the point that to appeal for direct evidence in order to evaluate Arnheim's<br />

work would be to miss the point that he has set out to establish a way <strong>of</strong> looking at the psychological<br />

experience <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> not a body <strong>of</strong> experimental detail.<br />

53<br />

But most attacks were on the theory <strong>of</strong> perception: E. Nagel (1952), Wholes, Sums and Organic Unities,<br />

in D. Lerner, ed., P<strong>art</strong>s and Wholes: <strong>The</strong> Hayden Colloquium on Scientific Method and Concept , reprint<br />

1963, London: Macmillan; B. Petermann (1932), <strong>The</strong> Gestalt <strong>The</strong>ory and the Problem <strong>of</strong> Configuration ,<br />

London: Kegan Paul. Arnheim writes from an analytic perspective on <strong>art</strong> and adopts the Gestalt<br />

assumption <strong>of</strong> inherent constructs which may be modified by training and experience.<br />

31


molar level and not by individual elements. Thus we can also look to this literature for<br />

an answer. It is this very independence <strong>of</strong> the whole from its p<strong>art</strong>s that Gestalt is<br />

initially attempting to explain 54 . But, because the effects <strong>of</strong> a complex system cannot be<br />

predicted from its p<strong>art</strong>s, it does not mean that the appropriate relations cannot be<br />

found 55 .<br />

Projective testing<br />

<strong>The</strong> projective drawing test has evolved from the search for emotional and<br />

psychiatric 'indicators', inferred from the acknowledged unreliability in scoring on<br />

intelligence measures in psychiatric populations. Psychoanalytic writings describe how<br />

traits and emotions from the disturbed person are ascribed to another (projection).<br />

Projection accompanies a refusal to acknowledge the projected feelings (denial). It<br />

functions as a defence mechanism 56 to protect the individual from repressed anxiety and<br />

conflicts. A projective test is NOT designed to probe the unconscious, but to provide<br />

a forum where desires, needs, beliefs and attitudes are revealed which may not be<br />

consciously known. It systematically assigns emotional, symbolic or expressive value<br />

to p<strong>art</strong>icular ways <strong>of</strong> marking a painting, or to p<strong>art</strong>icular images. <strong>The</strong> advantage <strong>of</strong><br />

projective tests over the unstructured single case method is in their systematic<br />

54 <strong>The</strong> Gestalt concept "has become the explanatory principle from which as a primary given fact, the<br />

phenomena may be deduced", Petermann (1932) op.cit., p.49.<br />

55<br />

Nagel (1952), op.cit. p.140 comments on criterion from Kohler, 1924, who proposes the same argument,<br />

paraphrased by Hogg (1969), loc.cit.<br />

56 Mary Levens (1989), Working with defence mechanisms in <strong>art</strong> therapy, in Gilroy and Dalley, op.cit.<br />

p.143-6 gives a good description <strong>of</strong> defence mechanisms.<br />

32


application to large numbers. Projective tests are claimed to detect hurt, anxiety,<br />

depression and psychiatric disturbance 57 , however, individual interpretation <strong>of</strong> the<br />

elements by the therapist is essential, since operational definitions are <strong>of</strong>ten ambiguous<br />

or non-existent.<br />

<strong>The</strong> projective <strong>art</strong> test assumes that a given drawing represents the self and that<br />

dysfunction in perception <strong>of</strong> the self will reflect in dysfunction <strong>of</strong> that representation.<br />

This externalising function <strong>of</strong> drawing is very important clinically as it presents a way<br />

<strong>of</strong> communicating outside language. Most projective <strong>art</strong> tests use the human figure. <strong>The</strong><br />

draw-a-person test 58 is a popular intelligence test for IQ or ability measures in normal<br />

children. It uses a friendly medium which is well understood by a child, is not dependent<br />

on language skill and the drawing is rated by a total sum <strong>of</strong> the recognisable p<strong>art</strong>s <strong>of</strong> a<br />

figure. Although developed for children, it is regularly used for adult psychiatric patients,<br />

for which population it is yet to prove reliable, since although it correlates highly with<br />

other developmental tests, it consistently over or under estimates individual scores in<br />

normal populations 59 .<br />

57<br />

see Abell et al, (1994) op.cit. for a review; K.G. Aikman, R.W. Belter and A.J. Finch (1992) Human<br />

Figure Drawings: Validity in assessing intellectual level and academic achievement, J.Clin Psychol. ,<br />

V.48(1):114-120) tested 216 child and adolescent psychiatric patients and found more than half<br />

misclassified. Goodenough herself provides an appendix chapter speculating on the distortions expected<br />

in psychiatric populations (1926, op.cit.).<br />

58<br />

see D. Harris (1963), Children's drawings as measures <strong>of</strong> intellectual maturity , New York: Harcourt<br />

Brace, p.12-36 for a review <strong>of</strong> the early descriptive and developmental studies; the best known is the<br />

Goodenough-Harris Draw-a-man scale, (F.L. Goodenough and D.B. Harris (1963), <strong>The</strong> Goodenough-Harris<br />

Drawing Test , New York: Harcourt, Brace, which revised and extended studies by Florence Goodenough<br />

(1926) Measurement <strong>of</strong> Intelli gence by Drawings , New York: Harcourt Brace.<br />

59<br />

S.C. Abell, A.M. Heiberg and J.E. Johnson (1994), Cognitive Evaluation <strong>of</strong> Young Adults by Means<br />

<strong>of</strong> Human Figure Drawings: an empirical validation <strong>of</strong> 2 methods, J. Clin. Psychol. , V.50(6):900-5; for<br />

a review see L. Slansky, M. Short-Degraff (1989), Validity and Reliability issues with Human Figure<br />

Drawing Assessments, Physical and Occupational <strong>The</strong>rapy in Paediatrics , V.9(3):127-142; and G.H.<br />

Fabry, and J.E. Bertinetti (1990), A Construct Validation <strong>Study</strong> <strong>of</strong> the Human Figure Drawing Test,<br />

33


Attempts to lists characteristics <strong>of</strong> clinical significance have produced no<br />

consensus <strong>of</strong> agreement between studies 60 . When attributes <strong>of</strong> the picture which are<br />

supposed to accompany symptoms are examined, the question <strong>of</strong> what kind <strong>of</strong><br />

accompaniment is still at issue. Projective tests do not have answers, they collect<br />

information. For example, the interpretation <strong>of</strong> a subject's projection onto the highly<br />

ambiguous stimuli <strong>of</strong> inkblots (the Rorschach Test) is said to reveal deep unconscious<br />

needs and personality factors. <strong>The</strong> prototype <strong>of</strong> the projective approach is the landmark<br />

work <strong>of</strong> Machover (1949) 61 , which stresses the significance <strong>of</strong> isolated signs and details<br />

in human figure drawings. <strong>The</strong> usefulness <strong>of</strong> this method has been challenged by the<br />

failure <strong>of</strong> validation, reliability and validity in objective studies, and that it is by no means<br />

certain what they measure, how they work, or if indeed they do work 62 . Later authors<br />

have adopted a more clinical intuitive stance 63 . Wadeson 64 had doubts, as a few other<br />

Perceptual and Motor Skills , V.70:465-466, give modest support for validation but do not recommend<br />

its use in isolation.<br />

60<br />

Harriet Wadeson and William T. Carpenter (1976), A Comparative <strong>Study</strong> <strong>of</strong> Art Expression <strong>of</strong><br />

Schizophrenic, Unipolar Depressive, and Bipolar Manic-Depressive Patients, J. Nervous Mental Dis. , May<br />

V.162(5):334-344. Found characteristics <strong>of</strong> different diagnostic groups <strong>of</strong> patients unrelatable to table<br />

expectations derived from the literature. Wadeson later commented that there was as much evidence for<br />

as against these characteristics (1980 op.cit.) For recent studies, refer to Chapter 2.<br />

61<br />

K. Machover (1949), Personality<br />

10th ed., Charles C. Thomas.<br />

Projection in the Drawing <strong>of</strong> the Human Figure , Springfield IL: 1978<br />

62<br />

Sophia Kahill (1984) Human Figure Drawing in Adults: an update <strong>of</strong> the empirical evidence 1967-1982,<br />

Canadian Psychol. V.25(4):269-292 noted some improvements in research methods but had nothing to<br />

add to previous reviews "While it is obvious that figure drawings are not meaningless, establishing what<br />

it is they mean with any precision or predictability is difficult" (p.288).<br />

63<br />

G.D. Oster and P. Gould (1987), Using Drawings in Assessment and <strong>The</strong>rapy: a guide for mental health<br />

pr<strong>of</strong>essionals , New York: Brunner Mazel, describes a drawing protocol; E. Koppitz (1983), Projective<br />

Drawings in Children and Adults, Sch. Psychol. Review , V.12:421-427. provides informal projective<br />

scoring scores which depend on experience/insight <strong>of</strong> administrator; recent example <strong>of</strong> Silver: Rawley<br />

Silver and Joanne Ellison (1992), Identifying and Assessing Self-Images in Drawings by Delinquent<br />

Adolescents, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.22(4):339-352.<br />

64<br />

Harriet Wadeson (1987), <strong>The</strong> Dynamics <strong>of</strong> Art Psychotherapy , New York: Wiley.<br />

34


writers who have published detailed discussion <strong>of</strong> formal research and its problems 65 ,<br />

over the relevance <strong>of</strong> <strong>quantitative</strong> methodology for assessing the qualitative idiographic<br />

insights provided by interpretation <strong>of</strong> the productions from projective testing. After a<br />

career <strong>of</strong> scientific investigation into the <strong>psychopathology</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>, Wadeson challenged the<br />

received wisdom about what constituted 'schizophrenic' or 'depressed' drawings. She<br />

contended that,<br />

Although the graphic and sculptural characteristics can provide<br />

information about the <strong>art</strong> therapy client's state, patients <strong>of</strong> the same<br />

diagnosis may exhibit different graphic or sculptural characteristics" ...<br />

"<strong>The</strong>re is no such thing as a 'schizophrenic picture'. <strong>The</strong>re are confused<br />

looking pictures, fragmented organization, bizarre representations 66 .<br />

Wadeson <strong>of</strong>fered a set <strong>of</strong> twelve drawings for readers to test their skills to decide if the<br />

<strong>art</strong>ist was psychotic. She implies difficulties.<br />

Few researchers have taken note <strong>of</strong> criticisms in reviews <strong>of</strong> projective tests 67 .<br />

Despite the lack <strong>of</strong> experimental validation 68 , the standardised procedures from projective<br />

65<br />

H. Wadeson (1978), Some uses <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy data in research, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.18(1):11-18; Bruce<br />

Males (1979), Is it right to carry out scientific research into <strong>art</strong> therapy? <strong>The</strong>rapy V.3:5; Males (1980) Art<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy: Investigations and implications, Inscape , 4(2):13-15; T. Dalley (1980), Assessing the therapeutic<br />

effects <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>: an illustrated case study, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.7:11-17; David Edwards (1987)<br />

Evaluation op.cit.; Sean McNiff (1986), Freedom <strong>of</strong> research op.cit.; McNiff (1987) Research and<br />

Scholarship in the creative <strong>art</strong>s therapies, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.14:285-92 arguing against<br />

inappropriate methods for the subject; Andrea Gilroy (1992), Research in Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, in Waller and<br />

Gilroy, op.cit., pp.229-247; Michael Franklin and Rosalie Plitsky (1992), <strong>The</strong> problem <strong>of</strong> interpretation:<br />

implications and strategies for the field <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.19(3):163-175.<br />

66 Wadeson (1987), op.cit. p.93.<br />

67<br />

Generally, A. Anastasi and B. Foley's (1941) 4 <strong>art</strong>icle survey <strong>of</strong> the literature, condemned the poor<br />

quality <strong>of</strong> research: empirical tests were badly reported, lacking in basic information; associations <strong>of</strong><br />

characteristics were idiosyncratic, unreplicated, used extreme groups and results were <strong>of</strong>ten contradictory<br />

or uninterpretable (A survey <strong>of</strong> the literature on <strong>art</strong>istic behaviour in the abnormal IV, experimental<br />

investigations, J. Gen. Psychol. V.23:187-237). Qualitative variation not differences were noted across<br />

groups especially in writing in (p.193). Research on spontaneous work (Section III,<br />

35<br />

Psychological<br />

Monographs , V.52(6):1-71) revealed "ambiguous ill defined mentalistic concepts which add much<br />

confusion to the problem. It is difficult in certain studies to draw the line between observation and<br />

interpretation" (p.64); In 1968, C.H. Swenson, Empirical Evaluations <strong>of</strong> Human Figure Drawings,


tests are still popular in assessment, diagnosis and treatment decisions, probably because<br />

<strong>of</strong> what Swenson (1968) 69 described as "a random p<strong>art</strong>ial reward schedule" due to the<br />

occasional case where projective drawings gave an indication <strong>of</strong> a client's problem,<br />

although clinical caution in their interpretation seems rare 70 . <strong>The</strong>re have been very few<br />

empirical studies, reflected in the lack <strong>of</strong> confidence in statistical approaches, and these<br />

mostly still lack methodological validity 71 . <strong>The</strong> focus has been to isolate the diagnostic<br />

content through association <strong>of</strong> symbols or identify 'markers' 72 . However, some studies<br />

show naive raters can globally categorise pictures <strong>of</strong> patients and non-patients through<br />

'intuitive' feelings <strong>of</strong> abnormality 73 and can be reliable. Thus it seems that there is<br />

Psychological Bulletin , V.70:20-44, advised researchers attend to characteristics <strong>of</strong> the work rather than<br />

unrelated hypotheses and criticised interpretive assumptions. Hammer (1968, op.cit.) and others after<br />

made similar recommendations, but Cathy Malchiodi (1994) op.cit. was able to make virtually the same<br />

criticisms describing the term 'assessment' as a misnomer, most tests as directives, and that few interrater<br />

studies, reliable and valid scoring systems were available.<br />

68<br />

E.F. Hammer (1968) op.cit., Clinical Application <strong>of</strong> projective drawings; Swenson's eight year reviews<br />

<strong>of</strong> research (C.H. Swenson, Empirical Evaluations <strong>of</strong> Human Figure Drawings, Psychological Bulletin<br />

1957, V.54:431-466, and 1968 op.cit.) concluded that "Machover's hypotheses concerning the DAP have<br />

seldom been supported by the research" and more recent publications have nothing to add: Scott (1981)<br />

Measuring Intelligence with the Goodenough-Harris Drawing test, Psychol. Bull. V.89:483-505; Kahill<br />

(1984), op.cit.; Abell (1994), op.cit.<br />

69 Swenson (1968) op.cit. p.370.<br />

70<br />

Most writers acknowledge the appropriate uses for the DAP are with body image disorders, such as this<br />

example: Sandy K. Reuven (1998), Reversal <strong>of</strong> a body image disorder (Macrosomatognosia) in Parkinson's<br />

disease by treatment with AC pulsed electromagnetic fields, Intl. J. <strong>of</strong> Neuroscience 1998, V.93:1-2.<br />

Abuses <strong>of</strong> the process continue, for example, the use <strong>of</strong> the Goodenough-Harris Draw-a-Man test, as<br />

developmental indicator for adult psychiatric populations, despite the lack <strong>of</strong> research evidence for it,<br />

general admonition (inc. the authors) and numerous studies condemning the practise as unreliable.<br />

71 <strong>The</strong> claim by the authors <strong>of</strong> the Diagnostic Drawing Series, a standardised evaluation from 3 pictures,<br />

to have demonstrated replicable differences between patients (B. Cohen et al., 1988, op.cit.) is discredited<br />

at length in the reliability analysis, Chapter 2 <strong>of</strong> this thesis.<br />

72<br />

Recent studies (over the last 22 years), both empirical and other are discussed in the literature review,<br />

Chapter 2.<br />

73<br />

Bernard I. Levy and Elinor Ulman (1967), Judging Psychopathology from Paintings, J. Abnormal<br />

Psychol. , V.72(2):182-7; Ulman and Levy (1968), An Experimental Approach to the Judgement <strong>of</strong><br />

Psychopathology from Paintings, Bull. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.8(1):3-12; Ulman and Levy (1973), Art <strong>The</strong>rapists<br />

as Diagnosticians, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.13:35-8. <strong>The</strong>se <strong>art</strong>icles reprinted three times (1975, 1984 and<br />

36


something recognisable about the <strong>art</strong>work which has not been isolated. Projective testing<br />

approaches continue to be developed 74 , and recently there have been encouraging<br />

developments based on more rigidly defined formal criteria with children 75 .<br />

<strong>The</strong> research contribution <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapy to assessment issues<br />

Wadeson's (1976) table <strong>of</strong> psychiatric characteristics <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> from the previous literature<br />

had little empirical basis 76 . Gantt and Schmal's (1974) annotated bibliography <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong><br />

therapy literature over 33 years 77 listed 1175 items from English and foreign language<br />

sources: only 39 were classified as 'research' and not all dealt with <strong>art</strong> therapy. <strong>The</strong>re was<br />

little attempt at replication, reliability, and definition <strong>of</strong> terms. Linda Gantt and J.<br />

Howie's (1979, unpublished) ch<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> correspondences between characteristics <strong>of</strong> the<br />

<strong>art</strong>work <strong>of</strong> patients and the DSM111 seemed to <strong>of</strong>fer an alternative framework for<br />

1992) show that from 55 judges <strong>of</strong> varying backgrounds, some <strong>art</strong> or psychiatry related, all predicted<br />

patient status better than chance; Linda Gantt (1990) tested 6 <strong>art</strong> therapy graduates on 30 pictures between<br />

6 DSMIIIR groups and attained the same results: A Validity <strong>Study</strong> <strong>of</strong> the Formal Elements Art <strong>The</strong>rapy<br />

Scale (F EATS) for diagnostic information in patients' drawings , Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, U.<br />

Pittsburgh, Pensylvania, U.S.A.<br />

74<br />

For the main projective tests in use today, see D. Arrington (1992), Art-based assessment procedures and<br />

instruments used in research, in H. Wadeson, A Guide to Conducting Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Research , Mundelein,<br />

IL: Am. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Assn., pp.157-178.<br />

75<br />

Micheal S. Trevisan (1996), Review <strong>of</strong> the Draw A Person: Screening Procedure for Emotional<br />

Disturbance, Measurement and Evaluation Counselling and Development V.28(4):225-8. Reviews the<br />

DAP:SPED by J. Naglieri and S.I. Pfeiffer (1992) Performance <strong>of</strong> disruptive behaviour disordered and<br />

normal samples <strong>of</strong> the DAP:SPED, Psychological Assessment , V.4(2):156-159 with a complex and<br />

comprehensive projective approach to assess 55 items <strong>of</strong> emotional disturbance in children.<br />

76 Wadeson and Carpenter (1976), Comparative <strong>Study</strong> op.cit. were unable to relate characteristics <strong>of</strong><br />

different diagnostic groups <strong>of</strong> patients to a table derived from general analysis <strong>of</strong> expectations from the<br />

literature.<br />

77 L. Gantt and M. Schmal (1974), Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, A Bibliography (1940-73) (George Washington University<br />

and National Institute <strong>of</strong> Mental Health (NIMH), U.S.A (unpublished).<br />

37


investigation <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>work, but few have taken up this challenge 78 . Moore's review <strong>of</strong> 392<br />

papers from 1974-1980 79 found only 69 claimed to deal with research, although some<br />

were speculations and suggestions for research possibilities. <strong>The</strong> bulk <strong>of</strong> the literature,<br />

even in the 1990s has been concerned with techniques <strong>of</strong> therapy. <strong>The</strong>re have been no<br />

recent comprehensive reviews (to my knowledge) to bring us up to date 80 . <strong>The</strong> spate <strong>of</strong><br />

books in the 1970-80s were illustrated, focused on case material and theoretical positions<br />

and had little to say about research or diagnostic questions and the '90s 'cookbooks' <strong>of</strong><br />

techniques and theories 81 advocate feminist 82 , phenomenological, empathetic and<br />

qualitative approaches using case studies 83 . <strong>The</strong> phenomenological type <strong>of</strong> investigation<br />

78 Ch<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> pictorial characteristics equating to diagnostic symptomatology related to DSMIII categories<br />

(1979), NIMH unpublished. Linda Gantt advises that the ch<strong>art</strong> is now out <strong>of</strong> date and the work must be<br />

redone (personal correspondence, 1994).<br />

79<br />

R. Moore (1981), Art <strong>The</strong>rapy in Mental Health , Rockville MD: NIMH.<br />

80 Diane Waller's review takes us only to 1982.<br />

81<br />

Such as Bruce Moon's (1992) rather evangelical Essentials <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy training and practice ,<br />

Springfield, IL: Thomas; Leah B<strong>art</strong>al and Nira Ne'eman (1993), <strong>The</strong> Metaphoric Body: Guide to<br />

Expressive Th erapy Through Images and Archetypes , London: J. Kingsley; Waller and Gilroy (1992),<br />

Handbook, op.cit.<br />

82<br />

Shirley Riley (1997), Conflicts in Treatment Issues <strong>of</strong> Liberation, Connection and Culture: Art <strong>The</strong>rapy<br />

for Women and their Families, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.14(2):102-8 on women's need to be heard and access to<br />

communication in male dominated treatment systems; Franklin and Plitsky (1992) problems <strong>of</strong><br />

interpretation, op.cit.<br />

83<br />

see for example discussion <strong>of</strong> propriety <strong>of</strong> empirical research, advocating separate systems <strong>of</strong> empathetic,<br />

understanding research and diagnosis on the basis that problems <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy clients are 'essentially<br />

creative': Borrowsky Junge and Linesch (1993), Our Own Voices op.cit.; Linda Gantt (1986), 'Systematic<br />

investigation <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> works: some research models drawn from neighbouring fields, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy ,<br />

V.24(4):111-18 arguing inadequate training <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapists for empirical research therefore more rigour<br />

in appropriate methods; Andrea Gilroy (1992), Research in Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, in Waller and Gilroy, Handbook,<br />

op.cit. pp.229-247 cites many more advocates for phenomenological research; Helen Payne, ed. (1993),<br />

Handbook <strong>of</strong> Inquiry in the Art <strong>The</strong>rapies: One River, Many Currents , London: Kingsley, passim; David<br />

Aldridge (1994), Single-Case Research Designs for<br />

the Creative Art <strong>The</strong>rapist, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.21(5):333-342; Herman Smitskamp (1995), <strong>The</strong><br />

Problem <strong>of</strong> Pr<strong>of</strong>essional Diagnosis in the Arts <strong>The</strong>rapies, Arts in Psychotherapy,<br />

V.22(3):181-187; Rosalie<br />

H. Politsky (1995), Towards a Typology <strong>of</strong> Research in the Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapies, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , V.22(4):307-314; Cathy Malchiode (1993), crisis, op.cit.<br />

2 recent examples <strong>of</strong> these processes: Quail and Peavy (1994), phenomenological study, op.cit. use 'the<br />

verbal descriptions <strong>of</strong> the client as the main source <strong>of</strong> data'. <strong>The</strong>se retrospective impressions <strong>of</strong> a group<br />

38


is thought to generate more appropriate information than empirical techniques for<br />

psychotherapy, and fits in with a general trend <strong>of</strong> more integrated approaches suggested<br />

by the new breed <strong>of</strong> recent books 84 . However, studies based on these precepts present<br />

a deluge <strong>of</strong> unanalysed information in which it is difficult to distinguish a purpose or<br />

research value.<br />

<strong>The</strong> problems <strong>of</strong> the approach to psychiatric pictures<br />

Single case descriptive methods, projective tests and indeed most descriptions or<br />

interpretations <strong>of</strong> psychiatric paintings have in common an approach typical <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong><br />

criticism to the paintings, which has been applied without consideration <strong>of</strong> the<br />

assumptions the language implies. One <strong>of</strong> the most obvious assumptions is the conscious<br />

and serious intention <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong>ist 85 . This approach cannot cope with involuntary<br />

experience were stimulated by interviews about selected drawings and relate change in the client to the <strong>art</strong><br />

process. Quail feels that this validates and confirms the value <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>-therapy; Debra Linesch (1994),<br />

Interpretation in Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Research and Practice: <strong>The</strong> Hermeneutic Circle, <strong>The</strong> Arts in Psychotherapy ,<br />

V.21(3):185-195 applies a model <strong>of</strong> interaction to a case study <strong>of</strong> an abused girl.<br />

84<br />

for example: Frederick J. Leger (1998), Beyond the therapeutic relationship: behavioral, biological and<br />

cognitive foundations <strong>of</strong> psychotherapy , New York: Haworth, attempts to integrates and unite multifarious<br />

psychotherapies and discusses scientific difficulties; Arthur Robbins et al.(1998), <strong>The</strong>rapeutic Presence:<br />

Bridging Expression and Form , London: Kingsley, explores nontraditional interactions; and <strong>art</strong>icles:<br />

Herman M. Adler (1997), Towards a Multimodal Communication <strong>The</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapy: the vicarious<br />

coprocession, Am J. <strong>of</strong> Psychotherapy , V.51(1):54-66, supports common therapeutic processes <strong>of</strong> music,<br />

<strong>art</strong> and other events; Sarah G. Banker, <strong>The</strong> Power <strong>of</strong> Art and Story: women therapists create their own fairy<br />

tales, in A. Robbins, <strong>The</strong>rapeutic Presence op.cit., explains how symbolic presences open up multiple<br />

solutions for expression; G.L. Engels (1977), <strong>The</strong> Need for a New Medical Model: a challenge for<br />

Biomedicine, Science , April 8, V.196(4286):129-37, discusses the out-<strong>of</strong>-date medical model <strong>of</strong> disease<br />

and the new personal rather than illness focus; Ruth Cohn Balletino (1998), <strong>The</strong> Need for a New Ethical<br />

Model in Medicine: a challenge for conventional, alternative and complementary practitioners, Advances ,<br />

V.14(1):6-16 joins the debate and introduces suggestions for a new ethics code for new boundaries.<br />

85 Gilroy and Dalley (1989) Pictures at an Exhibition, op.cit. is a synthesis <strong>of</strong> essays dealing with <strong>art</strong><br />

therapy <strong>of</strong> mental patients, and psychoanalytic examination <strong>of</strong> the work <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>ists, using the same kind <strong>of</strong><br />

language and treatment <strong>of</strong> the subjects, and assuming an active creative process. Recent example <strong>of</strong> use<br />

<strong>of</strong> language and aesthetic analysis to <strong>art</strong>work <strong>of</strong> mental illness in Dalley and Case (1992), op.cit. Ch. 6,<br />

Development <strong>of</strong> psychoanalytic understanding p.119-145; Art and Psychoanalysis p.71-96.<br />

39


expressions <strong>of</strong> a disordered psyche. <strong>The</strong>refore conventional methods <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> criticism are<br />

not sufficient to identify the psychiatric components <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>work produced by patients.<br />

<strong>The</strong> madman is not an <strong>art</strong>ist and neither is <strong>art</strong> a cure for madness 86 .<br />

86<br />

John Birchtnell (1981) op.cit. recognises (but does not subscribe to) the belief by <strong>art</strong> therapists that<br />

making <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> is in itself therapeutic; Refer back to introduction to ch.1 for examples <strong>of</strong> this theory; Shaun<br />

McNiff, a prolific author on <strong>art</strong> therapy, has described the <strong>art</strong> therapist as "One who works with the<br />

supernatural, a technician <strong>of</strong> the sacred, a master <strong>of</strong> ecstacy, mystic healer, priest and <strong>art</strong>ist" (1979 - From<br />

shamanism to <strong>art</strong> therapy, Art Psychotherapy V.6(3):155-61).<br />

40


<strong>The</strong> problems <strong>of</strong> secondary sources as data<br />

Most research still relies on clinical observation <strong>of</strong> case studies and they comprise the<br />

major p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the published literature. <strong>The</strong>se reports suggest that the <strong>art</strong> produced by the<br />

patient changes with improvement in the patient, and more contraversially that the act<br />

<strong>of</strong> making the <strong>art</strong> promotes change in the patient. <strong>The</strong> generally accepted solution to the<br />

recognised influence <strong>of</strong> the interpreter on the interpretation has been to have the patient<br />

provide a verbal explanation <strong>of</strong> the picture 87 . So far, serious methodological difficulties<br />

have not been recognised or addressed and reliable evidence for interpretive accounts is<br />

not available 88 .<br />

Presently, the measures which are used to assess <strong>art</strong> in therapy assess the<br />

therapist's or the client's opinions <strong>of</strong> the psychotherapeutic process, or attempt to<br />

correlate verbal interpretations with the visual products. <strong>The</strong> transformation to a verbal<br />

explanation is useful, because the language <strong>of</strong> psychiatric diagnosis helps place the<br />

elements <strong>of</strong> the picture in a clinical framework. However this transformation loses its<br />

visual integrity <strong>of</strong> form as it places the emphasis on what the patient tells us, or even<br />

what the therapist tells us, with a focus on interpretation <strong>of</strong> a covert message. In this<br />

respect it loses the integrity <strong>of</strong> psychiatric diagnosis, in which form is implicit 89 . This<br />

way <strong>of</strong> thinking about <strong>art</strong> has not been shown to be wrong, but it is difficult to test the<br />

truth <strong>of</strong> it because <strong>of</strong> the problem that we have to rely on what people tell us about their<br />

87<br />

But that interpretation may not be a translation has been recognised, David Maclagan (1989), op.cit.;<br />

H. Wadeson (1975) Is interpretation <strong>of</strong> sexual symbolism necessary? Arts in Psychotherapy,<br />

V.2(3-4):233-<br />

9.<br />

88 Franklin and Plitsky (1992) Problems <strong>of</strong> interpretation, op.cit.<br />

89 K. Jaspers (1963)<br />

General Psychopathology (Manchester U.P., 7th ed. Eng. Trans.).<br />

41


own paintings, or on interpretations from others, or even on speculative models. <strong>The</strong>se<br />

alternatives are certainly subjective, probably incomplete and/or distorted, but they are<br />

virtually all we have to work with. Science has nothing to <strong>of</strong>fer when it comes to<br />

symbols or unravelling <strong>of</strong> metaphor. <strong>The</strong>re are no generally accepted methods for<br />

translation nor is there any sign <strong>of</strong> such a thing in the near future. Any picture can be<br />

interpreted in quite different ways by any two 'experts' each following their own theories<br />

and I know <strong>of</strong> no empirical evidence which supports one approach at the expense <strong>of</strong><br />

another. <strong>The</strong>re is evidence that there are loose associations between psychiatric<br />

disturbance and psychiatric indicators in pictures, but we cannot assume there will be a<br />

simple point for point mapping between verbally explained and non-verbal<br />

symptomatology, or even a direct relationship.<br />

How will research benefit from a <strong>psychopathology</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>?<br />

As a general topic, there are a surprisingly high number <strong>of</strong> unknowns about <strong>art</strong> activity.<br />

We know very little about visuospatial cognitions except that understanding is not coded<br />

verbally. Advances in behaviourial science, the clearer definition <strong>of</strong> diagnostic<br />

classifications for mental illness and the greater accuracy <strong>of</strong> statistical procedures give<br />

hope for advancement in fields which have been traditionally problematical because <strong>of</strong> the<br />

interaction <strong>of</strong> confounding variables.<br />

Specifically, for <strong>art</strong> therapy to be seen to be effective: that is, to change the<br />

condition <strong>of</strong> the patient, psychotherapy needs to be closely related to topics in which the<br />

symptomatology is present. This has not yet been demonstrated in the effects <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong><br />

42


therapy. It would markedly improve the psychiatric impression <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy to<br />

identify the conditions where <strong>art</strong> therapy is most relevant and also to identify the<br />

direction in which it would be most effective to go 90 .<br />

More speculatively, a technique which sets us on the road to map visuospatial<br />

functions may allow a clearer neurobiological understanding <strong>of</strong> disorder. <strong>The</strong> ability to<br />

describe symptomatology relating to widely differing cerebral locations would be <strong>of</strong> help<br />

to neurobiological research. <strong>The</strong>re may be groups <strong>of</strong> disturbed patients whose condition<br />

may only be described effectively with visuospatial <strong>psychopathology</strong> and these may<br />

currently be misdiagnosed and treated.<br />

<strong>The</strong> need for another <strong>art</strong> assessment<br />

Research based on reported interpretive techniques and clinical observation <strong>of</strong> patient's<br />

paintings has proved difficult, p<strong>art</strong>ly because <strong>of</strong> ambiguities in the description.<br />

Confusion has also arisen in the description <strong>of</strong> diagnosis, outcome <strong>of</strong> treatments and<br />

research methodology. It is unlikely that single case studies will provide more<br />

information and there are no systematic assessment measures which explore the<br />

visual/verbal split.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re seems no reason why there cannot be formal characteristics which<br />

correspond with p<strong>art</strong>icular diagnoses or symptomatology. Projective testing has not seen<br />

the formal elements <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong>work as the focus <strong>of</strong> systematic study, and evidence<br />

90<br />

Some <strong>of</strong> this is covered by Joan Woddis (1986) Judging by Appearances, Arts in Psychotherapy ,<br />

V.13(2): 147-9; also Maralynn Hagood (1990) Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Research in England: Impressions <strong>of</strong> an<br />

American <strong>art</strong> therapist, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.17(1): 75-9.<br />

43


pointing to integrative or global judgments rather than specific indicators seems to<br />

indicate diagnosis <strong>of</strong> holistic disturbance. No evidence, however, supports the benefits<br />

<strong>of</strong> such separate diagnosis for psychotherapeutic treatment. Most studies <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> have<br />

chosen not to study the <strong>art</strong>work itself, but the message conveyed. This approach neglects<br />

possible non-verbal elements and also unconscious disclosures.<br />

<strong>The</strong> way forward<br />

If diagnosis matters, <strong>psychopathology</strong> matters. <strong>The</strong>re are strong hints <strong>of</strong> systematic<br />

differences between two dimensional <strong>art</strong>work from different groups <strong>of</strong> patients. We<br />

cannot understand completely how we use communication by studying only the meaning<br />

<strong>of</strong> what is communicated. In order to study meaningful pathology <strong>of</strong> visuospatial<br />

relations, it is necessary, not only to investigate what is communicated, but also how it<br />

is communicated. We must look at communication from the point <strong>of</strong> communication - the<br />

object <strong>of</strong> study.<br />

<strong>The</strong> first stage in establishing a measure must be to show the association <strong>of</strong><br />

variables with already existing classifications <strong>of</strong> symptomatology. Previous literature has<br />

failed to do this.<br />

First Steps: Review <strong>of</strong> the literature<br />

So where do we go from here? How can past experience inform and develop a<br />

new approach unless the literature can be compared and classified? Recent suggestions<br />

include comparison <strong>of</strong> case study approaches to produce an overall view <strong>of</strong> qualitative<br />

44


work 91 . This suggestion has merit and this thesis uses impressionistic reports and case<br />

studies to generate comparative information as a complement to that <strong>of</strong> statistical studies,<br />

in order to provide a balanced view <strong>of</strong> the overall approach to the investigation <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong><br />

product in mental illness.<br />

Chapter 2 details the last 20 years <strong>of</strong> published research studies into two<br />

dimensional <strong>art</strong> by psychiatric patients. <strong>The</strong>re are four reasons why this 20 year period<br />

is considered:<br />

1. Reviews and tables <strong>of</strong> pathological characteristics in the <strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> mental disorder from<br />

the more recent literature are neither systematic nor complete.<br />

2. <strong>The</strong> literature on diagnosis in <strong>art</strong> contains similar clinical observations about diagnostic<br />

groups. <strong>The</strong>se generalisations are not supported by statistical or scientific research. <strong>The</strong><br />

relatively recent refinement <strong>of</strong> more reliable diagnostic categories, clinical terminology and<br />

standards <strong>of</strong> reporting now permits a re-examination <strong>of</strong> some basic questions about<br />

psychiatric symptoms in <strong>art</strong>.<br />

3. Earlier research was based on clinical observation from many disciplines, later research<br />

aimed to develop theory and used a variety <strong>of</strong> illustrative background.<br />

4. <strong>The</strong> earlier literature has been adequately summarised before.<br />

91 A. Gilroy (1992) Research in Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, op.cit. p.238, "<strong>The</strong> experimentally based case study ... leads<br />

to data which is easily comparable with other case-studies and types <strong>of</strong> therapeutic intervention".<br />

45


Chapter Two: Review <strong>of</strong> the recent literature over 20 years.<br />

1. Is there a way through the jungle? relevant literature to this study<br />

Anyone who tries to review the literature on <strong>art</strong> and mental health encounters a huge<br />

body <strong>of</strong> literature, most <strong>of</strong> which is <strong>of</strong> poor quality. It is difficult to ascertain what is<br />

known about how the characteristics <strong>of</strong> mental disorder may manifest pathologically in<br />

the <strong>art</strong>work <strong>of</strong> those affected since a published comprehensive and systematic review <strong>of</strong><br />

the more recent literature over the last 20 years is not available. Chapter Two aims to<br />

collect all the available interpretable evidence <strong>of</strong> pathological characteristics in the <strong>art</strong>work<br />

<strong>of</strong> psychiatric patients from published papers over the last 20 years. It was more<br />

appropriate to categorise the literature under methodology rather than concepts as the<br />

literature is constrained by the methodological difficulties which encumber it.<br />

<strong>The</strong> literature search method<br />

Studies met the following inclusion criteria:<br />

(a) Subjects for case and controlled studies were adults or adolescent: the evidence<br />

suggests that children's drawings show considerably more inter-individual<br />

variation than those <strong>of</strong> adults 92 , and there are also developmental complications,<br />

so it seemed sensible to narrow the subject categories in the comparable studies,<br />

to those whose motivations, <strong>psychopathology</strong> and clinical picture there is access.<br />

<strong>The</strong> impressionistic papers showed more range <strong>of</strong> illustration for general<br />

92<br />

Maureen Cox (1992), Children's Drawings , Harmondsworth: Penguin.<br />

46


principles, so some were included where their comments/experiences were<br />

relevant to the range;<br />

(b) Studies involved psychiatric patients 93 ;<br />

(c) Studies examined patients' drawings or paintings 94 .<br />

(d) Reports and papers in English.<br />

Articles for review were selected from a computerised search <strong>of</strong> 3 databases and from a<br />

manual search. <strong>The</strong> Psychlit, Medline and BIDS databases were searched using the<br />

following search criteria:<br />

(i) ART or DRAWING or PAINTING or PICTURE and THERAPY or<br />

PSYCHOPATHOLOGY or PSYCHIAT* or SCHIZOPHRENI* or<br />

PATIENT.<br />

(ii) Not STATE-OF-THE; not SYMPTOM-PICTURE; not CLINICAL-PICTURE;<br />

not CHILD* in DE; or PRESCHOOL in DE; or SCHOOL-AGE in DE; not<br />

PICTURE-SORT*.<br />

This strategy <strong>of</strong> searching anywhere in the abstract for significant words was not efficient<br />

as much had to be eliminated by eye due to the context, but assigned descriptors <strong>of</strong>ten<br />

did not mention <strong>art</strong>work and studies <strong>of</strong> interest were classified under disparate categories.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were considerable overlaps from the BIDS database and Medline with studies<br />

93 <strong>The</strong>re were many studies which used undiagnosed groups (including <strong>art</strong>ists), sometimes described as<br />

'screened' for psychiatric disturbance - the procedure remained ambiguous as did the disturbance. <strong>The</strong><br />

authors typically extrapolated their conclusions to psychiatric patients but as research this is meaningless,<br />

and such studies were excluded.<br />

94 But not drawing tests as p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> a battery for a purpose which did not depend on the <strong>art</strong> score, for<br />

example IQ, ability. Pain drawings and maps and some drawings, e.g. cubes, were excluded as the task<br />

47


etrieved from the Psychlit, which produced many more hits. <strong>The</strong> first two sources were<br />

clearly inferior for this type <strong>of</strong> material which was well within the Psychlit domain and<br />

not a borderline subject.<br />

Because research in the field <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> is not fully represented on databases,<br />

additional unsystematic searches <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy journals, books, citations contributed to<br />

it over the six year progress <strong>of</strong> this research. Through personal correspondence over four<br />

years <strong>of</strong> the writing, I undertook to discover any major omissions and a small amount <strong>of</strong><br />

unpublished work was added. This review concentrates on published studies, but they<br />

have the advantage <strong>of</strong> accessibility and <strong>of</strong> peer review as that <strong>of</strong> uncatalogued journals and<br />

unpublished work is <strong>of</strong> uneven quality. Although, no doubt, not every paper is<br />

represented here, especially unpublished work, there is sufficient coverage <strong>of</strong> the area to<br />

make informed judgements and there is no reason to believe that overlooked papers would<br />

provide different information 95 .<br />

All <strong>art</strong> therapy journals referenced here were international, mostly American,<br />

<strong>The</strong>re is only one British journal dedicated to <strong>art</strong> therapy, Inscape 96 , which is neither<br />

catalogued nor indexed. An unsystematic search <strong>of</strong> an incomplete collection at the local<br />

nursing library found <strong>art</strong>icles which were mostly <strong>of</strong> speculative clinical interest and<br />

did not call for expression or personal input.<br />

95<br />

Studies in other languages, especially eastern Europe seem to have a greater emphasis on interdisciplinary<br />

therapy (impressions from abstracts).<br />

96 Produced by the British Association <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapists.<br />

48


current pr<strong>of</strong>essional and administrative issues. Articles generally lacked sufficient detail<br />

to contribute to this review 97 .<br />

Search Results<br />

This search resulted in 751 studies <strong>of</strong> which 428 met the inclusion criteria from the<br />

databases 98 and 24 were added from other sources 99 . To limit the data to that <strong>of</strong> direct<br />

relevance to research, these were classified into:<br />

impressionistic papers 253<br />

case studies 148<br />

controlled studies 51<br />

Complete references for all papers appear as tables <strong>of</strong> Authorities in Appendix 3.<br />

<strong>The</strong> literature was organised broadly and qualitatively as follows:<br />

1. Individualistic theories or philosophy <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> and healing presented as impressions,<br />

which were examined for their perspective on the purpose <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> in therapy (the<br />

impressionistic studies introduced a number <strong>of</strong> popular views about the nature<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> and healing. It would be unwise, in an exploratory study, to exclude such<br />

97 A search by Bloch (1988) <strong>of</strong> 20 years <strong>of</strong> Inscape <strong>art</strong>icles found only 13 research-based papers (reported<br />

by Gilroy 1992, Research in Art <strong>The</strong>rapy op.cit.) reflecting the prelevant view that "research has little<br />

impact on psychotherapy practise".<br />

98 Database abstracts were first screened for inclusion criteria for foreign papers difficult to obtain. Suitable<br />

papers were obtained from British Library resources. Only a few papers were <strong>of</strong> very limited circulation<br />

and were omitted but generally papers from the large databases were accessible.<br />

99 Only one was a controlled study.<br />

49


a major proportion <strong>of</strong> the study material without at least investigating what it had<br />

to say) 100 ;<br />

2. Studies <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>ists were separated from the case studies because, although many<br />

the same assumptions and speculative impressions appeared, they were reported<br />

differently than those <strong>of</strong> other patients;<br />

3. Case studies (including series studies) were examined for their common underlying<br />

concepts and for generalisable information on imagery or interpretation;<br />

4. Controlled studies were examined for the common underlying concepts and for<br />

the generalisability <strong>of</strong> their findings.<br />

What we need to know from the literature:<br />

Two kinds <strong>of</strong> information were required from this review <strong>of</strong> the literature:<br />

1. What kind <strong>of</strong> techniques, orientation and concepts are involved in <strong>art</strong> therapy so<br />

that we may allow for it.<br />

2. What kind <strong>of</strong> characteristic is attributed to which set <strong>of</strong> painted marks.<br />

Problems <strong>of</strong> extracting this information<br />

<strong>The</strong> traditional anti-scientific bias <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> literature, results in a heavy emphasis on the case<br />

study and otherwise poor methodology. <strong>The</strong> problem <strong>of</strong> how to classify the techniques<br />

and concepts sympathetically is complicated because there is confusion in reports <strong>of</strong><br />

100 <strong>The</strong> type <strong>of</strong> classification was broadly qualitative and thematic: originally impressionistic studies and<br />

opinions were separate from speculation and theories including psychoanalytic theory, but they were<br />

ultimately combined because distinctions were blurred and their communications indistinguishable.<br />

50


therapy between what is generally accepted as established theory and the opinions and<br />

beliefs <strong>of</strong> the author. Moreover, access to the data is impeded because it is difficult to<br />

separate what actually happens from how it is interpreted. This is important because it<br />

affects the types <strong>of</strong> questions investigated. For example, the question 'where on the<br />

surface <strong>of</strong> the paper does the patient express?' is not generally addressed. <strong>The</strong> main<br />

assumption, that it is not on the surface <strong>of</strong> the paper, but from the interaction <strong>of</strong> the<br />

client's associations with the work, shapes the presentation <strong>of</strong> the report and the main<br />

direction <strong>of</strong> research towards content analysis. Of course, therapy encourages projection<br />

<strong>of</strong> emotions onto transitional objects 101 , and this may include imagery in <strong>art</strong>work, but as<br />

research it is <strong>of</strong>ten difficult to form any conclusions about what is reported. Reports<br />

<strong>of</strong>ten neglect much practical detail on what actually happened on the paper to generate<br />

the interpretation because they are not concerned with point 2 <strong>of</strong> 'what we need to know'<br />

- above, but represent the main concerns <strong>of</strong> the literature with point 1, so this section<br />

concentrates on the first p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> what we need to know, the concepts and orientation.<br />

Why consider impressionistic reports?<br />

If the search were limited to properly controlled scientific criteria, very few studies<br />

would qualify. <strong>The</strong> subsequent picture would therefore be unrepresentative <strong>of</strong> the bulk<br />

<strong>of</strong> literature. To neglect these voices is to neglect some serious, if not structured or<br />

proven conclusions from this collected experience about the way <strong>art</strong> is thought to reflect<br />

101<br />

from psychoanalytic theory: an object or person viewed as a psychological bridge, allowing a person<br />

to make a transition from primary narcissism to a mature emotional attachment to others (adapted from<br />

A.S. Reber (1985), A Dictionary <strong>of</strong> Psychology , Harmondsorth: Penguin).<br />

51


<strong>psychopathology</strong>, something which is not at all clear from the controlled studies. <strong>The</strong><br />

hypotheses and direction <strong>of</strong> the controlled studies are <strong>of</strong>ten takedn from this collected<br />

experience and there are many assumptions derived from clinical experience which are not<br />

at all obvious, especially in the interpretation <strong>of</strong> results.<br />

Impressionistic and theoretical studies<br />

Description <strong>of</strong> the range <strong>of</strong> studies included in this breakdown<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were 253 general impressionistic papers or papers which reported theories or<br />

personal experiences to show the therapeutic effects or benefits <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>. <strong>The</strong>se papers<br />

differed from research studies in that they focused on the explanatory, speculative and<br />

theoretical not the p<strong>art</strong>icular. <strong>The</strong>y discussed typical work <strong>of</strong> patients and tried to<br />

explain their conclusions or argued different theoretical standpoints. <strong>The</strong> purpose <strong>of</strong> this<br />

breakdown and analysis was tw<strong>of</strong>old: primarily, to identify the way therapists<br />

considered that <strong>art</strong> produced by a patient contributed to beneficial change in the patient;<br />

secondarily, to identify the theoretical basis <strong>of</strong> explanations so that the underlying<br />

concepts may be explored and to test the relationship <strong>of</strong> explanation or theory to<br />

p<strong>art</strong>icular benefits gained.<br />

Characteristics <strong>of</strong> each paper were summarised. <strong>The</strong>re were 5 categories <strong>of</strong><br />

information common to most papers 102 and the discussions centre around the explanation<br />

<strong>of</strong> the value <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> to therapy (complete descriptions <strong>of</strong> categories appendix 1):<br />

102 A complete description <strong>of</strong> categories appears in appendix 1.<br />

52


Descriptive information about the study, whether it gave examples <strong>of</strong><br />

patient's work, techniques, advice or discussed experiences or theories;<br />

Diagnoses <strong>of</strong> the patient group;<br />

<strong>The</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> the study: psychotherapeutic, psychoanalytic, diagnostic or<br />

assessment, environmental, social or psychosomatic;<br />

Author's personal explanation <strong>of</strong> how <strong>art</strong> promotes therapeutic<br />

change: as an illustration or signpost for the therapist, as innately<br />

curative, as a vehicle for insight for the patient, as a vehicle for transfer <strong>of</strong><br />

learning, as an environment, or for communication.<br />

Benefit to the patients: expression <strong>of</strong> feelings, communication, symptom<br />

relief or healing, developmental or social, body awareness, through<br />

relationship.<br />

<strong>The</strong> purpose <strong>of</strong> this analysis was to explore how the collected observations <strong>of</strong> these<br />

authors' experiences <strong>of</strong> the effect <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> in therapy varied with the conditions <strong>of</strong> therapy<br />

and the diagnosis; that is, how consistent the effect was.<br />

Analysis <strong>of</strong> 253 impressionistic and theoretical papers according to their theories <strong>of</strong> the<br />

value <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> in therapy. Table 1<br />

D 103<br />

I<br />

A<br />

G<br />

all<br />

studies<br />

freq %<br />

sign<br />

posts<br />

freq %<br />

self<br />

healing<br />

freq %<br />

group<br />

interact'n<br />

freq %<br />

53<br />

insight<br />

freq %<br />

transfer<br />

<strong>of</strong> skill<br />

freq %<br />

communi<br />

cation<br />

freq %<br />

1 15 6 6 6.5 1 3 1 5 2 14 1 5.6 3 21 1 2<br />

2 24 9.5 14 15 1 3 1 5 0 0 3 17 1 7 4 7<br />

no<br />

explanation<br />

freq %<br />

103 Diagnostic group:<br />

1 Schizophrenic; 2 Traumatic stress/sex abuse; 3 Substance abuse; 4 Psychotic; 5<br />

Alzheimer's/Dementia/Brain Damage; 6 Emotional disorder; 7 Depression; 8 Conduct Disorder; 9 Normal;<br />

10 Retarded; 11 Undifferentiated psychiatric patients; 12 Sex Abnormalities/Abusers.


3 13 5 5 5 1 3 2 10 0 0 2 11 0 0 3 5<br />

4 12 5 4 4 4 11 1 5 1 7 0 0 1 7 1 2<br />

5 9 4 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 2 11 1 7 6 10<br />

6 13 5 2 2 4 11 2 10 1 7 0 0 2 14 2 3<br />

7 12 5 5 5 1 3 0 0 1 7 1 5.6 0 0 4 7<br />

8 4 1.6 1 1 1 3 0 0 0 0 1 5.6 0 0 1 2<br />

9 3 1 3 3 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

10 15 6 2 2 2 6 1 5 2 14 3 17 0 0 5 8.5<br />

11 126 50 48 52 20 55.5 11 55 7 50 5 28 6 43 29 49<br />

12 7 3 2 2 1 3 1 5 0 0 0 0 0 0 3 5<br />

100% 253 92 36 20 14 18 14 59<br />

Did the effect vary with diagnostic group?<br />

Table 1 shows that there were far more papers related to undifferentiated psychiatric<br />

patients (diagnostic group 11) than to any specific diagnosis. <strong>The</strong> most common<br />

explanation <strong>of</strong> the function <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> in psychotherapy for undifferentiated patients and for<br />

most other diagnoses was that the patient's work was thought to show 'signs' which<br />

illustrated and provided access to the trauma site for the therapist. No explanation was<br />

specific to a p<strong>art</strong>icular diagnostic group but there were indications that: signpost<br />

explanations were more common for patients with traumatic stress, schizophrenia,<br />

depression and non psychiatric patients; healing explanations were more common in<br />

emotional disorders; and few papers <strong>of</strong>fered explanations <strong>of</strong> effect for brain disease, brain<br />

damage or for retardation.<br />

So, we can conclude, according to the collected experience <strong>of</strong> these authors, the<br />

function <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> in therapy was not determined by diagnosis.<br />

54


Did the effect vary with conditions <strong>of</strong> the study?<br />

Origin: <strong>The</strong> papers were divided between whose which <strong>of</strong>fered arguments or presented<br />

an opinion and those <strong>of</strong>fering an experience or reporting a programme. Similar<br />

percentages <strong>of</strong>fered no explanation and insight explanations. More opinion studies<br />

<strong>of</strong>fered signpost and healing explanations and many more communication explanations,<br />

but experience studies <strong>of</strong>fered more group interaction and transfer <strong>of</strong> skill explanations.<br />

Description <strong>of</strong> study: Only 14% <strong>of</strong> papers used case material, the majority presented<br />

either illustrative examples or gave no coherent explanation. Nearly half the papers which<br />

described techniques gave a signpost explanation consistent with their advice to generate<br />

them. Advice was more evenly distributed, but just over half gave signpost or healing<br />

explanations. More than half the general recommendation for <strong>art</strong> papers <strong>of</strong>fered either<br />

signpost or healing explanations, but most <strong>of</strong> the rest <strong>of</strong>fered no explanation at all.<br />

Table 2a: Environmental description for 253 impressionistic and theoretical papers<br />

according to the explanation <strong>of</strong> therapeutic <strong>art</strong> value.<br />

Origin <strong>of</strong> study<br />

material<br />

opinion<br />

discussion<br />

experience<br />

programme<br />

description <strong>of</strong> study<br />

case material<br />

technique<br />

advice<br />

general recommend'n<br />

theoretical base<br />

psychotherapeutic<br />

psychoanalysis<br />

environmental<br />

all<br />

studies<br />

n=253<br />

freq %<br />

94 37<br />

44 17<br />

90 36<br />

25 10<br />

35 14<br />

96 38<br />

95 37<br />

27 11<br />

77 30<br />

39 15<br />

53 21<br />

sign<br />

posts<br />

freq %<br />

40 43<br />

14 15<br />

36 39<br />

2 2<br />

11 12<br />

42 46<br />

30 33<br />

9 10<br />

35 38<br />

18 20<br />

10 11<br />

self<br />

healing<br />

freq %<br />

15 42<br />

7 19<br />

14 39<br />

0 0<br />

4 11<br />

6 17<br />

20 56<br />

6 17<br />

10 28<br />

3 8<br />

11 31<br />

55<br />

group<br />

interaction<br />

freq %<br />

4 20<br />

4 20<br />

9 45<br />

3 15<br />

4 20<br />

9 45<br />

6 30<br />

1 5<br />

6 30<br />

2 10<br />

4 20<br />

Insight<br />

Freq %<br />

5 36<br />

3 21<br />

4 29<br />

2 14<br />

2 14<br />

8 57<br />

4 29<br />

0 0<br />

6 43<br />

5 36<br />

1 7<br />

transfer<br />

<strong>of</strong> skill<br />

freq %<br />

6 33<br />

1 6<br />

7 39<br />

4 22<br />

2 11<br />

9 50<br />

6 33<br />

1 6<br />

6 33<br />

1 6<br />

6 33<br />

communi<br />

cation<br />

freq %<br />

8 57<br />

3 21<br />

3 21<br />

0 0<br />

1 7<br />

2 14<br />

11 79<br />

0 0<br />

4 29<br />

5 36<br />

1 7<br />

no<br />

explanation<br />

freq %<br />

16 27<br />

12 20<br />

17 29<br />

14 24<br />

11 19<br />

20 34<br />

18 30<br />

10 17<br />

10 17<br />

5 8<br />

20 34


social/developmental<br />

energy theories<br />

assessment<br />

19 7<br />

31 12<br />

34 13<br />

4 4<br />

9 10<br />

16 17<br />

2 6<br />

8 22<br />

2 6<br />

explanation - how it<br />

works<br />

no's in each group 92 36 36 14 20 8 14 5 18 7 14 5 59 23<br />

56<br />

2 10<br />

4 20<br />

2 10<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

2 14<br />

1 6<br />

2 11<br />

2 11<br />

1 7<br />

2 14<br />

1 7<br />

Complete descriptions <strong>of</strong> value to therapy categories appear in Appendix 1.<br />

<strong>The</strong>oretical base: <strong>The</strong> most popular theoretical base was psychotherapeutic. Half the<br />

environmental and the developmental studies <strong>of</strong>fered no explanation and most <strong>of</strong> the rest<br />

used signpost or healing explanations. Despite the theoretical orientation <strong>of</strong> the<br />

discussion or therapy, the signpost explanation was more prevalent than any other. <strong>The</strong><br />

assessment methods predictably concentrated on signposts, but a considerable percentage<br />

<strong>of</strong> psychotherapeutically orientated papers explained the function <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> as an energy or<br />

power derived through the therapist or through the patient or through communication<br />

with the unconscious. However, where the discussion centred on the mystical conduction<br />

<strong>of</strong> healing power through the therapist to the patient or where the <strong>art</strong> environment and<br />

the practice <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> itself was thought to be therapeutic in some way, there was still an<br />

equal focus on the signpost explanation.<br />

Table 2b: Commonly claimed benefits for 253 impressionistic and theortetical papers<br />

according to explanation <strong>of</strong> the therapeutic value <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong><br />

Benefits to patient 104<br />

express feelings<br />

develop skill<br />

communicate<br />

self awareness<br />

symptom relief<br />

relationship<br />

all<br />

studies<br />

freq %<br />

156 22<br />

107 15<br />

159 22<br />

125 18<br />

140 20<br />

22 3<br />

sign<br />

posts<br />

freq %<br />

72 26<br />

30 11<br />

64 23<br />

43 16<br />

53 19<br />

10 4<br />

self<br />

healing<br />

freq %<br />

19 19<br />

12 12<br />

21 21<br />

20 20<br />

24 24<br />

2 2<br />

group<br />

interaction<br />

freq %<br />

12 20<br />

12 20<br />

13 21<br />

11 18<br />

11 18<br />

2 3<br />

Insight<br />

Freq %<br />

7 17<br />

7 17<br />

8 19<br />

8 19<br />

10 24<br />

1 2<br />

transfer<br />

<strong>of</strong> skill<br />

freq %<br />

8 15<br />

8 15<br />

10 19<br />

15 29<br />

9 17<br />

2 4<br />

communi<br />

cation<br />

freq %<br />

9 21<br />

4 9<br />

14 33<br />

8 19<br />

6 14<br />

1 2<br />

9 15<br />

6 10<br />

9 15<br />

no<br />

explanation<br />

freq %<br />

29 20<br />

34 24<br />

29 20<br />

20 14<br />

27 19<br />

4 3<br />

104 <strong>The</strong>re were four measures <strong>of</strong> benefits to patients allowing 4 answers in any order. Frequency and<br />

percentage here represent totalised positive answers to reduce non-answering bias: only 5% (13) <strong>of</strong> studies<br />

did not describe one benefit for the patient, but many studies gave two or more benefits.


Benefits: <strong>The</strong> most common benefits <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> were given as expression <strong>of</strong> feelings,<br />

communication and symptom relief, with little emphasis on the relationship as a benefit<br />

in itself. <strong>The</strong> majority <strong>of</strong> studies, no matter which benefits were listed, including<br />

relationships, <strong>of</strong>fered a signpost explanation; the next most common explanation was <strong>of</strong><br />

healing.<br />

Summary<br />

<strong>The</strong> main purpose <strong>of</strong> this analysis was to clarify the function <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> in therapy and also<br />

to find out if explanations changed with different diagnostic group, whether explanations<br />

derived primarily from the opinion <strong>of</strong> the therapist or through experience changed with<br />

the theoretical base <strong>of</strong> the therapy, and whether different benefits were apparent in each<br />

explanation.<br />

No explanation stood out within p<strong>art</strong>icular diagnoses. <strong>The</strong> explanations were<br />

independent <strong>of</strong> the theoretical base <strong>of</strong> the therapy/discussion and did not vary with<br />

different benefits claimed for the patients. Where the paper dealt with a general<br />

recommendation for <strong>art</strong> or gave advice, there were more healing explanations, but on the<br />

whole, signpost explanations were more common. Papers which relied on the opinion <strong>of</strong><br />

the therapist, rather than those which related specific experience or described a<br />

programme, advanced many more communication explanations and slightly more signpost<br />

and healing explanations, focusing on content in the picture. Signpost explanations were<br />

57


advanced from all types <strong>of</strong> papers but mostly from those with a psychotherapeutically<br />

based theory and least from those with social or developmental base.<br />

<strong>The</strong> self healing explanation, which is obfuscated by the terminology in which it<br />

is swaddled, seems to derive from Jungian theory <strong>of</strong> contacting the unconscious, but sees<br />

the therapist as a conductor for a force or power <strong>of</strong> healing. This is not an explanation<br />

but a speculative model. Authors who <strong>of</strong>fered these explanations generally gave advice,<br />

came from a psychotherapeutic or an environmental base and claimed all benefits equally.<br />

Explanations other than signpost or healing were marginal and equally thinly<br />

distributed. All explanations were independent <strong>of</strong> any variable examined here. <strong>The</strong><br />

papers which described an experience or a programme dealt mainly with group interaction<br />

and transfer <strong>of</strong> skill, looking at formal measures.<br />

Conclusion<br />

This analysis has clarified the terms and general area <strong>of</strong> interest, but has not provided an<br />

explanation <strong>of</strong> the function <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> in psychotherapy, nor any clearer idea <strong>of</strong> the area <strong>of</strong><br />

operation expected for different theories. <strong>The</strong> primary benefits to patients were<br />

expression <strong>of</strong> feelings, communication and symptom relief. It is assumed that therapists<br />

have access to a means <strong>of</strong> decoding personal associative material, but consistent<br />

explanations were not provided on how this would be accomplished and practically based<br />

reports dealt with the communication value <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>. This is not unexpected in such general<br />

material, but is irrelevant to the main issue here <strong>of</strong> exactly what is decoded by the<br />

58


therapist to aid the therapy or assessment. This issue is further explored in the next<br />

section which deals with case and controlled studies.<br />

We can say then that the direction <strong>of</strong> these papers indicates there is change in the<br />

patient, which parallels a change in the <strong>art</strong>work, and that generally the explanations do<br />

not relate the change to the methods to direct the change. <strong>The</strong> therapist reports that<br />

<strong>art</strong>work produced by the patient is translated but the method <strong>of</strong> translation and how it<br />

produces change in the patient is not discussed.<br />

Artists: are they special cases?<br />

Aesthetic interpretation <strong>of</strong> psychiatric <strong>art</strong> by <strong>art</strong> historians<br />

<strong>The</strong>re have been what looks like points <strong>of</strong> convergence between aesthetics and<br />

psychology, but which are, in reality, similarities in superficial language. Some historians<br />

have adopted psychoanalytical and psychological jargon, but most <strong>of</strong> this is<br />

uninterpretable in a psychoanalytic sense. Various psychoanalytical perspectives on <strong>art</strong><br />

have been given, generally by medical people. Few historians have tackled this area,<br />

probably with good reason, as the assumption that there must be an essentially normal<br />

way <strong>of</strong> painting 105 , and that divergence would have pathological roots, seems<br />

questionable. Griselda Pollock and others 106 have tried to answer these kinds <strong>of</strong><br />

questions but most work is concentrated on trained <strong>art</strong>ists.<br />

105 Exemplified by a painting everyone understands - such as Constable's '<strong>The</strong> Haywain'.<br />

106<br />

Some examples <strong>of</strong> recent readable texts which seriously attempt to unravel or question unconscious<br />

symbolic material (but all use <strong>art</strong>istic illustrations): Griselda Pollock (1988) Ch.6, Women and sign:<br />

psychoanalytic readings in Pollock, Vision and Difference , London: Routledge pp.120-154; G. Pollock<br />

and J.M. Ross, (eds.), (1988) <strong>The</strong> Oedipus Papers , Conn: Madison; Jim Hopkins (1992), Psychoanalysis,<br />

interpretation and science in J. Hopkins and Anthony Saville, (eds.), Psychoanalysis Mind and Art:<br />

59


Art therapists begin their career from <strong>art</strong> training and this influences the ways in<br />

which they try to make sense <strong>of</strong> a picture 107 : the search for iconography, the borrowed<br />

language <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> criticism in content appraisal 108 .<br />

Very little <strong>art</strong> historical analysis looks at the broad discrete forms <strong>of</strong> the actual<br />

work; the language is only relevant and meaningful in relation to other objects. Critical<br />

descriptive language is used to describe a continuum <strong>of</strong> iconographical, contextual, social<br />

and aesthetic perspectives.<br />

<strong>The</strong> romanticism generally put forward about <strong>art</strong>ists adds to false assumptions<br />

and myths that grow up around them. For example, popular opinion sees Van Gogh as<br />

a typical example <strong>of</strong> a mad <strong>art</strong>ist, but none <strong>of</strong> his famous pictures were painted at the<br />

height <strong>of</strong> his madness. It is highly unlikely that the crow picture by Van Gogh,<br />

constantly pictured in movies as his last work before he shot himself, actually was his<br />

last. Louis Wain's pictures <strong>of</strong> cats are <strong>of</strong>ten quoted as showing the process <strong>of</strong> degeneracy<br />

- as the form dissolves, the madness progresses. But who is to say that he was not trying<br />

to portray or simplify his ideas? In the case <strong>of</strong> Richard Dadd, there was no visual<br />

perspectives on Richard Wollheim , Oxford: Blackwell; Peter Fuller (1980) Art and Psychoanalysis ,<br />

London: Writers Readers; Donald Kusbit, Signs <strong>of</strong> Psyche in Modern and Postmodern Art , Cambridge:<br />

Cam.U.P.<br />

Also perspectives on how we understand <strong>art</strong>: John M. Thorburn (1925) Is <strong>art</strong> symbolic? (pp.73-79) and<br />

Art as the relation <strong>of</strong> outer and inner (pp.151-6) in Thorburn, Art and the Unconscious: a psychoanalytical<br />

application to a problem <strong>of</strong> philosophy , London: Kegan Paul; Michael J. Parsons (1989) How we<br />

understand <strong>art</strong>: a cognitive developmental area <strong>of</strong> aesthetic experien ce , Cambridge U.P. (2nd ed. original<br />

1987) describes 5 stages <strong>of</strong> perception: favoritism, beauty, expression, style, form.<br />

107 C.F. Nodine, P.J. Locher and E.A. Krupinski (1993) <strong>The</strong> role <strong>of</strong> formal <strong>art</strong> training on perception and<br />

aesthetic judgement <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> comparisons, Leonardo V.26:219-227 proved differences in perceptions/eye<br />

movements <strong>of</strong> people with <strong>art</strong> training, and without. Main finding was concentration on thematic patterns<br />

from <strong>art</strong> trained, and focus on representational issues/accuracy, without.<br />

108 <strong>The</strong> requirement <strong>of</strong> an <strong>art</strong> degree as a primary qualification for would-be <strong>art</strong> therapists has provoked<br />

concerns that too much emphasis is placed on the aesthetic and the mistaken but pervasive concept <strong>of</strong> the<br />

creation <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> was itself therapeutic, J. Birchtnell (1981), Is Art <strong>The</strong>rapeutic? Inscape I: 10-13.<br />

60


counterp<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the thought disorder apparent in his written notebooks 109 . <strong>The</strong> well<br />

known collections <strong>of</strong> psychiatric <strong>art</strong> work which were used to illustrate <strong>psychopathology</strong><br />

were by selected exceptionally talented painters studied for research. <strong>The</strong> knowledge<br />

cannot be separated from the intention. Work by naive <strong>art</strong>ists is personal, rarely involves<br />

an audience and does not operate in a deliberate way to research and develop the work.<br />

It does not contribute to <strong>art</strong> because it does not intentionally refer to it and thus it cannot<br />

be placed on the continuum. <strong>The</strong>refore, for work by non-talented patients, <strong>art</strong> critical<br />

interpretations, which vary with the theoretical stance <strong>of</strong> the interpreter, are not<br />

appropriate.<br />

<strong>The</strong> assumption that a picture is a sort <strong>of</strong> print-out <strong>of</strong> how the world is seen and<br />

that some unconscious force paints through the <strong>art</strong>ist must be wrong. <strong>The</strong> best 'outsider'<br />

<strong>art</strong>ists, even though they might lack training and technical skill, show a search for<br />

organisation, considerable awareness <strong>of</strong> balance and awareness <strong>of</strong> the world and that<br />

knowledge is communicated through their pictures. Studies <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> and perception show<br />

that in order to construct an image <strong>of</strong> power and balance a great deal <strong>of</strong> awareness is<br />

needed 110 . Making a picture is a constructive process and a complicated one, with<br />

metaphors such as paint standing for light and 3d objects.<br />

Studies <strong>of</strong> psychiatrically disturbed <strong>art</strong>ists tended to focus on the discussion <strong>of</strong><br />

whether there was something about mental illness itself which contributed to <strong>art</strong>istic<br />

109<br />

from J.M. MacGregor (1989), <strong>The</strong> Discovery <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the insane , N.J.:Princetown U.P.<br />

110<br />

Ernst Gombrich (1977) Art and Illusion: a study in the psychology <strong>of</strong> pictorial representation , Princeton<br />

NJ: Phaidon (5th ed. original 1960); J.J. Gibson (1980) foreward in M.A. Hagen, (ed.), <strong>The</strong> Perception<br />

<strong>of</strong> Pictures V.1, New York: Ac. Press, and for an explanation that cognition must be involved in moving<br />

between images to interpret spatial relations: Shona Rogers and Alan Costall (1983), Pictorial perception<br />

and Gibson's concept <strong>of</strong> information, Leonardo , V.16(3):180-2.<br />

61


talent and therefore do not fit in with other reports. <strong>The</strong> purpose <strong>of</strong> this analysis was<br />

to examine the commonalities between 29 studies <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>ists and investigate possible<br />

generalisation. <strong>The</strong> impressionistic interpretations <strong>of</strong> pictures or studies about <strong>art</strong>ists<br />

were summarised (case summaries table 3, appendix 2); 18 were case studies and 11<br />

discussions. <strong>The</strong>re were 2 types <strong>of</strong> discussion:<br />

(1) Whether or how work was creative or aesthetic, and<br />

(2) that it illustrated mental illness:<br />

<strong>The</strong> main focus was on illustration <strong>of</strong> psychosis or depression (15 studies) and<br />

<strong>of</strong> how psychosis affected or enhanced creativity <strong>of</strong> the mentally ill. 20 studies reported<br />

that mental illness had a positive effect on the creativity or <strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the subject and all<br />

implied that the practice <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> had a 'healing effect', by which they meant that the<br />

practise <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> was seen to relieve the <strong>art</strong>ist <strong>of</strong> a mental burden. Descriptions were poor<br />

but generally some expression <strong>of</strong> emotion and some form <strong>of</strong> catharsis was reported. 6<br />

studies reported no effect and only one reported a negative effect. <strong>The</strong>y differ from the<br />

usual case study in that:<br />

no other type <strong>of</strong> medical writing used so little information; and<br />

what was used was selected, or adopted a subjective view <strong>of</strong> the product <strong>of</strong><br />

mental illness as a separate issue.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se works were assumed to deviate from a standard <strong>of</strong> normal, but, at the same time,<br />

perfectly normal modern <strong>art</strong> was also shown to possess the same attributes that were<br />

described as indications <strong>of</strong> <strong>psychopathology</strong>. All studies here concentrated on illustrated<br />

diagnostic indications <strong>of</strong> dissolution, fragmentation or disorder <strong>of</strong> thought and<br />

62


communication, but ignored the fact that these paintings apparently represented highly<br />

successful communications <strong>of</strong> content. <strong>The</strong> act <strong>of</strong> illustrating coherently and effectively<br />

in a familiar medium was not usually impaired.<br />

Conclusion<br />

Artists are hard to work with and the meagre evidence suggests <strong>art</strong> therapists do not feel<br />

comfortable with their technical skill. It is difficult to assess how many actually<br />

succeeded in using <strong>art</strong> for therapy (despite the reporting) as the analytic process rests on<br />

an unconscious use <strong>of</strong> compositional relationships, where an <strong>art</strong>ist is all too aware. <strong>The</strong><br />

evidence suggests <strong>art</strong>ists too are uncomfortable with this use <strong>of</strong> their work 111<br />

(pr<strong>of</strong>essionally, their own explanations are rarely accepted as the best interpretations).<br />

<strong>The</strong> style and aim <strong>of</strong> these studies relates to speculations on a possible positive<br />

aesthetic effect on creative output, thus is contradictory to that <strong>of</strong> studies <strong>of</strong> untrained<br />

psychiatric patients which tries to identify visual elements <strong>of</strong> individual<br />

<strong>psychopathology</strong> from the work. That <strong>art</strong> has therapeutic qualities is mentioned, but<br />

although the benefits <strong>of</strong> expression as a way <strong>of</strong> externalising conflict and communicating<br />

with the unconscious are described in both cases, there seems no supporting evidence<br />

relating concrete visual elements to remission <strong>of</strong> symptoms or better communication in<br />

other forms for the <strong>art</strong>ist.<br />

111<br />

Leslie Gertler (1985) <strong>The</strong>rapy with an aging <strong>art</strong>ist, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.23(3):93-9 (study 28),<br />

recommends methods <strong>of</strong> removing the concerns <strong>of</strong> content, as does D.C. Muenchow, J. Aresenian (1974)<br />

An <strong>art</strong>ist in turmoil during <strong>art</strong> therapy, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.14(1):18-23 (study 29); <strong>The</strong> author <strong>of</strong> study<br />

9, too, advises readers that the work <strong>of</strong>ten goes beyond the interpretation <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong>ist.<br />

63


2. Research studies<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were two kinds <strong>of</strong> studies which <strong>of</strong>fered evidence to support their authors'<br />

conclusions:<br />

1. 119 case or series studies presented examples <strong>of</strong> work and interpretations or<br />

extrapolations <strong>of</strong> this evidence. This kind <strong>of</strong> study was oriented towards<br />

exploring and developing the concepts, techniques and orientation <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy.<br />

2. 51 studies <strong>of</strong> groups systematically compared <strong>art</strong>work with that <strong>of</strong> another group.<br />

This kind <strong>of</strong> study attempted to define characteristics common to specific<br />

groups <strong>of</strong> patients.<br />

Section 1. Comparison method for case studies<br />

Much <strong>of</strong> the information presented in these studies could not be classified because <strong>of</strong> the<br />

personal way it was presented: studies presented a section <strong>of</strong> a personal history,<br />

individual to the client; the information given was necessarily selected by the author to<br />

contribute to their interpretation and thus was not complete. Data was referred to that<br />

was not presented (other pictures or feelings or comments) and the extent <strong>of</strong> material not<br />

presented could not be estimated; thus the interpretation by the author was personal,<br />

subjective and not generalisable. Statistical analysis <strong>of</strong> such description would be <strong>art</strong>ifice.<br />

Organisation <strong>of</strong> this section<br />

64


<strong>The</strong> purpose <strong>of</strong> this analysis is to explain how <strong>art</strong> concepts and techniques benefitted the<br />

individual and whether studies identified diagnostic characteristics from psychiatric<br />

paintings. <strong>The</strong>re were 4 purposes <strong>of</strong> making <strong>art</strong> defended in this literature:<br />

1. 67 studies presented illustrations <strong>of</strong> the image drawn by the client and concluded<br />

that the primary benefit in <strong>art</strong>making was the expression <strong>of</strong> feelings.<br />

2. 31 studies presented illustrations <strong>of</strong> themes or signs in the <strong>art</strong>work <strong>of</strong> the client<br />

and identified typical diagnostic characteristics.<br />

3. 17 studies presented illustrations <strong>of</strong> the image drawn by the client and concluded<br />

that the primary benefit in <strong>art</strong> therapy was the therapeutic relationship, within<br />

which 8 concluded that the primary benefit in <strong>art</strong>making was to occupy the<br />

patient in engaging activity within a supportive environment.<br />

4. 4 studies identified a change in the client's behaviour whilst undertaking <strong>art</strong><br />

therapy.<br />

Descriptive information is presented and discussed through 5 categories <strong>of</strong> information<br />

common to all studies 112 . <strong>The</strong> discussions centre around the primary purposes <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>-<br />

making: the therapy value to the patient, and which variables are independent:<br />

Descriptive information about the subjects: age (adolescent, adult, old),<br />

sex, diagnosis (1-15 see table 1, note 2, for details);<br />

Orientation or intentions <strong>of</strong> the researcher: not known, cognitive,<br />

projective, occupational, analytical;<br />

112 A complete description <strong>of</strong> all information categories appears in Appendix 1 <strong>of</strong> this thesis.<br />

65


Method <strong>of</strong> study or description <strong>of</strong> technique: projective, psychoanalytic,<br />

expressive, occupational, or comparative;<br />

Form <strong>of</strong> study - what is described or interpreted: content, formal or<br />

stylistic elements, mix <strong>of</strong> content/form, behaviour or verbal;<br />

Benefit to the patients: cath<strong>art</strong>ic/reflective, communication,<br />

healing/symptom relief, developmental or social, relationship.<br />

Accordingly information is presented by levels <strong>of</strong> benefit identified, which can be<br />

considered as results for the purposes <strong>of</strong> this analysis.<br />

Form and content<br />

This division is made to differentiate analysis <strong>of</strong> form, used here to distinguish<br />

descriptions <strong>of</strong> the local qualities <strong>of</strong> elements and regional qualities <strong>of</strong> complexes within<br />

a visual design from descriptions <strong>of</strong> internal relations among the elements and among the<br />

complexes within the object; i.e. what the painting represents to the individual, how that<br />

representation is associated with other phenomena in the mind <strong>of</strong> the patient; in other<br />

words, the meaning, here described as content analysis. This discrimination has not been<br />

successfully made in the literature but it is crucial to determining the subjectivity <strong>of</strong> the<br />

analysis.<br />

Expression <strong>of</strong> feelings. Description <strong>of</strong> the range <strong>of</strong> explanations within this term.<br />

67 studies concluded that the primary benefits <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>making for the patient was in "the<br />

expression <strong>of</strong> feelings". <strong>The</strong> term was ambiguous: it was used by the majority <strong>of</strong> papers<br />

66


and indeed throughout the literature without further explanation to identify a goal for<br />

therapy and also as a benefit in itself. Few papers questioned whether feelings were<br />

expressed or what expression was or how they identified that it had occurred; the<br />

identification was always global and subjective. Thus these studies were separate from<br />

those which <strong>of</strong>fered specific interpretations for expressive content, which appear later<br />

in this thesis. <strong>The</strong> interpretation <strong>of</strong> expression <strong>of</strong> feeling was <strong>of</strong>ten not p<strong>art</strong>icularly<br />

related to the drawing medium, but rather to the personal experience <strong>of</strong> the<br />

therapist/researcher. <strong>The</strong>se studies argued the importance <strong>of</strong> psychotherapeutic method;<br />

they illustrated their papers with drawings by the patient, but were vague as to specific<br />

exemplars; they confirmed personal theories with reference to the illustrations and<br />

emphasis on the relationship with the therapist. <strong>The</strong>y especially referred to the<br />

idiosyncrasies <strong>of</strong> the individual unconscious mind <strong>of</strong> the patient. <strong>The</strong> purpose <strong>of</strong> this<br />

analysis was to identify how patients are said to express feelings through <strong>art</strong> and in what<br />

way their <strong>art</strong> output contributes to the benefits which are said to derive from that<br />

expression.<br />

Who benefits?<br />

Demographics: <strong>The</strong> total number <strong>of</strong> subjects over the 67 studies was 180: 28 studies<br />

dealt with males, 26 with females and 13 with mixed groups. One study used 42<br />

subjects, but 67% <strong>of</strong> studies (n=45) were single cases, the other 21 had up to 10 subjects.<br />

Age ranged from 8 to 90 years, but only 37% (n=21) studies dealt with under 16 year<br />

67


olds and only 2 with over 60s. <strong>The</strong> typical subject was therefore a case study <strong>of</strong> an adult<br />

with affective disorder.<br />

Table 1 showing diagnostic groups for case studies by levels <strong>of</strong> benefits where the<br />

primary purpose <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>making was the expression <strong>of</strong> feelings<br />

D 113<br />

I<br />

A<br />

G<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

5<br />

6<br />

7<br />

10<br />

All studies<br />

n=67<br />

freq %<br />

9 13.4<br />

13 19.4<br />

4 6<br />

9 13.4<br />

5 7.5<br />

13 19.4<br />

11 16.4<br />

3 4.5<br />

no benefit<br />

n=8<br />

freq %<br />

2 25<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

2 25<br />

2 25<br />

1 12.5<br />

1 12.5<br />

0 0<br />

catharsis<br />

n=16<br />

freq %<br />

2 12.5<br />

5 31.3<br />

2 12.5<br />

1 6.3<br />

1 6.3<br />

2 12.5<br />

2 12.5<br />

1 6.3<br />

68<br />

communi<br />

-cation<br />

n=20<br />

freq %<br />

1 5<br />

5 25<br />

0 0<br />

3 15<br />

1 5<br />

6 30<br />

3 15<br />

1 5<br />

healing<br />

n=10<br />

freq %<br />

1 10<br />

1 10<br />

1 10<br />

2 20<br />

0 0<br />

2 20<br />

3 30<br />

0 0<br />

social/<br />

developm't<br />

n=8<br />

freq %<br />

1 12.5<br />

2 25<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

1 12.5<br />

1 12.5<br />

2 25<br />

1 12.5<br />

relation<br />

ship<br />

n=5<br />

freq %<br />

1 20<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

1 20<br />

0 0<br />

1 20<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was a broad range <strong>of</strong> disorders treated with no p<strong>art</strong>icular bias to any diagnostic<br />

group. Cath<strong>art</strong>ic benefits were p<strong>art</strong>icularly apparent in people with emotional trauma<br />

and depression, who also showed higher communicative benefits but the other<br />

beneficiaries showed more range <strong>of</strong> distribution.<br />

Results: From Table 2, overleaf, the majority <strong>of</strong> the studies (54%, n=36) recorded the<br />

primary benefits as communicative and cath<strong>art</strong>ic-reflective. <strong>The</strong>se were achieved by<br />

researchers who were oriented towards projective techniques, who used these techniques<br />

to facilitate the expression <strong>of</strong> emotion (for communication there was an equal use <strong>of</strong><br />

educational methods) and analysed the content <strong>of</strong> the finished work. Secondly came<br />

113<br />

1 Schizophrenia<br />

2 Emotional trauma<br />

3 Drug/Alcohol addiction<br />

4 Psychotics or phobias<br />

5 Brain disease/damage<br />

6 Adjustment disorder/emotional reaction<br />

7 Affective disorder/depression<br />

10 Retarded


healing or symptom relief which was achieved through the same combination although<br />

there was more emphasis on expression in behaviour in the analysis, but not as an<br />

intended method. Thirdly, developmental-social benefits were also claimed for<br />

educational methods and the therapeutic relationship for projective methodology, but<br />

descriptions <strong>of</strong> expressive form were inconsistent with this aim, they were almost<br />

exclusively behavioral for relationship and mostly for social.<br />

Descriptive statistics for whole study, interactive variables. Table 2, showing<br />

consistency <strong>of</strong> the research by levels <strong>of</strong> benefit from expression <strong>of</strong> feelings<br />

LEVELS OF<br />

BENEFIT -><br />

<strong>Study</strong> variables<br />

Sex<br />

male<br />

female<br />

mixed<br />

Orientation<br />

not known<br />

cognitive<br />

projective<br />

social/occupational<br />

analytical<br />

Method <strong>of</strong> study<br />

illustrative<br />

proj./expressive<br />

behaviour<br />

psychoanalytic<br />

educational/exp.ce<br />

Form <strong>of</strong> study<br />

formal/style<br />

content analysis<br />

mix content/style<br />

behaviour<br />

verbal analysis<br />

other<br />

All<br />

Studies<br />

n=67<br />

freq %<br />

28 41.8<br />

26 38.8<br />

13 19.4<br />

3 4.5<br />

6 9<br />

38 56.7<br />

9 13.4<br />

11 16.4<br />

4 6.0<br />

39 58.2<br />

4 6.0<br />

7 10.4<br />

13 19.4<br />

1 1.5<br />

33 49.3<br />

5 7.5<br />

17 25.4<br />

9 13.4<br />

2 3.0<br />

No<br />

benefit<br />

n=8<br />

freq %<br />

2 25<br />

4 50<br />

2 25<br />

1 12.5<br />

0 0<br />

2 25<br />

2 25<br />

3 37.5<br />

1 12.5<br />

5 62.5<br />

1 12.5<br />

1 12.5<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

1 12.5<br />

0 0<br />

4 50<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

Cath<strong>art</strong>ic<br />

benefit<br />

n=16<br />

freq %<br />

10 62.5<br />

4 25<br />

2 12.5<br />

1 6.3<br />

2 12.5<br />

12 75<br />

0 0<br />

1 6.3<br />

1 6.3<br />

9 56.3<br />

3 18.8<br />

2 12.5<br />

1 6.3<br />

0 0<br />

9 56.3<br />

1 6.3<br />

4 25<br />

2 12.5<br />

0 0<br />

69<br />

comm'n<br />

benefit<br />

n=20<br />

freq %<br />

9 45<br />

8 40<br />

3 15<br />

1 5<br />

3 15<br />

12 60<br />

1 5<br />

3 15<br />

2 10<br />

11 55<br />

0 0<br />

1 5<br />

6 30<br />

1 5<br />

8 40<br />

3 15<br />

4 20<br />

3 15<br />

1 5<br />

healin<br />

g<br />

n=10<br />

freq %<br />

3 30<br />

5 50<br />

2 20<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

6 60<br />

3 30<br />

1 10<br />

0 0<br />

7 70<br />

0 0<br />

2 20<br />

1 10<br />

0 0<br />

4 40<br />

1 10<br />

3 30<br />

2 20<br />

0 0<br />

develo<br />

pment/<br />

social<br />

n=8<br />

freq %<br />

0 0<br />

5 62.5<br />

3 37.5<br />

0 0<br />

1 12.5<br />

3 37.5<br />

3 37.5<br />

1 12.5<br />

0 0<br />

3 37.5<br />

0 0<br />

1 12.5<br />

4 50<br />

0 0<br />

2 25<br />

0 0<br />

3 37.5<br />

2 25<br />

1 12.5<br />

relation<br />

ship<br />

n=5<br />

freq %<br />

3 60<br />

1 20<br />

1 20<br />

0 0<br />

3 60<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

2 40<br />

0 0<br />

4 80<br />

0 0<br />

1 20<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

1 20<br />

0 0<br />

4 80<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

Primary benefit 8 12 16 24 20 30 10 15 8 12 5 7.5<br />

<strong>The</strong> main purpose <strong>of</strong> this analysis was (a) to determine the concepts and techniques <strong>of</strong><br />

studies which claim to produce expression <strong>of</strong> feelings, and (b) to describe the pictorial


form <strong>of</strong> this expression and how it was therapeutic. <strong>The</strong> form <strong>of</strong> expression was<br />

predominantly through content <strong>of</strong> the picture and the primary benefits were cath<strong>art</strong>ic<br />

release <strong>of</strong> emotion and communication. <strong>The</strong> overriding orientation <strong>of</strong> the research was<br />

towards content analysis through projective methodology. However, there were<br />

indications that other methods seemed to produce similar benefits.<br />

<strong>The</strong> table does not indicate whether the orientation <strong>of</strong> the research was consistent<br />

in method and results. Accordingly contingency tables were produced 114 , from which<br />

Chi-square results (summarised below in table 3) showed no correlations between the<br />

description, the method <strong>of</strong> study used, the form <strong>of</strong> expression or the benefits claimed.<br />

Table 3: Chi square results for association between method <strong>of</strong> study, form <strong>of</strong> expression<br />

and benefits for 67 studies which claimed 'expression <strong>of</strong> feelings' as the main benefit for<br />

the use <strong>of</strong> Art with psychiatric patients.<br />

Orientation <strong>of</strong> researchers _ 2 = 26.29640<br />

df = 20: p


stringent cut<strong>of</strong>f point <strong>of</strong> p


Content: 16 studies reported changes or signs in the themes, the subject matter or what<br />

was represented. 43 patients (22 males and 21 females) were described 115 . 11 were<br />

single case studies and the diagnostic groups were fairly evenly distributed. <strong>The</strong> studies<br />

interpreted common themes in drawings by the same patient. <strong>The</strong>y focus on two types<br />

<strong>of</strong> information:<br />

(1) the therapeutic effect <strong>of</strong> the treatment, and<br />

(2) description <strong>of</strong> the characteristics <strong>of</strong> the subject's pictures and associations with the<br />

diagnosis.<br />

Table 4 (Appendix 2) summarises demographic, diagnostic, pictorial and interpreted<br />

information.<br />

How Art Promotes <strong>The</strong>rapy: <strong>The</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapist decodes and sometimes co-author a<br />

private language, the complexity <strong>of</strong> which may provide a way <strong>of</strong> avoiding direct<br />

confrontation with an emotive issue or at least distance. <strong>The</strong> interpretation thus also<br />

becomes a product <strong>of</strong> both the therapist and the patient. <strong>The</strong> <strong>art</strong> begins as a device for<br />

indirect communication until therapy ceases to be a non-verbal event. <strong>The</strong> therapeutic<br />

value may be similar to desensitisation therapy in that it becomes easier to face the<br />

underlying issue which is producing the psychological damage by repeated exposure to<br />

analogues <strong>of</strong> it.<br />

Are there consistently meaningful systematic signs in content <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong> work?<br />

115 2 studies (15 and 16) described paintings over many years in a variety <strong>of</strong> settings.<br />

72


<strong>The</strong>re were no specific visual constructs to which a p<strong>art</strong>icular meaning could be<br />

assigned and in fact observable representations were not generally discussed. <strong>The</strong>se<br />

studies mostly described common patterns 'read' by the therapist 116 : a struggle<br />

progressing through the guidance <strong>of</strong> the therapist which generally related to inner<br />

resolution <strong>of</strong> the immediate situation <strong>of</strong> the patient, especially to body image. Symbols<br />

were interpreted in 11 out <strong>of</strong> 16 studies: for example, the metaphor <strong>of</strong> broken land was<br />

said to correspond to body image from a male with brain injury 117 ; the mouth as a symbol<br />

for transition through childhood from an autistic adolescent 118 ; and in psychotics, a<br />

struggle for separation 119 and egocentrism 120 ; ambivalence by a foster child as a reaction<br />

to his life changes 121 ; metaphors <strong>of</strong> loss from an alzheimer's patient who died <strong>of</strong><br />

cancer 122 ; symbols <strong>of</strong> realistic hopes in depression 123 and less recognisable imagery from<br />

aphasics 124 .<br />

116<br />

Shown graphically by B.M. Cohen and Carol, T. Cox (1989) (Breaking the code: Identification <strong>of</strong><br />

multiplicity through <strong>art</strong> productions, Dis. Progress in the Dissociative Disorders , V.2(3):132-7), (Table<br />

4, study 16) who identify 10 categories described as thematic, structural and process (but all fit the<br />

content/thematic category in this review) which relate directly to the language and techniques <strong>of</strong><br />

psychotherapy.<br />

117 Table 4, study 8.<br />

118 Table 4, study 4.<br />

119 Table 4, study 10.<br />

120 Table 4, study 9.<br />

121 Table 4, study 1.<br />

122 Table 4, study 11.<br />

123 Table 4, study 15.<br />

124 Table 4, study 14.<br />

73


Pictures by bipolar depressives show gloomy colours and themes (minus<br />

phenomena) in the depressive phase and (plus properties) bright colours in the manic<br />

phase 125 , which tends to support the illustration-<strong>of</strong>-the-illness hypothesis assumed by<br />

many <strong>of</strong> the authors, but it will be seen in the report from observable characteristics<br />

(next) that pictures by unipolar depressives and schizophrenics <strong>of</strong>ten show these same<br />

qualities 126 or, equally, bright colours and happy themes 127 .<br />

Where studies described the outcome <strong>of</strong> therapy, they tried to show that changes<br />

in the theme <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong>work parallelled improvement in the patient 128 . Additionally, the<br />

element <strong>of</strong> hostility, discussed in 8 studies (indicated on table 4 by *), probably<br />

represents denial, the initial opposition to the therapeutic alliance, which provides a<br />

supportive structure for confrontation. <strong>The</strong>se conclusions suggests that thematic<br />

interpretation <strong>of</strong> pictures relate to therapy rather than the illness.<br />

Conclusion for content studies<br />

<strong>The</strong>re is no agreement as to how the meaning <strong>of</strong> what is represented is extracted, or<br />

consistency <strong>of</strong> application to further examples, although Cohen and Cox (1989) have<br />

made a promising st<strong>art</strong>. Categorisation <strong>of</strong> signs can only be attempted when terms and<br />

methods are systematic or consistent, but terminology, methods and theoretical base vary<br />

in all studies considered here. Much background knowledge is also assumed on the p<strong>art</strong><br />

125 Table 4, studies 6 and 12.<br />

126 Table 4, study 5; Table 6 studies 20, 23, 24.<br />

127 Table 4, study 15.<br />

128 Table 4, studies 3, 5, 8, 9.<br />

74


<strong>of</strong> the reader. <strong>The</strong> opinion <strong>of</strong> the interpreter is inextricably linked with what was actually<br />

observed and what is known about the patient 129 . Largely, the therapist commented upon<br />

the meaning <strong>of</strong> the picture and its internal relations based on what the patient said,<br />

knowledge <strong>of</strong> the patient and observations on significant relationships between figures,<br />

feelings, colours and details. Thus content is not divisible from style. <strong>The</strong> difficulty <strong>of</strong><br />

interpretation is illustrated by the description <strong>of</strong> pictures produced by a Native American<br />

psychiatric patient, whose compositions were thought <strong>of</strong> as impoverished and<br />

incongruent with instructions until her background was considered 130 . A few studies<br />

mentioned but did not analyse equally diverse specific identifiable formal signs or changes<br />

in the paintings attributed to therapy, which are included in the following discussion <strong>of</strong><br />

form where appropriate.<br />

<strong>The</strong> most common uses or benefits suggested for <strong>art</strong> therapy were as a monitor<br />

<strong>of</strong> progress and as a therapeutic communication. <strong>The</strong>se suggestions are only specifically<br />

valid for <strong>art</strong> if there is some meaningful way to access the images. <strong>The</strong> interpretation<br />

varied with the relationship between therapist and patient, orientation <strong>of</strong> the therapist,<br />

and the emotional involvement <strong>of</strong> both, which is not to say that it is not meaningful;<br />

certainly it has meaning and effects as related to the verbal interaction with the patient,<br />

just that no evidence justifies its relation to the <strong>art</strong>.<br />

Observable characteristics in the <strong>art</strong>work <strong>of</strong> patients (form)<br />

129 Problems for the therapist in personal involvement are discussed later under relationships.<br />

130 Table 4, study 2.<br />

75


This section does not deal strictly with form as there were overlaps between what was<br />

observed and what was recognised in depicted material, that were impossible to separate.<br />

This group <strong>of</strong> studies, however, was qualitatively distinct from those categorised as<br />

content analysis because the findings were primarily to do with observable changes in the<br />

<strong>art</strong>work <strong>of</strong> the patient. Art therapy reports commonly focused upon the content <strong>of</strong> the<br />

<strong>art</strong>work to describe individual variation in the <strong>art</strong>work <strong>of</strong> the patients rather than<br />

collective. <strong>The</strong>ir style <strong>of</strong> reporting <strong>of</strong>ten neglected the objective findings to concentrate<br />

on the analysis <strong>of</strong> meaning. <strong>The</strong> categorisation here thus may actually contradict the<br />

theoretical orientation expressed by the authors <strong>of</strong> some <strong>of</strong> these papers. 3 papers using<br />

mixed groups <strong>of</strong> 'psychiatric patients' accounted for 65% <strong>of</strong> the total cases and their<br />

general findings are described first.<br />

General signs <strong>of</strong> psychiatric disturbance<br />

General signs were summarised seperately (Table 5, appendix 2). <strong>The</strong>re were three<br />

general indications <strong>of</strong> psychiatric status:<br />

(1) distortion <strong>of</strong> figures;<br />

(2) odd placing <strong>of</strong> drawing elements;<br />

(3) circling behaviour.<br />

Specific diagnostic signs<br />

Table 6 (appendix 2) summarised studies which reported specific diagnostic signs in the<br />

analysis <strong>of</strong> formal <strong>art</strong>work.<br />

76


Analysis<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were many overlaps and contradictions among studies that reported a majority <strong>of</strong><br />

formal signs in descriptions <strong>of</strong> patient <strong>art</strong>. Actual events were <strong>of</strong>ten obscured by the<br />

reporting style <strong>of</strong> emotive description, techniques and the general imprecision <strong>of</strong> clinical<br />

observation; only suggestions for further investigation can be made. As previously noted<br />

in the content section, the characteristics <strong>of</strong> depression are p<strong>art</strong>icularly unclear 131 ; the<br />

most confusing reports are <strong>of</strong> parallel illustrative affect (e.g. random uncontrolled lines<br />

and colours, lack <strong>of</strong> structure or organisation and focus, expressive <strong>of</strong> disorientation,<br />

turbulence and lack <strong>of</strong> connectedness 132 ) also noted in the depressive phase <strong>of</strong> bipolar<br />

disorder 133 , characterised by disturbed content and missing detail as mutilated figures,<br />

more primitive drawings, while two studies report monochromatic rigid well defined still<br />

life 134 , sombre dull colours which parallel the depressive state 135 , also noted in bipolar<br />

disorder 136 , changing to bright colours on recovery. However, 2 studies reported bright<br />

colours and happy themes characterising <strong>art</strong> therapy in depression as a state <strong>of</strong> hope 137 .<br />

This conflict <strong>of</strong> opinion perhaps explains the non-significant findings reported from a<br />

131 Table 6, study 25.<br />

132 Table 6, study 23.<br />

133 Table 4, study 6.<br />

134 Table 6, study 20.<br />

135 Table 6, studies 19 and 24.<br />

136 Table 4, study 12, together with less creative activity, less or missing detail, less action.<br />

137 Table 4, studies 7 and 15 mainly content scale.<br />

77


correlation <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> elements with depression or anxiety tests on 100 patients with<br />

alexithymia who were asked to draw their illness 138 .<br />

Organic disorders were characterised by distortion <strong>of</strong> form 139 (which was also a<br />

general sign), perseveration, simplification & proportion errors, disconnections, limited<br />

colour & difficulty comprehending directions 140 . All <strong>of</strong> these were also apparent in<br />

borderline personality disorder, depression, brain injury, aphasia, manic depression and<br />

dementia 141 .<br />

Degeneration <strong>of</strong> the image appeared as a product <strong>of</strong> declining intellectual<br />

function 142 , but regression to childlike forms was present in dementia, manic depression,<br />

schizophrenia and personality disorder 143 . Fragmentation in personality disorder 144 may<br />

easily be mistaken for the description <strong>of</strong> uncontrolled lines and lack <strong>of</strong> connectedness in<br />

depression, aphasia or disconnections in organic disorders and schizophrenia 145 . Lack <strong>of</strong><br />

structure appears in reports <strong>of</strong> depression, schizophrenia, aphasia 146 and is difficult to<br />

differentiate from disorganisation, which was identified as a general sign (Table 5,<br />

138 Table 6, study 25.<br />

139 Table 5, general signs, studies 30 and 31.<br />

140 Table 6, study 27.<br />

141 Table 6: Borderline personality disorder, study 19; depression, study 20 and 23; brain injury, study 17;<br />

dementia, study 18; Table 4: aphasia, study 14; manic depression, study 6.<br />

142 Table 6, studies 17, 18 and 8.<br />

143<br />

Table 6, childlike forms, in: dementia, study 18; schizophrenia, study 26 personality disorder, study<br />

19; Table 4: manic depression, study 6.<br />

144 Table 6, study 19<br />

145 Table 6, lack <strong>of</strong> connectedness in: depression, studies 20 and 23; organic disorder, study 27;<br />

schizophrenia, study 26; Table 4: aphasia, study 14.<br />

146 Table 6, lack <strong>of</strong> structure in: depression, studies 20 and 23; schizophrenia, studies 26 and 27; Table 4:<br />

78


appendix 2), present in the above groups, in personality disorder and organic disorders 147 .<br />

Rigidity was described as characteristic <strong>of</strong> both schizophrenia and depression 148 .<br />

<strong>The</strong>re is a suggestion that visual learning may occur unconsciously 149 and that<br />

anorexics may communicate more effectively nonverbally as they produce more and<br />

better quality visual work 150 . Indeed, this result relates to that <strong>of</strong> 500 alcoholics, who<br />

also produced more drawings <strong>of</strong> a person <strong>of</strong> better quality, that is, finer and improved<br />

detail, under treatment but with the qualification that previous measures may have been<br />

taken under the effects <strong>of</strong> drugs or starvation 151 . It is commonly thought such signs may<br />

be affected by drugs 152 , or concealed and distorted when the patient is under<br />

chemotherapy 153 .<br />

Summary <strong>of</strong> form<br />

It is clear that the methods used to analyse style or form <strong>of</strong> pictures between individuals<br />

or diagnostic groups were dependent on the subjective associations <strong>of</strong> the therapist or<br />

were inconsistent by type. For example, although manic depressives were judged<br />

depressed from the content <strong>of</strong> their paintings, they were judged manic on characteristics<br />

aphasia, study 14.<br />

147 Table 6, studies 19 and 27.<br />

148 Table 6, rigidity as a characteristic <strong>of</strong>: schizophrenia, study 26; depression, study 20.<br />

149 Table 6, study 22.<br />

150 Table 6, study 21.<br />

151 Table 6, study 28.<br />

152<br />

H. Wadeson (1980), Art Psychotherapy , New York: Wiley.<br />

153<br />

Manny Sternlicht, Pincus Rosenfeld, Louis Siegel (1973), Retesting with graphic production: resolution<br />

<strong>of</strong> a diagnostic dilemma, Art Psychotherapy , V.1(3-4):299-300.<br />

79


<strong>of</strong> form 154 . <strong>The</strong> formal elements were inseparable from the content issues where they<br />

were described emotively; for example, sombre colour, and by negative characteristics,<br />

which tended to favour content issues and overlap between groups. It could be argued<br />

that in many cases the intentions <strong>of</strong> the formal analysis were not so rigid, but if evidence<br />

which is said to be objective is presented to support the subjective conclusions <strong>of</strong> the<br />

therapist or researcher, then it is necessary that it be differentiated from those opinions.<br />

<strong>The</strong> formal analysis reflects the confusion <strong>of</strong> the content analysis, in that each study is<br />

individualistic, presenting different information for each subject and there are few<br />

associations within types <strong>of</strong> patient and little systematic description <strong>of</strong> the output <strong>of</strong> the<br />

patient.<br />

Conclusion for formal and content analysis<br />

Three points arise from this discussion <strong>of</strong> case and series studies:<br />

(1) Increased output may be associated with withdrawal <strong>of</strong> drugs and different mood<br />

states as much as with therapy; and<br />

(2) If the immediate situation <strong>of</strong> the patient is, as suggested by the content analysis, the<br />

overriding element in the pictures, and these associations are too subjective and individual<br />

to be consistent within patients, then holding the content constant should emphasise the<br />

characteristics <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>istic style. This would help to test whether the apparent diversity<br />

<strong>of</strong> pictorial characterises in depression was due to environmental circumstances or some<br />

other factor;<br />

154 Table 4, studies 6 and 12.<br />

80


(3) Formal characteristics may be easier to standardise, systemise and rate than content<br />

because they do not depend on subjective opinion.<br />

I suggest that formal characteristics are rated on positive scales, without reference to<br />

content and with firm discriminations between terms so that they do not overlap with<br />

other terms.<br />

<strong>The</strong> <strong>The</strong>rapeutic relationship<br />

Table 7 (appendix 2) summarises papers which concluded that the therapeutic or<br />

supportive relationship was the most important feature <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy. <strong>The</strong>re were three<br />

types <strong>of</strong> relationship presented by 18 studies:<br />

1. Nurturing relationships;<br />

2. Communicative relationship;<br />

3. Relationships which were environmentally supportive.<br />

Nurturing relationships<br />

7 studies (4 males and 3 females with largely different diagnoses and aged 15-30) dealt<br />

with personal and individual guidance <strong>of</strong> subjects towards a resolution <strong>of</strong> their situation.<br />

<strong>The</strong> therapists claimed to recognise structural or developmental <strong>psychopathology</strong> in the<br />

<strong>art</strong>work <strong>of</strong> the patient and that psychodynamic exploration <strong>of</strong> these images gave them<br />

access or understanding <strong>of</strong> their patients' world. <strong>The</strong>y claimed to undertake role play<br />

which allowed the client to transfer their undesirable emotions onto them and thus<br />

through guidance resolve it, or by directed drawing or mirroring to help the client<br />

81


ecognise and work through the situation visually. <strong>The</strong> nurturing relationships showed<br />

two different therapy styles:<br />

(1) Drawing or 'painting together' was used for three cases by four therapists who saw<br />

themselves as operating from within the patient, they saw aspects <strong>of</strong> the patient resisting<br />

or using defenses and who must be made receptive. <strong>The</strong>y did this through a special kind<br />

<strong>of</strong> bond with the patient described as a symbiotic relatedness, merging with another<br />

person, uniting, and giving empathetic response. <strong>The</strong> therapist acted as a kind <strong>of</strong><br />

'psychic plumber', tracing blockages in the system and replaced the damaged p<strong>art</strong> with<br />

a new corrective experience, or provided... (what was) denied, reorganised and<br />

restructured leaving room for further growth. <strong>The</strong> 'inside' therapists used the <strong>art</strong> as a<br />

holding environment for the patients and saw their products as records <strong>of</strong> progress, and<br />

as maps <strong>of</strong> the system <strong>of</strong> the patient.<br />

(2) the other three therapists saw themselves operating outside the patient, their job was<br />

to encourage, to focus, to help nourish the inner self and emotional needs , to help<br />

develop insight in the patient and promote growth, they described their relationship as<br />

an alliance, as having good relations, serving as self-objects. This relationship allied the<br />

therapist with the patient in a common goal to communicate and saw patient's products<br />

as communications whose meanings were obscured.<br />

Communicative relationships<br />

Three communication relationships were all developed with young male non-verbal<br />

schizophrenics by female <strong>art</strong> therapists who claimed to use <strong>art</strong> as an outlet for the<br />

82


expression <strong>of</strong> unsocial feelings <strong>of</strong> the patient. <strong>The</strong> interaction itself seemed to be the main<br />

benefit to the patient and the <strong>art</strong> was most useful as an environment for it.<br />

Supportive relationships<br />

From 14 patients (2 young females and 12 young adult males) 11 were retarded. Various<br />

techniques were used with the aim <strong>of</strong> increasing self esteem through acquiring control over<br />

the materials and skill which, it was claimed, would then generalise an understanding <strong>of</strong><br />

internal control in other areas <strong>of</strong> the patient's life and increase self awareness. It was<br />

emphasised that <strong>art</strong> was most useful as an environment, especially with retarded people,<br />

because it provided an atmosphere <strong>of</strong> equality and respect with natural interaction<br />

Summary for case studies primarily benefitting from the therapeutic relationship<br />

Mostly, drawings were said to illustrate the present situation <strong>of</strong> the patient and thus<br />

change it in an analogy to psychotherapy. In the nurturing relationships, the therapist<br />

implemented change through other procedures than <strong>art</strong>, but used <strong>art</strong> as a containment area<br />

(or holding environment). Communicative relationships also used the <strong>art</strong> environment as<br />

a base for interactive therapy, although the therapists pointed out elements which were<br />

said to refer to feelings, it was from a personal knowledge <strong>of</strong> the patient rather than from<br />

any characteristics inherent in the <strong>art</strong>. <strong>The</strong> naturally nonthreatening environment was<br />

again the main benefit <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> in supportive relationships, although some <strong>of</strong> what was<br />

reported was not therapy orientated but occupational, in that there was no change in the<br />

patient expected.<br />

83


Conclusion<br />

<strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> was not inherently related to the therapy for the patient but was felt by<br />

the therapist to provide a special sort <strong>of</strong> receptive atmosphere. Artwork was an activity<br />

in which patients felt able and liked to p<strong>art</strong>icipate. When used therapeutically, it <strong>of</strong>fers<br />

a non direct form <strong>of</strong> confrontation with underlying issues <strong>of</strong> conflict and the obscurity<br />

<strong>of</strong> the communication encourages verbal interactions and allows mutual involvement<br />

towards the goal <strong>of</strong> interpretation <strong>of</strong> the meaning through this medium. <strong>The</strong>refore the<br />

stated purpose <strong>of</strong> the use <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> in therapy relationships was communicative, but the<br />

underlying purpose <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong> component was distraction and distancing for the patient.<br />

4. Change in behaviour<br />

Table 8 (appendix 2), summarised information from 4 studies which used an <strong>art</strong><br />

environment but measured changes in behaviour. <strong>The</strong>ir results reflect the conclusions <strong>of</strong><br />

the supportive relationships; the environment <strong>of</strong>fers a non-threatening situation which<br />

is enjoyed by the subjects. <strong>The</strong> skills learned increase self esteem, especially if<br />

recognised by others and increased involvement in the work. <strong>The</strong> other studies reported<br />

verbal/nonverbal comparisons <strong>of</strong> disturbance <strong>of</strong> thought in schizophrenia (1) that lower<br />

levels <strong>of</strong> disorder occurred whilst engaging in <strong>art</strong>work; and (2) Speech and language were<br />

both affected, but planning and carrying out a plan were the most affected. <strong>The</strong>se results<br />

do not cover the area, but one suggests that nonverbal pathways may not be as affected<br />

by thought disorder as verbal pathways. Speculatively, it is possible that the reason <strong>art</strong><br />

84


is less affected is that drawing is not rigidly sequential and that relationships and time are<br />

expressed in different dimensions than the construct <strong>of</strong> language.<br />

Summary <strong>of</strong> Case Studies<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were two elements recorded in <strong>art</strong> therapy research using case studies: (a) the effect<br />

<strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong> process upon the patient <strong>of</strong> which reports the studies mostly consisted, and<br />

(b) the process <strong>of</strong> decoding the picture, which was reported obscurely, individualistically<br />

and inconsistently.<br />

(a) <strong>The</strong>rapy: <strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> with patients seems to be therapeutic since observational<br />

evidence <strong>of</strong> lower symptom levels and educational or social benefits such as skills learnt<br />

by the patient are presented and there is a suggestion that <strong>art</strong> may provide an alternative<br />

route to communication for the thought disordered patient. Art therapy is generally<br />

useful for retarded people as an activity where people feel able, although there is no<br />

agreement on what kind <strong>of</strong> techniques to apply. <strong>The</strong> relationship between the therapist<br />

and the patient was very important in the view <strong>of</strong> the therapist and was said to provide<br />

therapeutic benefits in itself, but the nature <strong>of</strong> the involvement by the therapist tended<br />

to obscure the reporting. Thus the therapist was not the ideal researcher.<br />

(b) Decoding: <strong>The</strong> largest section examined here was that categorised as general<br />

expression <strong>of</strong> feelings, where projective methods and content interpretation were thought<br />

to induce cath<strong>art</strong>ic release and facilitate communication between the therapist and patient.<br />

However, these benefits seemed to be independent <strong>of</strong> method or orientation <strong>of</strong> the study<br />

and seem to indicate that the <strong>art</strong> provides a third interactive function for the patient.<br />

85


<strong>The</strong>matic interpretation varied with the patient and was dependent on the relationship<br />

with the therapist. No common correspondence could be discerned when comparing<br />

studies in this review. <strong>The</strong> interpretation <strong>of</strong> content was a shared communicative device,<br />

depictions <strong>of</strong>ten bore emotional loads, were idiosyncratic and did not translate to<br />

universal characteristics.<br />

Observable characteristics in <strong>art</strong>work do indicate psychiatric status, whatever the<br />

orientation <strong>of</strong> the therapist or the psychiatric status <strong>of</strong> the patient, but there is no<br />

agreement on differentiation, definition <strong>of</strong> terms or typical diagnostic characteristics.<br />

Systematic research which differentiates between content and form would further this<br />

investigation and test premises which are expressed as knowledge in the literature; no<br />

study used formal characteristics alone in this review.<br />

Case studies are the traditional and best known method <strong>of</strong> reporting therapy and<br />

Chapter One describes how this type <strong>of</strong> research is still recommended by prominent<br />

writers and the weaknesses <strong>of</strong> this approach. <strong>The</strong> authors are unable to even describe the<br />

paintings, as the problem <strong>of</strong> standardising terminology, identifying and describing changes<br />

in observable form or in content has not been successfully addressed. <strong>The</strong> usual method<br />

has been to print sample paintings from which it is intuited which elements represent the<br />

improvement <strong>of</strong> the patient. <strong>The</strong> lack <strong>of</strong> established psychopathological links means that<br />

the case study method does not fulfil the crucial point discussed, how <strong>psychopathology</strong><br />

is expressed in <strong>art</strong>work. This is a very poor method <strong>of</strong> assessment.<br />

86


Controlled Studies. Comparison and Analysis<br />

Problems in categorising the data for analysis<br />

Several factors limit the conclusions which can be drawn from this review <strong>of</strong> controlled<br />

investigations into patient <strong>art</strong>. <strong>The</strong> tendency <strong>of</strong> the research was towards clinical tools,<br />

and there was little replication or validation <strong>of</strong> the plethora <strong>of</strong> instruments proposed.<br />

Speculative studies and unpublished, unvalidated measures were <strong>of</strong>ten cited by other<br />

studies as though they were proven instruments; some studies did not even specify<br />

which scale they were using and others did not explain or validate measures developed<br />

for the study.<br />

Inappropriate statistics were frequent, and test design and results <strong>of</strong>ten bore little<br />

relation to the intentions, conclusions and interpretations <strong>of</strong> the author. This meant the<br />

orientation <strong>of</strong> the researcher and the relation <strong>of</strong> the description or purpose <strong>of</strong> the<br />

instrument to what was recorded had to be considered. In many studies the focus on the<br />

interpretation <strong>of</strong> the work dominated the description <strong>of</strong> the objective phenomena, and the<br />

opinions and involvement <strong>of</strong> the therapist were also treated as objective phenomena. <strong>The</strong><br />

use <strong>of</strong> subjective or interpretive criteria was <strong>of</strong>ten erroneously described as formal, or<br />

objective. Behavioural and other changes occurring in the patient, were attributed to the<br />

<strong>art</strong> therapy even though it may have been geared towards a totally different goal, and<br />

other therapeutic influences were ignored.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were two stages to this analysis:<br />

(1) to find out what kind <strong>of</strong> techniques, orientation and concepts produce results or no<br />

results <strong>of</strong> whatever kind in <strong>art</strong> therapy; and<br />

87


(2) to find out how the <strong>art</strong> relates to the kind <strong>of</strong> characteristics attributed to it.<br />

<strong>The</strong> problems were: in (1), that the study definitions <strong>of</strong> approaches were sometimes<br />

inconsistent with their practice; and in (2), the information reported was <strong>of</strong>ten<br />

incomplete. <strong>The</strong> solution I adopted was to descriptively examine as much <strong>of</strong> the<br />

literature as possible for the first question and to narrow the focus gradually for the<br />

second question, dropping out studies which could not supply the information.<br />

<strong>The</strong> DAPA 1996 study<br />

One <strong>of</strong> the studies picked up by the literature search was the DAPA pilot study<br />

(1996) 155 , it has not been included because as p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the present thesis, this research was<br />

itself developed from an informal version <strong>of</strong> the process <strong>of</strong> examination <strong>of</strong> the literature.<br />

Hypotheses and development <strong>of</strong> the measure has occurred over a period <strong>of</strong> 6 years.<br />

Inclusion <strong>of</strong> the DAPA test would hardly affect the qualitative analysis, because it carries<br />

few content fields, but would probably bias the meta-analysis which tries to show the<br />

direction and efficacy <strong>of</strong> the main body <strong>of</strong> literature.<br />

Organisation <strong>of</strong> this section<br />

<strong>The</strong> literature is discussed in 3 p<strong>art</strong>s 156 :<br />

155<br />

Hacking, S., Foreman, D., Belcher, J. (1996) <strong>The</strong> DAPA: a new way <strong>of</strong> quantifying psychiatric<br />

paintings, J. Nervous Mental Dis . 184 p.425-9.<br />

156 <strong>The</strong>re are three analyses which contain different variations from the same pool <strong>of</strong> 79 studies. For clarity,<br />

studies retained the same identification number in all analyses. Numbers 1-79 alphabetically are used<br />

throughout. A full list <strong>of</strong> studies appears in Appendix 3 (Table <strong>of</strong> Authorities).<br />

88


1. All studies reported enough information about tests between patients and<br />

controls, or patient groups to be evaluated methodologically for orientation,<br />

concepts and results.<br />

2. A small number <strong>of</strong> studies which investigated the correspondence <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> tests<br />

against criterion measures and were not included in group 1 were added for the<br />

discussion <strong>of</strong> validity.<br />

3. A small group <strong>of</strong> studies which investigated inter-rater reliability were added to<br />

those studies from group 1 which used more than one rater for:<br />

(a) an evaluation <strong>of</strong> psychopathological signs in patient <strong>art</strong>work;<br />

(b) a meta analysis to determine the effect size for <strong>art</strong> measures (Chapter 3).<br />

Amongst the 51 controlled studies were 4 comparisons between therapy groups. <strong>The</strong>y<br />

measured behavioural details using non-<strong>art</strong> measures and one rater, although two used<br />

another criterion measure. <strong>The</strong>y are included in the analysis where appropriate, but their<br />

aims, directions and information presented were different and they were omitted from<br />

some <strong>of</strong> the tables for clarity. Group sizes therefore do not always add up to 51.<br />

Methodology for analysis <strong>of</strong> controlled studies<br />

Characteristics <strong>of</strong> each paper were tabulated according to the variables described below.<br />

Descriptive statistics were used for a preliminary comparison <strong>of</strong> data from all 51 studies<br />

to find out what results were claimed for <strong>art</strong> therapy. <strong>The</strong>re were 3 categories <strong>of</strong><br />

information common to most papers: information about the test subjects, the study<br />

purpose and procedure, the results and conclusions from the test.<br />

89


<strong>The</strong>re were 7 demographic variables:<br />

sex <strong>of</strong> subject;<br />

ages, whether adolescent (13-18), adult (18-60), older adult (60+);<br />

diagnosis <strong>of</strong> condition 1, 2, 3 and 4 (condition 1 was the main experimental<br />

group and condition 2 the main comparison group, which were normal controls<br />

if included);<br />

number <strong>of</strong> subjects in condition 1;<br />

total no. <strong>of</strong> subjects in study;<br />

whether subjects were matched on age and sex;<br />

no. <strong>of</strong> judges.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were 5 study factors:<br />

orientation, type <strong>of</strong> therapy <strong>of</strong>fered;<br />

measurement method, what the instrument was intended to measure;<br />

design <strong>of</strong> the test - pre and post intervention, post intervention only,<br />

comparison <strong>of</strong> test pictures or retrospective;<br />

no. <strong>of</strong> measures used;<br />

derivation <strong>of</strong> the main study measure, whether own test, adapted or established.<br />

Sensitivity <strong>of</strong> the test and results.<br />

measurement form, what type <strong>of</strong> elements in the picture the instrument was<br />

actually sensitive to;<br />

the results <strong>of</strong> the study: differences between patients and normal or patient<br />

controls; or no difference.<br />

90


One alternative was marked for each category 157 . For ease <strong>of</strong> readability, integer<br />

percentages are used here. Interactive effects were investigated and the results for the<br />

ordinal variables are presented in tables 8a and 8b.<br />

Demographic variables: descriptive statistics for demographic variables are shown in<br />

Table 1 and Table 2.<br />

Table 1. Descriptive Statistics: controlled studies n=51<br />

Variable Mean Std. Dev. Min. Max.<br />

Numbers <strong>of</strong> subjects<br />

in experimental<br />

groups<br />

No. subjects each<br />

paper covered<br />

39 40 5 239<br />

115 190 11 1373<br />

No. <strong>of</strong> judges 2 8 1 60<br />

No. <strong>of</strong> measures 2 2 1 15<br />

Table 2. Frequency and percentage <strong>of</strong> demographic variables for 51 controlled studies by<br />

levels <strong>of</strong> result.<br />

Variables<br />

Sex male<br />

female<br />

mixed<br />

Age 13-18<br />

18-60<br />

60+<br />

Controls<br />

Matched<br />

Unmatched<br />

Frequency<br />

& Percent<br />

all studies<br />

n=51<br />

freq %<br />

5 10<br />

4 8<br />

42 82<br />

6 8<br />

41 80<br />

4 8<br />

20 39<br />

31 61<br />

Result 1<br />

Difference in<br />

patients/nonpatients<br />

n=21<br />

freq %<br />

0 0<br />

2 14<br />

18 86<br />

0 0<br />

17 81<br />

4 19<br />

10 48<br />

11 52<br />

157 A full list <strong>of</strong> alternatives is presented in Appendix 1.<br />

91<br />

Result 2<br />

Difference<br />

patient<br />

subtypes<br />

n=12<br />

freq %<br />

3 25<br />

0 0<br />

9 75<br />

2 17<br />

10 83<br />

0 0<br />

5 42<br />

7 58<br />

Result 3<br />

No Difference<br />

or<br />

Inconsistency<br />

n=14<br />

freq %<br />

2 14<br />

1 7<br />

11 79<br />

2 14<br />

12 86<br />

0 0<br />

3 21<br />

11 79


No. 1<br />

judges 2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

60<br />

No. 2<br />

<strong>Study</strong> 3<br />

groups 4<br />

35 69<br />

9 18<br />

4 8<br />

2 4<br />

1 2<br />

32 63<br />

15 29<br />

4 8<br />

13 62<br />

3 14<br />

3 14<br />

1 5<br />

1 5<br />

13 62<br />

6 29<br />

2 10<br />

92<br />

8 67<br />

2 17<br />

1 8<br />

1 8<br />

0 0<br />

6 50<br />

5 42<br />

1 8<br />

10 71<br />

4 29<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

10 71<br />

3 21<br />

1 7<br />

Age and sex: Few studies gave much demographic information. Many studies simply<br />

described the group as 'adult', but the majority described only the group ranges or the<br />

mean age even when they were considerably wide. <strong>The</strong> sex distribution was <strong>of</strong>ten only<br />

described as 'mixed' so could have been considerably uneven and sometimes group<br />

numbers were missing, especially <strong>of</strong> diagnostic categories.<br />

Controls: 72% <strong>of</strong> studies used non-psychiatric controls, and gave little information on<br />

age, sex, origin, screening procedure and numbers. Where descriptions <strong>of</strong> controls were<br />

given, they were always hospital staff. Mostly, there was no indication that control<br />

pictures were done under the same conditions, or even the same number <strong>of</strong> pictures!<br />

(One study used an average <strong>of</strong> up to 99 pictures from therapy sessions for patients,<br />

against one picture painted in a non psychiatric outside <strong>art</strong> group 158 ).<br />

Matching: Chapter one makes the point that IQ is difficult to match for psychiatric<br />

patients, since tests may not be sympathetic to psychiatric disturbance, but the majority<br />

<strong>of</strong> studies failed to match for age, sex and mostly even group numbers.<br />

158<br />

C. Bergman and M. Gonzalez (1993), Art and Madness: can the interface be quantified? Am. J. Art<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy , V.31:81-90 on development <strong>of</strong> the SPAR scale.


Table 3a. Diagnostic groups 1-4. Frequency and percentage for 51 controlled studies.<br />

Diagnosis,<br />

schizophrenia<br />

emotional trauma<br />

drug/alcohol<br />

psychotic phobia<br />

neurological damage<br />

depression<br />

conduct disorder<br />

retarded<br />

gender disorder<br />

personality disorder<br />

mixed patients<br />

normal<br />

All studies,<br />

diagnosis 1<br />

n=51<br />

freq %<br />

15 29<br />

4 8<br />

1 2<br />

3 6<br />

6 12<br />

6 12<br />

1 2<br />

1 2<br />

1 2<br />

1 2<br />

12 23.5<br />

0 0<br />

All studies,<br />

diagnosis 2<br />

n=51<br />

freq %<br />

5 10<br />

1 2<br />

0 0<br />

2 4<br />

2 4<br />

1 0<br />

1 2<br />

0 2<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

3 6<br />

36 71<br />

93<br />

All studies,<br />

diagnosis 3<br />

n=19<br />

freq %<br />

2 4<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

1 2<br />

5 10<br />

1 2<br />

2 4<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

6 12<br />

2 4<br />

All studies,<br />

diagnosis 4<br />

n=4<br />

freq %<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

1 2<br />

1 2<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

2 4<br />

Diagnoses: 1. Main experimental group; 2. Control group; 3. Experimental group 2; 4. Experimental<br />

group 3.<br />

Table 3b. Diagnostic group 1. Frequency and percentage by levels <strong>of</strong> result.<br />

Diagnosis,<br />

1<br />

schizophrenia<br />

emotional trauma<br />

drug/alcohol<br />

psychotic phobia<br />

neurological damage<br />

depression<br />

conduct disorder<br />

retarded<br />

gender disorder<br />

personality disorder<br />

mixed patients<br />

1. Difference<br />

patients/nonpatients<br />

n=21<br />

freq %<br />

7 33<br />

2 9.5<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

3 14<br />

4 19<br />

0 0<br />

1 5<br />

0 0<br />

1 5<br />

3 14<br />

2. Difference<br />

patient<br />

subtypes<br />

n=12<br />

freq %<br />

4 33<br />

1 8<br />

1 8<br />

2 17<br />

2 17<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

2 17<br />

3. No<br />

Difference or<br />

inconsistency<br />

n=14<br />

freq %<br />

4 29<br />

1 7<br />

0 0<br />

1 7<br />

1 7<br />

1 7<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

1 7<br />

0 0<br />

5 36<br />

Diagnoses: Frequency tables for diagnoses are shown in Table 3a, and in 3b by<br />

experimental group results. <strong>The</strong> main comparison group were classified under condition<br />

2, and the experimental under condition 1. Those normal controls appearing in groups<br />

3 and 4 were less important to the study than the main comparison group. <strong>The</strong> most<br />

frequent diagnosis was Schizophrenia, mostly undifferentiated and which probably<br />

included diagnoses which were differentiated by other studies. Schizophrenia is not a


satisfactory classification as it is not an exact diagnosis and there are forms that have<br />

quite different phenomenology. Some studies simply tested ward groups (which can be<br />

very variable) and in many cases there was no operational criteria typical <strong>of</strong> a common<br />

symptom picture.<br />

Other study effects: It was impossible to control for experience <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>, as the vast<br />

majority <strong>of</strong> studies did not describe the psychiatric history <strong>of</strong> their patients. Although<br />

one or two tried to control for <strong>art</strong> experience, their reliance on formal or school education<br />

tended to favour the controls and did not account for therapy sessions, which could be<br />

p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> a patient's life for many years.<br />

Time: the studies were spread fairly evenly over the 22 year period; 50% <strong>of</strong> the studies<br />

either side <strong>of</strong> 1986.<br />

Table 4. T-tests were performed to compare the means <strong>of</strong> ordinal demographic variables<br />

by Year <strong>of</strong> <strong>Study</strong>.<br />

GROUP 1 = YEARS 1973-1977 (12 CASES); GROUP 2 = YEARS 1992-1996 (18 CASES)<br />

df=28 for all variables<br />

Variable<br />

Group--<br />

No. Subjects 1<br />

(Condition 1) 2<br />

Age 1<br />

3=13-18; 2<br />

5=18-60; 6=60+<br />

Sex 1<br />

1=male; 2<br />

2=female; 3=mixed<br />

Matched 1<br />

Controls(age/sex) 2<br />

match 1=yes; 2=no<br />

No. Judges 1<br />

2<br />

No. Measures 1<br />

2<br />

mean Standard<br />

Deviation<br />

33.50<br />

39.22<br />

4.83<br />

4.83<br />

2.83<br />

2.61<br />

1.58<br />

1.61<br />

0.67<br />

0.78<br />

1.42<br />

2.44<br />

26.62<br />

38.56<br />

0.58<br />

0.92<br />

0.58<br />

0.70<br />

0.51<br />

0.50<br />

0.99<br />

1.35<br />

0.79<br />

3.31<br />

Standard<br />

Error<br />

7.68<br />

9.09<br />

0.17<br />

0.22<br />

0.17<br />

0.16<br />

0.14<br />

0.12<br />

0.28<br />

0.32<br />

0.23<br />

0.78<br />

94<br />

T<br />

Value<br />

-0.45 0.66 NS<br />

0.00 1.00 NS<br />

0.91 0.37 NS<br />

-0.15 0.88 NS<br />

-0.24 0.81 NS<br />

-1.05 0.30 NS<br />

Pooled Variance<br />

2-tailed probability


Table 5: Non-parametric tests were performed to compare the ranks <strong>of</strong> categorical study<br />

variables by Year <strong>of</strong> <strong>Study</strong>.<br />

GROUP 1 = YEARS 1973-1977 (12 CASES); GROUP 2 = YEARS 1992-1996 (18 CASES)<br />

df=28 for all variables<br />

Variable<br />

Group -<br />

Diagnosis<br />

Orientation<br />

Measurement method<br />

Measurement form<br />

design<br />

test derivation<br />

results<br />

mean<br />

rank 1<br />

15.67<br />

11.00<br />

11.92<br />

12.08<br />

14.92<br />

11.58<br />

16.08<br />

mean<br />

rank 2<br />

15.39<br />

15.17<br />

17.89<br />

17.78<br />

15.89<br />

18.11<br />

15.11<br />

95<br />

z score pooled variance<br />

2 tailed prob.<br />

-0.0857<br />

-0.3644<br />

-1.9695<br />

-1.8276<br />

-0.3664<br />

-2.2196<br />

-0.3122<br />

0.9317<br />

0.7156<br />

0.0489<br />

0.0676<br />

0 7141<br />

0.0264<br />

0.7549<br />

Elements <strong>of</strong> change over 22 years: t-tests were performed to compare ordinal<br />

demographic variables over the period <strong>of</strong> the study (22 years) (Table 4), in order to<br />

assess change in research techniques and orientation. <strong>The</strong> categorical variables were<br />

compared by rank, using the Man-Whitney non-parametric t-equivalent (Table 3b). No<br />

differences were found in demographic variables, in orientation <strong>of</strong> therapy, measurement<br />

form, the design <strong>of</strong> the test or the results, but there were differences in the measurement<br />

method (the described purpose <strong>of</strong> the test). Derivation <strong>of</strong> the test and measurement form<br />

just missed significance but is considered a strong trend here (see Table 6). Both the early<br />

tests and the later described their methods as formal comparisons or mixed content and<br />

form comparisons, but the earlier studies used more direct formal comparisons and the<br />

later more mixed and more non-<strong>art</strong> tests. <strong>The</strong> earlier emphasis on form probably reflects<br />

the exploratory nature <strong>of</strong> the studies and their developmental basis. <strong>The</strong>ir tests are<br />

almost exclusively self-developed and more sensitive to style and formal elements; the<br />

later studies were more split between self-developed and existing tests. <strong>The</strong>y compared<br />

and described more interpretatively and used behavioural terms, their tests were less<br />

sensitive to form than content comparisons in the same test. <strong>The</strong>y also used behavioural


or non-<strong>art</strong> tests which were not seen at all previously. <strong>The</strong> actual tests do not<br />

significantly differ in their orientation, but the differences in the other variables tend to<br />

support the trend.<br />

Table 6: Significant variables identified from the Mann-Whitney non-parametric<br />

association test. Frequency and percentage for Group 1, early studies 1973-1977; Group<br />

2, late studies 1992-1996. Changes in methods and measurements.<br />

Variables with scales Early studies<br />

freq %<br />

Measurement method<br />

Formal<br />

Meaning content<br />

Mixed form and content<br />

Behaviour- non-<strong>art</strong><br />

Measurement form<br />

Formal comparison<br />

Meaning interpretive<br />

Mixed form and content<br />

Non-<strong>art</strong> or behaviour<br />

Verbal<br />

Derivation <strong>of</strong> test<br />

This study self-developed<br />

Adapted existing<br />

Existing<br />

Observational/clinical<br />

7 58<br />

1 8<br />

4 33<br />

7 58<br />

3 25<br />

2 17<br />

10 83<br />

1 8<br />

1 8<br />

Late studies<br />

freq %<br />

5 28<br />

1 6<br />

9 50<br />

3 17<br />

6 33<br />

4 22<br />

2 11<br />

4 22<br />

2 11<br />

7 39<br />

3 17<br />

7 39<br />

1 6<br />

<strong>The</strong> z values for all variables in Table 5 are negative which indicates linear relations. Over<br />

22 years, similar elements are measured in <strong>art</strong> tests although there were differences in<br />

orientation towards content and more interpretation <strong>of</strong> content from the later studies.<br />

Most earlier tests were developed for the research and the later studies used a mix <strong>of</strong> self<br />

developed and existing tests, but with no greater controls. Controlled comparisons exhibit<br />

the same type <strong>of</strong> mixed sex and ill-differentiated groups. <strong>The</strong>re is no better match <strong>of</strong><br />

controls to condition 1 subjects and results are still ambiguous and inconsistent. Thus,<br />

research into <strong>art</strong> <strong>psychopathology</strong> has remained static.<br />

96


Table 7: Frequency and percentage <strong>of</strong> test variables for whole sample by Results 159 .<br />

Variables n=51<br />

freq %<br />

Test design<br />

pre & post int'n<br />

post intervention<br />

comparison only<br />

retrospective<br />

No. measures<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

15<br />

Derivation <strong>of</strong> test<br />

developed this study<br />

adapted<br />

observation/clinical<br />

established<br />

Orientation<br />

comparison only<br />

expressive<br />

therapy - self esteem<br />

Measurement method<br />

formal comparison <strong>art</strong><br />

meaning/content<br />

mixed comparison<br />

non<strong>art</strong>/behaviour<br />

Measurement form<br />

objective detail<br />

content subject/theme<br />

mix objective/content<br />

other behaviour<br />

verbal<br />

All studies Result 1<br />

Difference<br />

patients/<br />

nonpatients<br />

6 12<br />

3 6<br />

36 71<br />

6 12<br />

31 61<br />

9 18<br />

8 16<br />

2 4<br />

1 2<br />

23 45<br />

8 16<br />

4 8<br />

16 31<br />

43 84<br />

3 6<br />

5 10<br />

19 37<br />

4 8<br />

21 41<br />

7 13<br />

18 35<br />

11 22<br />

13 26<br />

7 14<br />

2 2<br />

n=21<br />

freq %<br />

2 10<br />

1 5<br />

16 76<br />

2 10<br />

14 67<br />

3 14<br />

3 14<br />

0 0<br />

1 5<br />

8 38<br />

5 24<br />

1 5<br />

7 33<br />

21 100<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

8 38<br />

2 10<br />

11 52<br />

0 0<br />

9 43<br />

4 20<br />

7 33<br />

1 5<br />

0 0<br />

97<br />

Result 2<br />

Difference<br />

patient<br />

subtypes<br />

n=12<br />

freq %<br />

1 8<br />

0 0<br />

9 75<br />

2 17<br />

8 67<br />

3 25<br />

0 0<br />

1 8<br />

0 0<br />

5 42<br />

1 8<br />

3 25<br />

3 25<br />

10 83<br />

1 8<br />

1 8<br />

5 42<br />

0 0<br />

6 50<br />

1 8<br />

4 33<br />

2 17<br />

4 33<br />

2 17<br />

0 0<br />

Result 3<br />

No Difference or<br />

inconsistency<br />

n=14<br />

freq %<br />

1 7<br />

0 0<br />

11 79<br />

2 14<br />

7 50<br />

2 14<br />

4 29<br />

1 7<br />

0 0<br />

9 64<br />

2 14<br />

0 0<br />

3 21<br />

12 86<br />

1 7<br />

1 7<br />

6 43<br />

2 14<br />

4 29<br />

2 14<br />

5 36<br />

5 36<br />

2 14<br />

0 0<br />

2 14<br />

Design: <strong>The</strong> majority <strong>of</strong> the studies (71%, n=36) compared a picture specially produced<br />

for the test rather than pictures produced during therapy. Pre and post measures were<br />

159 Results - all cases frequencies percentage<br />

1. Difference patient/non patient 21 41.2<br />

2. Difference subtypes <strong>of</strong> patient 12 23.5<br />

3. No differences / inconsistencies 14 27.4<br />

4. Difference for therapy groups 4 7.8<br />

TOTAL 51


favoured (12% n=6) over post measures (6% n=3) for therapy state <strong>of</strong> patients. 12%<br />

compared spontaneous pictures retrospectively.<br />

No. <strong>of</strong> measures: 61% (n=31) <strong>of</strong> studies used only the test described and the diagnosis;<br />

18% (n=9) used one other measure and 22% (n=11) used 3 or more.<br />

Derivation <strong>of</strong> measure: 69% (n=35) <strong>of</strong> tests were either; designed for the study (23),<br />

were adaptions <strong>of</strong> an existing test (8), or were observed or clinical ratings (4), and only<br />

31% <strong>of</strong> studies used an established test.<br />

<strong>The</strong> orientation variable shows 84% (n=43) <strong>of</strong> studies measured drawings produced for<br />

the study and not as p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> a therapy programme.<br />

Measurement method: Scales have not been contrasted on this review as many studies<br />

failed to provide coherent definitions <strong>of</strong> what they did measure, relying on common <strong>art</strong><br />

terms. Most studies included present or countable items, but this form <strong>of</strong> measurement<br />

is not always objective as it <strong>of</strong>ten requires interpretation, if only in the sense that marks<br />

must be recognised and judged against an internal model by the rater. For example, to<br />

note that a head is or is not unusually large, a head must be reconstructed from the marks<br />

on the paper and compared with what the rater thinks is normal.<br />

This category brings together what is generally accepted as observable criteria: i.e.<br />

those generally recorded in draw-a-person tests that there is little disagreement in<br />

recognising. In applying the main test measure to the drawings, 41% (n=21) <strong>of</strong> tests<br />

used a comparison requiring both subjective and objective judgments; 37% (n=19) used<br />

a direct comparison <strong>of</strong> observable detail (e.g. draw a person tests); 12% used non <strong>art</strong> tests<br />

(e.g. IQ, achievement, behavioural tests etc.).<br />

98


Measurement form: <strong>The</strong> most frequent forms measured from the patient were observed<br />

details (35%), the next was a mix <strong>of</strong> details and content (25%) closely followed by<br />

content measures <strong>of</strong> subject/theme (22%) and behaviour (12%).<br />

Results: (see notes, Table 7). According to this analysis <strong>of</strong> 51 studies, 72.5% (n=37) <strong>of</strong><br />

studies report differences between the experimental group and controls, either between<br />

patient/non patient (41%, n=21), subgroups (23% n=12) or therapy groups (7.8%, n=4)<br />

(these claims will be further investigated in Chapter Three, within the tightly controlled<br />

comparison criteria <strong>of</strong> meta analysis).<br />

Summary: <strong>The</strong> typical test compared a drawing done in standardised conditions by<br />

about 30 adult schizophrenics <strong>of</strong> both sexes, and an unmatched control group <strong>of</strong> people<br />

without psychiatric history. <strong>The</strong> test would be designed for the study, be rated by the<br />

author, and would not be compared with any other measure except the diagnosis.<br />

Patients' pictures would be expected to differ from non-patients by both content,<br />

typically subject or theme decisions; and objective detail, such as p<strong>art</strong>icular colours and<br />

positioning <strong>of</strong> figures.<br />

Interactive effects for study variables by level <strong>of</strong> Result (shown in Tables 2 and 7).<br />

Demographics: Most studies employed mixed sex experimental groups, adult subjects<br />

and 2 conditions (normal controls). Sex and age were <strong>of</strong>ten only reported as a range or<br />

categorically, i.e. adult. Differences between patients and controls were found in all <strong>of</strong><br />

the older groups (Table 2), but this may reflect differences in expectations, measurement<br />

99


procedures, or publishing bias, since there were very few studies using old people.<br />

Control groups for studies that found differences either in patient/control or between<br />

subtypes, were equally balanced in matched controls and non-matched, and more <strong>of</strong>ten<br />

schizophrenic, than a range <strong>of</strong> other psychiatric diagnoses. Where no differences were<br />

found, control groups were not usually matched (Table 2), but there was a clear<br />

predominance <strong>of</strong> either undifferentiated patients or schizophrenics. Unsurprisingly,<br />

studies finding subtype differences used more experimental groups; 4 included normal<br />

controls.<br />

Design and sensitivity <strong>of</strong> test: <strong>The</strong> tests finding differences were most frequently<br />

designed to measure a mix <strong>of</strong> observable qualities and interpretative elements and their<br />

instruments were generally congruent with his aim, although 20% measured content<br />

exclusively. Studies finding no difference emphasised the measurement <strong>of</strong> formal<br />

elements in the design rather than content, but a third <strong>of</strong> their instruments were<br />

exclusively sensitive to content. Tests which found patient/nonpatient differences used<br />

a mix <strong>of</strong> self-developed, adapted and established tests; tests finding differences between<br />

subtypes used a mix <strong>of</strong> self developed, observational and established tests; but tests<br />

which found no differences used many more tests developed for the study (Table 7).<br />

Reliability and Validity: <strong>The</strong> majority <strong>of</strong> studies which found no differences used<br />

slightly more 1-rater tests (Table 2) but more criterion measures than studies finding<br />

differences (Table 4), all results levels had few measures and raters.<br />

100


Summary: Studies finding no differences were fewer and less controlled than those<br />

which found differences; they used more unmatched controls, more undifferentiated<br />

patients, and more tests developed for the study, coupled with less judges. <strong>The</strong>ir<br />

instruments were less suited to their stated purpose than tests that found differences.<br />

Table 8a: Analysis <strong>of</strong> variance compared 5 demographic variables by 7 study factors.<br />

162<br />

<strong>Study</strong> Factors Diagnoses Design Valid Test<br />

Test variables<br />

No. <strong>of</strong> subjects<br />

condition 1<br />

age <strong>of</strong> subjects<br />

3=13-18; 5=18-60;<br />

6=60+<br />

sex <strong>of</strong> subjects<br />

1-male; 2-female;<br />

3-mixed<br />

controls matched<br />

1-yes; 2-no<br />

df=12 sig<br />

F <strong>of</strong> F<br />

df=3 sig<br />

F <strong>of</strong> F<br />

101<br />

df=3 sig<br />

F <strong>of</strong> F<br />

0.51 0.89 0.62 0.61 0.28 0.84<br />

1.68 0.12 0.67 0.58 1.79 0.16<br />

2.41 0.02 160<br />

0.28 0.84 0.38 0.77<br />

1.78 0.09 2.10 0.11 4.40 0.01 161<br />

no. <strong>of</strong> judges 0.19 1.00 0.11 0.95 0.57 0.64<br />

160 Too few cells are filled for meaningful analysis. 2 <strong>of</strong> the 12 diagnostic groups cover 53% <strong>of</strong> studies.<br />

161 Studies using adapted tests were less likely to match for age and sex in controls.<br />

Summaries <strong>of</strong> age and sex match in control by levels <strong>of</strong> validated test<br />

VALIDTEST LEVELS Mean Std Dev Cases<br />

For Entire Population 1.61 .49 51<br />

1 test developed for study 1.83 .39 23<br />

2 test adapted 1.25 .46 8<br />

3 established test 1.44 .51 16<br />

4 observation/clinical 1.75 .50 4<br />

162 Non<strong>art</strong> behaviourial measures tended to be used with younger subjects.<br />

Summaries <strong>of</strong> age group for condition 1 by levels <strong>of</strong> measurement method<br />

LEVELS OF METHOD Mean Std Dev Cases<br />

For Entire Population 4.80 .75 51<br />

1 direct comparison 4.89 .57 19<br />

2 meaning 4.75 .50 4<br />

3 some interpretation 4.95 .74 21<br />

4 non<strong>art</strong> - behaviourial 4.33 1.03 6


Table 8b. Analysis <strong>of</strong> variance performed to compare the means <strong>of</strong> 5 ordinal<br />

demographic variables by 7 study factors.<br />

<strong>Study</strong> Factors Orientation<br />

(df=5)<br />

F p<br />

No. <strong>of</strong> subjects<br />

condition 1<br />

subjects age<br />

3=13-18; 5=18-60; 6=60+<br />

sex <strong>of</strong> subjects<br />

1-m; 2-f; 3-mixed<br />

controls matched<br />

1-yes; 2-no<br />

Measurement<br />

method (df=5)<br />

F p<br />

102<br />

Measurement<br />

Form (df=5)<br />

F p<br />

Results<br />

(df=4)<br />

F p<br />

1.03 0.36 0.72 0.58 2.05 0.10 0.81 0.53<br />

1.92 0.16 2.63 0.05 7<br />

5.29 0.00 163<br />

0.35 0.71 0.44 0.78 2.88 0.03 165<br />

2.47 0.06 164<br />

0.92 0.46<br />

0.49 0.62 1.58 0.20 1.39 0.25 0.92 0.45<br />

no. <strong>of</strong> judges 0.16 0.85 0.34 0.85 0.39 0.82 0.41 0.80<br />

6 personality 3.00 .00 1<br />

163 Studies which used measures <strong>of</strong> behaviour or content used younger subjects.<br />

Summaries <strong>of</strong> age group for condition 1 by levels <strong>of</strong> measurement form<br />

MEASUREMENT FORM Mean Std Dev Cases<br />

For Entire Population 4.80 .75 51<br />

1 objective detail 5.17 .38 18<br />

2 content - theme 4.36 .92 11<br />

3 mixed 1 and 2 5.00 .41 13<br />

4 behaviour 4.00 1.10 6<br />

5 verbal 5.00 .00 2<br />

6 expressive other 5.00 .00 1<br />

164 <strong>The</strong>rapy groups were younger; groups in studies using criterion measures tended to be older; and studies<br />

finding a patient/nonpatient difference used no adolescents and contained all the older groups.<br />

Summaries <strong>of</strong> age group condition 1 by levels <strong>of</strong> result<br />

LEVELS OF RESULT Mean Std Dev Cases<br />

For Entire Population 4.80 .75 51<br />

1 difference in experim'l group1 5.10 .54 21<br />

2 diffs. in subgroups 4.67 .78 12<br />

3 no difference 4.80 .63 10<br />

4 therapy group 4.00 1.15 4<br />

5 noncorrelation/inconsistent 4.50 1.00 4<br />

165 Mixed objective/subjective elements were measured exclusively in mixed groups.<br />

Summaries <strong>of</strong> sex by levels <strong>of</strong> measurement form<br />

MEASUREMENT FORM Mean Std Dev Cases<br />

For Entire Population 2.73 .63 51<br />

1 objective detail 2.72 .67 18<br />

2 content - theme 2.64 .68 11<br />

3 mixed 1 and 2 3.00 .00 13<br />

4 behaviour 2.67 .82 6<br />

5 verbal 1.50 .71 2<br />

6 expressive other 3.00 .00 1


Demographic interactive effects: <strong>The</strong> means <strong>of</strong> ordinal demographic variables were<br />

compared between test factors and results are shown in Tables 8a and 8b. <strong>The</strong> study<br />

population was mostly adult and mixed sex. Analysis <strong>of</strong> variance showed most<br />

differences related to age <strong>of</strong> the study population: that studies using adapted tests were<br />

least likely to match for age and sex in controls; non<strong>art</strong> behaviourial or content measures<br />

were used with younger subjects as were therapy groups, possibly reflecting recognised<br />

difficulties in the interpretation <strong>of</strong> children's <strong>art</strong> 166 as the lesser success rate for<br />

patient/nonpatient differences indicates; groups in studies using more criterion measures<br />

tended to be older probably reflecting the greater level <strong>of</strong> disability measures given as<br />

standard in this population, medical opinion on these groups and also the longer term <strong>of</strong><br />

their confinement.<br />

Table 9: Frequency and percentage for Measurement Form by No. <strong>of</strong> criterion measures<br />

Diagnosis +<br />

Test = 1<br />

Measures n=31<br />

Freq %<br />

Measurement form<br />

objective comparison<br />

meaning/content<br />

mixed formal/content<br />

non<strong>art</strong>/behaviour<br />

verbal<br />

9 29.0<br />

7 22.6<br />

10 32.3<br />

3 9.7<br />

2 6.5<br />

Diagnosis, test +<br />

1 other = 2<br />

n=9<br />

Freq %<br />

2 22.2<br />

1 11.1<br />

3 33.3<br />

2 22.2<br />

1 11.1<br />

103<br />

Diagnosis + 3 or<br />

more others = 3+<br />

n=11<br />

Freq %<br />

7 63.6<br />

3 27.3<br />

0 0.0<br />

1 9.1<br />

0 0.0<br />

Design <strong>of</strong> test levels by criterion: 31 studies used no other criterion measure than<br />

diagnosis and the study test (Table 9) and these were mostly studies which found<br />

differences (Table 2). <strong>The</strong> comparison <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>work for 1 criterion measure was broadly<br />

166<br />

Glynn V. Thomas and Angele, M. J. Silk (1990) An Introduction to the psychology <strong>of</strong> children's<br />

drawings , Herts: Harvester Wheatsheaf.


spread between subjective and objective scales but with the addition <strong>of</strong> 1 other criterion<br />

measure, the number <strong>of</strong> content measures dropped and behaviour measures increased.<br />

For 3 or more measures (n=11) (which were equally split - difference/no differences),<br />

tests favoured objective comparison <strong>of</strong> countable items (n=7), although there were some<br />

content measures, there were no mixed comparisons. So, the observable and countable<br />

measures tended to use more criterion measures, the mixed formal and content measures<br />

used less criterion measures.<br />

Table 10: Frequency and percentage for test derivation and results by No. <strong>of</strong> judges.<br />

Judges 1 Judge<br />

n=35<br />

Freq %<br />

Test Derivation<br />

developed this study<br />

adapted<br />

observation/clinical<br />

established<br />

Results<br />

diff. pat/nonpat<br />

diff. patient groups<br />

no difference<br />

therapy groups<br />

12 34<br />

6 17<br />

4 11<br />

13 37<br />

13 37<br />

8 23<br />

10 29<br />

4 11<br />

2 Judges<br />

n=9<br />

Freq %<br />

7 78<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

2 22<br />

3 33<br />

2 22<br />

4 44<br />

0 0<br />

104<br />

2+ Judges<br />

n=7<br />

Freq %<br />

4 57<br />

2 29<br />

0 0<br />

1 14<br />

5 71<br />

2 29<br />

0 0<br />

0 0<br />

Test derivation and result levels by judges: <strong>The</strong> vast majority <strong>of</strong> studies (35) used<br />

only one rater and 13 used established tests (Table 10), most <strong>of</strong> which found differences.<br />

Thus, reliability was not established for 22 tests, <strong>of</strong> which most found no differences.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were fewer tests using more raters, but the majority <strong>of</strong> 2-rater tests (n=7; 78%)<br />

were developed for the research; half reported differences. All 7 tests with more raters


eported differences between patients/controls or between subgroups. So, generally,<br />

findings <strong>of</strong> difference increased with the number <strong>of</strong> judges 167 .<br />

Summary: Whether or not studies found differences, they were poorly controlled, but<br />

control was even poorer in studies finding no difference. Generally, all studies used test<br />

items which measured more content than they intended to, but the studies finding<br />

differences used mostly formal or mixed test measures which were consistent with the<br />

aims <strong>of</strong> the study. <strong>The</strong> majority <strong>of</strong> 1 rater tests found differences, but <strong>of</strong> studies which<br />

found differences, more multi-rater tests and more established tests were used, although<br />

they used less criterion measures, more mixed measures than those finding no difference.<br />

<strong>The</strong> studies finding no difference predominantly used instruments which measured<br />

content qualities only, but were usually described as formal or mixed. <strong>The</strong>y used less<br />

raters, but slightly more criterion measures, more content tests, more behaviour tests and<br />

many more self-developed tests (although all result levels had high percentages <strong>of</strong> self-<br />

developed tests). A validity note is that as more criterion measures were added, the use<br />

<strong>of</strong> form-only measures increased and mixed comparisons and content measures decreased.<br />

This probably reflects the rigour <strong>of</strong> the studies.<br />

Validity analysis <strong>of</strong> the literature<br />

Tests designed for study: Very little evidence has yet been produced that characteristic<br />

symptoms, which, when associated, lead to diagnostic categorisation <strong>of</strong> psychiatric status<br />

167 If we assume that established tests have already been reliability tested.<br />

105


are linked to those <strong>art</strong>istic characteristics which are interpreted by <strong>art</strong> therapists. Many<br />

<strong>of</strong> the studies reviewed here developed their own tests for the study using <strong>art</strong>istic<br />

characteristics which were said to be equivalent to the behaviourial symptomatic<br />

characteristics used for diagnosis. <strong>The</strong> only criterion used in many cases was how the<br />

experimenters thought the visual sequelae <strong>of</strong> thought disorder should look, supported by<br />

previous examples <strong>of</strong> the same process. This is not enough. It is unsound to conclude<br />

that paintings are or are not diagnostically valid if there is no evidence that the test items<br />

are a) reliable themselves or b) measure any symptomatic behaviour and c) relate to visual<br />

output. <strong>The</strong> DDS team 168 have argued that they cannot produce a validity index since<br />

there is yet no comparable instrument with the DDS, but there are many other<br />

established tests measuring diagnostic and other qualities which could provide non-visual<br />

indices for DDS correlations, and which so far have not been used. Indeed, the DDS itself<br />

has not even produced good correlations with its only criterion, diagnosis itself 169 ,<br />

although it has produced a reliable format. Adapted tests too, must provide a criterion<br />

measure, so that it is certain that aspects which have been deleted are not integral to the<br />

validity <strong>of</strong> the test. <strong>The</strong> first validity question therefore must be: are these assessment<br />

measures really measuring what they are designed to measure?<br />

Even where obvious and relatively consistent phenomena are reported, what is<br />

measured may be a confounding variable, something which accompanies the symptom,<br />

168 Mills et. al. (1993) Reliability and validity tests <strong>of</strong> the Diagnostic Drawing Series,<br />

Psychotherapy , V.20:83-88.<br />

106<br />

Arts in<br />

169<br />

See my critique later in Chapter 2, reliability analysis. DDS produced by B. Cohen, J. Hammer and<br />

S. Singer (1988) <strong>The</strong> Diagnostic Drawing Series: a systematic approach to <strong>art</strong> therapy evaluation and<br />

research, Arts in Ps ychotherapy , V.15(1): 11-21. Although to be fair, the DDS is head and shoulders<br />

above the competition, and further research on the statistics would produce a more suitable analysis.


ather than the symptom itself, thus unreliable, since it may appear in other<br />

circumstances 170 . <strong>The</strong> validity <strong>of</strong> a test is proved when it correlates highly with another<br />

proven method which measures similar elements.<br />

Validity Analysis <strong>of</strong> Controlled Studies<br />

70 studies were included in the analysis: all the controlled studies were included (51), 12<br />

studies were added from the literature which dealt specifically with criterion validity and<br />

7 studies from the literature on reliability 171 . 33 (47%) studies used an <strong>art</strong> test simply<br />

against the diagnosis and 37 (53%) used the diagnosis and another criterion. <strong>The</strong> criterion<br />

which was primary in the discussion was used for this review: 6 studies used a self<br />

report; 1 study used a test with both content and formal evaluations; 27 (39%) used a<br />

nonprojective measure <strong>of</strong> ability or IQ; and 3 used a verbal evaluation. <strong>The</strong> tables describe<br />

5 validity characteristics from each <strong>of</strong> 70 studies 172 .<br />

170<br />

For example, R. Langevin, and L.M. Hutchins (1973) found that judgement <strong>of</strong> patient status correlated<br />

higher with quality <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong>work than diagnosis, even when judges were prewarned what to expect (An<br />

experimental investigation <strong>of</strong> judges ratings <strong>of</strong> schizophrenic and non-schizophrenic paintings, J.<br />

Personality Assessment , V.37(6):537-543).<br />

171 All studies are detailed in the Table <strong>of</strong> Authorities (Appendix 3). <strong>Study</strong> numbers for those which<br />

appeared in the controlled analysis remain the same.<br />

172 A full list <strong>of</strong> alternatives appears in Appendix 1.<br />

107


3 Demographic variables were used from the controlled analysis:<br />

sex <strong>of</strong> subject;<br />

age, whether child/adolescent (under 18), adult (18-60), older adult (60+);<br />

diagnosis <strong>of</strong> the main experimental group;<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were 3 study factors:<br />

<strong>art</strong>test form, what form <strong>of</strong> information did the <strong>art</strong> test collect;<br />

criterion form, what form <strong>of</strong> information did the criterion collect;<br />

no. <strong>of</strong> criterion measures used;<br />

Sensitivity <strong>of</strong> the test and results.<br />

comparison, what type <strong>of</strong> qualities were the instruments being compared on;<br />

the results <strong>of</strong> the study: did the <strong>art</strong> test results correlate or significantly agree<br />

with the other criterion test.<br />

One alternative was marked for each category. Tables 1 and 2 show frequency and<br />

percentages <strong>of</strong> study factors for all studies, and are divided between studies showing<br />

association or none between the main <strong>art</strong> test and the criterion. <strong>The</strong> mean ranks for<br />

study factors between the two result levels were subjected to a non-parametric test <strong>of</strong><br />

association, and the results are presented in Table 3.<br />

108


Table 1. Frequency and percentage <strong>of</strong> criterion variables for 70 studies by result<br />

Variables<br />

No. criterion<br />

measures<br />

0<br />

1<br />

2<br />

3<br />

4<br />

15<br />

Comparison<br />

cognitive<br />

emotion<br />

development<br />

diagnosis<br />

Criterion form<br />

diagnosis<br />

self report/picture<br />

mix <strong>of</strong> content/form<br />

behaviourial/IQ<br />

verbal<br />

Art-test form<br />

KFD<br />

DAP<br />

Formal other<br />

copy<br />

other theme<br />

free<br />

all cases Result 1:<br />

associatio<br />

n<br />

n=70<br />

freq %<br />

33 47.1<br />

13 18.6<br />

12 17.1<br />

9 12.9<br />

2 2.9<br />

1 1.4<br />

11 15.7<br />

12 17.1<br />

9 12.9<br />

37 52.9<br />

33 47.1<br />

6 8.6<br />

1 1.4<br />

27 38.6<br />

3 4.3<br />

6 8.6<br />

24 34.3<br />

3 4.3<br />

5 7.1<br />

16 22.9<br />

16 22.9<br />

n=42<br />

freq %<br />

26 61.9<br />

6 14.3<br />

4 9.5<br />

3 7.1<br />

2 4.8<br />

1 2.4<br />

5 11.9<br />

8 19.1<br />

2 4.8<br />

27 64.3<br />

26 61.9<br />

2 4.8<br />

1 2.4<br />

12 28.6<br />

1 2.4<br />

3 7.1<br />

11 26.2<br />

1 2.4<br />

4 9.5<br />

12 28.6<br />

11 26.2<br />

109<br />

Result 2:no<br />

association<br />

n=28<br />

freq %<br />

Result 1: significant association <strong>of</strong> measure with criterion test.<br />

Result 2: no association <strong>of</strong> measure with criterion test<br />

7 25<br />

7 25<br />

8 28.6<br />

6 21.4<br />

0<br />

0<br />

6 21.4<br />

5 17.9<br />

4 25<br />

5 35.7<br />

7 25<br />

4 14.3<br />

0<br />

15 53.6<br />

2 7.1<br />

3 10.7<br />

13 46.4<br />

2 7.1<br />

1 3.6<br />

4 14.3<br />

5 17.9<br />

Most <strong>of</strong> the criterion tests used measured diagnosis or IQ. Diagnostic criteria were<br />

compared with a mix <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> tests (DAP 24%, free 30%, other theme 36%); thematic were<br />

more popular and they were always compared on diagnosis. Achievement tests were<br />

popularly compared with the Draw-a-person protocol (44%) but were compared equally<br />

on cognition (30%) and development (30%), and a smaller percentage but equal<br />

distribution <strong>of</strong> emotional (18.5%) and diagnostic functions (18.5%).


Table 2. Diagnostic groups for 70 studies with criterion measures. Frequency and<br />

percentage by levels <strong>of</strong> result.<br />

Result 1: sig.<br />

association<br />

Diagnosis, condition 1 n=42<br />

freq %<br />

schizophrenia<br />

emotional trauma<br />

drug/alcohol<br />

psychotic phobia<br />

neurological damage<br />

depression<br />

conduct disorder<br />

retarded<br />

gender disorder<br />

personality disorder<br />

mixed patients<br />

11 26.2<br />

5 11.9<br />

1 2.4<br />

2 4.8<br />

5 11.9<br />

4 9.5<br />

1 2.4<br />

3 7.1<br />

0 0.0<br />

1 2.4<br />

8 19.0<br />

Result 2: no<br />

association<br />

n=28<br />

freq %<br />

7 25.0<br />

2 7.1<br />

0 0.0<br />

1 3.6<br />

2 7.1<br />

2 7.1<br />

1 3.6<br />

2 7.1<br />

1 3.6<br />

0 0.0<br />

10 35.7<br />

Result 1: significant association <strong>of</strong> measure with criterion test.<br />

Result 2: no association <strong>of</strong> measure with criterion test<br />

Table 3. Non-Parametric test to show differences between study factors for findings <strong>of</strong><br />

association and non-association <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>-test with criterion measure. Mann-Whitney U -<br />

Wilcoxian Rank Sum Test.<br />

<strong>Study</strong> variables Mean Rank for<br />

association <strong>of</strong><br />

test- criterion<br />

n=42<br />

Age group<br />

Diagnosis<br />

Criterion form<br />

No. <strong>of</strong> criterion<br />

measures<br />

Art-test form<br />

Comparison<br />

36.58<br />

33.13<br />

30.29<br />

30.67<br />

39.32<br />

39.33<br />

Mean rank for no<br />

association <strong>of</strong><br />

test - criterion<br />

n=28<br />

33.88<br />

39.05<br />

43.32<br />

42.75<br />

29.77<br />

29.75<br />

110<br />

Z scores Significance<br />

level.<br />

2-tailed p<br />

value.<br />

-0.6545<br />

-1.2151<br />

-2.8692<br />

-2.5916<br />

-1.9900<br />

-2.1027<br />

0.5128<br />

0.2243<br />

0.0041<br />

0.0096<br />

0.0466<br />

0.0355<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were no biases in demographic variables <strong>of</strong> age and diagnosis for studies which<br />

showed association or not. <strong>The</strong> majority <strong>of</strong> studies which showed associations between<br />

the <strong>art</strong> test and the criterion measure did not employ other criteria than the clinical<br />

diagnosis and used a mixture <strong>of</strong> test methods, whereas the tests which found no<br />

associations used predominantly cognitive criteria (IQ or achievement tests), were equally<br />

divided between 0-3 other measures than diagnosis and used primarily formal test


measurements, favouring the Draw-a-person protocol. <strong>The</strong> comparison was<br />

predominantly developmental, which category was absent from the tests which showed<br />

associations; cognitive measures were also popular and least popular was emotion, which<br />

was most popular in the test which showed associations.<br />

Summary: <strong>The</strong>re were many more tests which showed associations with the criterion<br />

measure, but almost half the tests employed no other criteria than clinical diagnosis.<br />

However, the relationship <strong>of</strong> the diagnostic criterion to the <strong>art</strong> test was consistent with<br />

the comparison made by the study (on diagnosis), whereas the majority <strong>of</strong> the measures<br />

which employed another measure were achievement tests and their comparison was <strong>of</strong>ten<br />

not consistent with the orientation <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong> test (37% compared achievement tests on<br />

diagnostic or emotional criteria). If the tests using only the diagnostic criteria were set<br />

aside, there are 2 points to note:<br />

(1) Tests which compared developmental qualities in drawings showed no association<br />

with criterion measures; and<br />

(2) Tests which compared emotional qualities tended to show association with<br />

criterion measures.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were equal numbers <strong>of</strong> studies on both sides for tests which compare cognitive<br />

aspects. <strong>The</strong>se results confirm the recognised doubts about the validity <strong>of</strong> painting or<br />

drawing tests as developmental measures for psychiatric populations discussed in<br />

Chapter 1, and so their predictions <strong>of</strong> cognitive function and use in place <strong>of</strong> IQ tests.<br />

111


However, there were few tests which actually provided a suitable criterion for emotional<br />

qualities.<br />

Conclusion: For the controlled studies, the derivation <strong>of</strong> the measure had an influence<br />

on the control <strong>of</strong> the study as did poor control on the result levels <strong>of</strong> no difference. Type<br />

<strong>of</strong> treatment (orientation) and number <strong>of</strong> criterion measures varied with the age <strong>of</strong><br />

subjects. <strong>The</strong> interaction <strong>of</strong> demographic variables with levels <strong>of</strong> result together with the<br />

failure to match or describe experimental samples in many studies and the confusion <strong>of</strong><br />

comparisons with criterion measures show the substantial quandary <strong>of</strong> treating this<br />

literature as a whole. <strong>The</strong> advantage <strong>of</strong> examining a number <strong>of</strong> studies is that the general<br />

direction <strong>of</strong> the majority overrides the diversity <strong>of</strong> confounding variables, but<br />

developmental or cognitive criterion measures are inappropriate. This preliminary<br />

overview has accomplished its purpose in that it has shown, generally, differences<br />

predominate over no difference findings, and that diagnostic and emotional dimensions<br />

rather than developmental and cognitive dimensions are valid directions for further<br />

investigation. Further investigation, especially for characteristics which relate the <strong>art</strong> to<br />

symptomatology must use studies which conform to common research criteria and which<br />

minimise confounding variables. Comparable studies are those which use matched control<br />

groups, at least on age and sex, a reliability index (if using a test which is not established,<br />

if the rater is untrained or if there are mainly content variables).<br />

112


Reliability Analysis<br />

Judgement <strong>of</strong> characteristics differentiating patient groups<br />

All rating scales have limitations, especially those involving human subjects. Given<br />

enough different people performing a measurement, individual differences will contribute<br />

to error, but a single rater may be inconsistent, or may be scoring on other criteria than<br />

the published instrument. If a measure is unreliable, there is no possible way that any<br />

sort <strong>of</strong> statistically significant relationship or difference with that measure can be<br />

documented. It makes little sense therefore to list the characteristics for every study in<br />

the controlled and case analyses unless they have established inter-rater reliability so that<br />

it is certain their scoring is consistent and their definitions <strong>of</strong> terms unambiguous.<br />

Terms used in this analysis <strong>of</strong> studies<br />

Most studies have some counted or presence/absence scales, but there is a wide range <strong>of</strong><br />

opinion on what is described as objective or formal qualities. Here, formal qualities are<br />

broadly differentiated from objective qualities as relating to the structure <strong>of</strong> the picture;<br />

how it is made, rather than why it is made or what it represents 173 . Objective decisions<br />

can be made about elements <strong>of</strong> form and content; although a characteristic can be both<br />

formal and objective, it cannot be formal and subjective. For this review, objective<br />

categories may be considered as observable dimensions; for example: the presence or<br />

absence <strong>of</strong> some element, countable items, differentiation <strong>of</strong> structural aspects or<br />

elements <strong>of</strong> the picture, such as lines, shapes and colours. Objective dimensions also<br />

173 This form description is broader than the formal description for the DAPA, which appears in Chapter<br />

3. It is clearly not reasonable to apply criteria to studies which are not aimed at that point.<br />

113


include recognisable or identifiable objects/persons (but do not attribute meaning to the<br />

image or compare it with an internal model). <strong>The</strong>se objective categories can be<br />

differentiated from subjective categories in that they do not require interpretation <strong>of</strong> the<br />

image (fitting a meaning to the marks or images), do not require judgements <strong>of</strong> relations<br />

between images or opinions on such personal judgements as aesthetic quality or<br />

emotional associations. E.g. to note the presence <strong>of</strong> decoration, a subject must be<br />

reconstructed from the marks on the paper and compared with what the rater thinks is<br />

essential to structure: this is an opinion. On the other hand, phrasing can be misleading:<br />

rating criteria demanding presence or absence <strong>of</strong> a symbol does not require an objective<br />

but a subjective decision, because it requires the opinion <strong>of</strong> the rater on the intentions <strong>of</strong><br />

the <strong>art</strong>ist. Many <strong>of</strong> the tests here had global elements for which operational definitions<br />

cannot be considered reliable for a single rater.<br />

Selection: To minimise confounding variables, the studies which were to be included in<br />

the reliability assessment were those:<br />

1. Which actually measured <strong>art</strong> variables from the pictures (4 studies <strong>of</strong> the benefits<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy were excluded because the tests did not measure attributes <strong>of</strong> the<br />

picture; they used self reports or behaviourial assessment. 3 other non-<strong>art</strong><br />

studies were excluded: <strong>Study</strong> 71, which used the TAT Make a Picture Story Test<br />

protocol but assessed the verbal explanations <strong>of</strong> the patients; <strong>Study</strong> 62 and 15,<br />

which used rigid copy accuracy measures.<br />

2. Which used more than one rater if the rater was not trained for an established test<br />

or if the test used mainly content variables.<br />

114


3. Which assessed agreement between the raters: 2 studies were excluded, because<br />

although they used multiple raters, they did not assess their agreement but<br />

compared the judgements with diagnosis directly (including the most famous and<br />

<strong>of</strong>ten quoted series <strong>of</strong> studies <strong>of</strong> judgements by Levy and Ulman 174 , who used<br />

intra-rater measures, every judge being their own control), .<br />

18 studies survived these strictures, 13 <strong>of</strong> which had 2 or more raters from the controlled<br />

analysis, the majority using a test developed for the study. 5 studies used one rater and<br />

claimed to use a previously established test; they will be discussed first.<br />

Established tests with 1 rater - further exclusions<br />

<strong>The</strong>se studies claimed to use recognised tests, but examination reveals they used <strong>art</strong><br />

directives, adding their own scoring and therefore consistency was needed. <strong>Study</strong> 77<br />

found differences using the protocol for a projective tree drawing test with elderly<br />

schizophrenics, demented and control subjects on the basis <strong>of</strong> proven reliability, but did<br />

not reference this statement and used their own interpretive system, which was not<br />

subjected to reliability analysis. <strong>Study</strong> 29 found no significant differences in the size <strong>of</strong><br />

figures drawn by depressed patients and controls using a Draw-a-Person protocol, but<br />

this is only one element in the DAP scale and the method should be tested independently<br />

for reliability.<br />

Two studies used tests <strong>of</strong> emotional indicators: <strong>Study</strong> 70 used the DAPQ (Karp<br />

1990, unpublished) to find more incest markers in the drawings <strong>of</strong> abused than control<br />

women and different treatment <strong>of</strong> sexes. <strong>Study</strong> 55 found no differences in development<br />

174<br />

B.I. Levy and E. Ulman (1974) <strong>The</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> training on judgement <strong>of</strong> <strong>psychopathology</strong> from paintings,<br />

Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.14:24-5 (study 43).<br />

115


and emotional indicators in drawings from schizophrenic mothers than from control<br />

mothers using a Draw-a-Man test (unspecified) but 5 significant differences between the<br />

children.<br />

<strong>The</strong> study <strong>of</strong> emotional indicators as symptoms <strong>of</strong> pathology have mostly been<br />

done on drawings <strong>of</strong> children. <strong>The</strong>ir clinical utility is low as indicators are rare but their<br />

use as danger markers is recognised 175 . <strong>The</strong>re is no evidence to show these studies may<br />

generalise to adults.<br />

<strong>Study</strong> 34 used the DDS 176 to compare 81 eating disordered patients with an<br />

undescribed control sample, collected and rated by Cohen 6 years earlier. It is most<br />

undesirable for the two ratings being compared to be carried out by different observers 177 .<br />

Any systematic variation between observers would have been inseparable from any<br />

difference between groups. <strong>The</strong> original 1988 study is also not included since the DDS<br />

team have not been able to supply basic information such as numbers <strong>of</strong> controls and <strong>of</strong><br />

patient groups used in the study 178 .<br />

<strong>The</strong> reliability <strong>of</strong> the measures in these five studies is clearly more questionable<br />

than in studies which used their own tests and two or more raters. <strong>The</strong>refore, from the<br />

175<br />

Maralyn M. Trowbridge (1995) Graphic indicators <strong>of</strong> sexual abuse in children's drawings: a review <strong>of</strong><br />

the literature, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.22(5):405-93.<br />

176 Cohen et al (1988) op.cit.<br />

177<br />

It is included in the analysis <strong>of</strong> controlled studies, but not here as it did not include a separate reliability<br />

test.<br />

178 Despite repeated personal communications over 4 years: 1994, 1996 and 1998. <strong>The</strong> original paper states<br />

that further information is available, but from private letters and other communication the team cannot<br />

comment on the statistical basis <strong>of</strong> the paper. <strong>The</strong>ir reliability study, however, reported fully in 1993 was<br />

included.<br />

116


main analysis <strong>of</strong> 51 controlled studies, only those 13 studies whose characteristics were<br />

rated by more than one rater were included in the reliability analysis.<br />

<strong>The</strong> exclusion <strong>of</strong> the DDS<br />

<strong>The</strong> Diagnostic Drawing Series, published and peer reviewed in the <strong>art</strong>-therapy press 179<br />

is currently the most well known contemporary <strong>art</strong> therapy assessment instrument in<br />

America. It has repeatedly claimed to be the only <strong>art</strong> assessment method which has been<br />

reliably tested, validated and has demonstrated reliable differences between diagnostic<br />

groups. It uses 23 categories, most <strong>of</strong> which would be considered here as a more<br />

objective form <strong>of</strong> content analysis but half <strong>of</strong> which are claimed by the authors to be<br />

formal measures. <strong>The</strong> DDS is a useful therapeutic tool in that it <strong>of</strong>fers a standardised and<br />

structured format for assessment which is acceptable to a therapeutic milieu. Three<br />

pictures are required, a person, a tree and a free picture. <strong>The</strong> reliability <strong>of</strong> the categories<br />

has been demonstrated as good between 2 or 3 raters, although their methods <strong>of</strong><br />

calculating reliability are not clear and one study found that only 6 out <strong>of</strong> 23 categories<br />

showed good reliability as calculated by the Kappa statistic between 2 raters, which gives<br />

room for questions 180 . To ignore this major study could be seen as serious neglect, so I<br />

will give my reasons for leaving out this study at length.<br />

Are the claims for the DDS valid?<br />

179<br />

Cohen, Hammer and Singer (1988) op.cit.; also Art <strong>The</strong>rapy No. 15 1996, passim. <strong>The</strong> DDS appears<br />

all through this issue as the premier assessment method for <strong>art</strong> therapy and research. It is repeatedly<br />

claimed to have demonstrated clinically reliable differences between patient groups.<br />

117


<strong>The</strong> DDS (1988) study was an exploratory study which described an evaluation<br />

procedure applied to the pictures <strong>of</strong> 3 diagnostic groups <strong>of</strong> patients: Dysthymia,<br />

Depression, Schizophrenia, and one non-patient group within a population size <strong>of</strong> 239.<br />

In order to evaluate it properly, it would be necessary to know the sizes <strong>of</strong> the sample<br />

groups, since the detection <strong>of</strong> 100% accuracy in one sample may refer to a different<br />

number than another group. <strong>The</strong>re were indications within the text that the control group<br />

was in some way abnormal or small.<br />

<strong>The</strong> instrument itself rated up to 23 categories for each <strong>of</strong> three pictures by each<br />

patient. Some categories were reduced to 2 binary variables, and some with 3 or more<br />

choices were reduced in some other way that was not explained. <strong>The</strong> text indicated that<br />

there were then 36 variables in the end for each picture giving a patient pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong> up to<br />

108 variables between 3 pictures, treating the repeated measures as independent. This<br />

procedure was not well explained and their illustration form did not employ either 23<br />

categories or 36 variables. Furthermore, if the extra variables were included as was<br />

indicated on the results tables, there would have been more than 108 variables. Despite<br />

repeated enquiries, none <strong>of</strong> these concerns have been addressed by the authors.<br />

4 multiple-regression analyses were calculated, one for each group against the<br />

total population. That for an analysis to have 108 variables was unsatisfactory because<br />

<strong>of</strong> the sample size required was recognised by Cohen in the paper. <strong>The</strong> variables were<br />

rotated by some unspecified procedure but it is a mystery why they used the variables<br />

that appear in the resultant tables, which were supposed to compare between diagnostic<br />

180<br />

E.L. Neale (1994) <strong>The</strong> Children's DDS, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.11(2):119-126.<br />

118


groups, rather than any other. Each table used a different set <strong>of</strong> variables so they were<br />

difficult to compare. In addition, the variables were treated as though they were<br />

completely unrelated, but there must have been a high degree <strong>of</strong> multicollinearity (most<br />

<strong>of</strong> the variables must be related, especially those which are derived from the same<br />

category and those which measure different pictures using the same variable) and most<br />

would not contribute anything extra to that <strong>of</strong> the main differentiator, which means the<br />

co-efficient presented cannot really be interpreted as an effect.<br />

Cohen quotes the F-test as a measure that the combination <strong>of</strong> predictors does<br />

better than chance, meaning that some element in the equation differentiates the<br />

dependent variable (patient or control). However, this does not mean that it is effective.<br />

All the ANOVA tells us is that at least one characteristic is good. <strong>The</strong>re is evidence from<br />

some <strong>of</strong> the t-tests that the co-efficient <strong>of</strong> the variable is not 0, but even ignoring the<br />

method, looking at the coefficients as effect sizes 181 , the Dysthymia group shows an<br />

almost negligible effect (-0.08 to 0.13); the Depressive results and Controls are similar<br />

(depressed 0.13-0.17; controls 0.13-0.28), the strongest are water scenes for both<br />

(depressed 0.37 and control 0.36 182 ), which is not mentioned in the discussion <strong>of</strong> the<br />

results; only Schizophrenia shows a high effect on one variable, minimal trunk (0.63, but<br />

otherwise 0.16-0.29).<br />

I therefore disagree with most <strong>of</strong> the statements in the discussion <strong>of</strong> significant<br />

characteristics which are supposed to identify diagnostic groups, which include some<br />

181<br />

<strong>The</strong> coefficients could be interpreted as effect sizes if the variables were independent in the same way<br />

as the t-statistics.<br />

182 But the t-statistic has a significance <strong>of</strong> p=0.07 which does not indicate exclusivity.<br />

119


which were not significant - and I would totally disagree with the assumption that<br />

negative coefficients implied another characteristic was a strong indicator <strong>of</strong><br />

predictability, especially when the variable itself was tested, but not included: eg. "a<br />

striking feature <strong>of</strong> dysthymia was light pressure" (indicated by a negative coefficient on<br />

medium and heavy pressure). <strong>The</strong> t-values for heavy pressure were greater than one in<br />

both picture A and B, which merited some discussion, but surely this indicated heavy<br />

pressure was not applied, not that light pressure was applied! One <strong>of</strong> the study variables<br />

was light pressure - why was it not included in the results? Similarly monochrome as a<br />

significant characteristic is inferred from the negative correlations on multicoloured<br />

pictures from schizophrenic patients. Monochrome was one <strong>of</strong> the study variables, why<br />

was it not included in the schizophrenia results if it was a significant characteristic? 183<br />

It is obvious the technique <strong>of</strong> multiple regression is not appropriate for this<br />

analysis, as reflected in the low levels <strong>of</strong> the multiple r-squared statistic 184 and does not<br />

do justice to the clinical merit <strong>of</strong> the DDS.<br />

When the DAPA was being developed, the DDS was already rather famous.<br />

Over 10 years, it has developed a 4 page resources list 185 . I contacted the DDS team and<br />

183 <strong>The</strong> DDS team have declined to answer any <strong>of</strong> these serious criticisms. <strong>The</strong>y have also ignored my<br />

requests for clarification <strong>of</strong> their methods, since they are no longer in contact with their statistician. But<br />

the DDS team are still encouraging <strong>art</strong> therapists to use this measure and bring in new work (DDS<br />

Newsletter Dec. 1998).<br />

184 Multiple r 2 measures the fit <strong>of</strong> the model to the information provided - the closer to 1 the better. A<br />

good fit would typically be 0.7-0.99. <strong>The</strong> DDS results were: Dysthymia 0.15; Depression 0.10:<br />

Schizophrenia 0.22; Control 0.44. <strong>The</strong> control shows the best fit and the t-statistics are significant on<br />

more variables, but very weak. However, if the numbers in the group are very different - as is hinted in<br />

the paper, this might have a confounding effect.<br />

185<br />

DDS Resource List (1988) unpublished. 64 items mostly unpublished in cassette form. All published<br />

material is discussed in this thesis.<br />

120


invited them to provide substantiation for their claims since their original 1988 published<br />

study was uninterpretable for this review. Despite this study being ten years old none<br />

<strong>of</strong> these criticisms had been put to the DDS before 186 , which shows the statistical naivety<br />

<strong>of</strong> their readership. <strong>The</strong>ir whole output <strong>of</strong> papers and other resources are based on the<br />

1988 study. <strong>The</strong> DDS team could not provide me with a better estimate <strong>of</strong> the<br />

effectiveness <strong>of</strong> the DDS.<br />

Tabulation <strong>of</strong> studies for reliability and discriminant characteristics<br />

Firstly, each study had different combinations <strong>of</strong> reliable variables and different systems<br />

<strong>of</strong> combining these variables for analysis. 28 studies were included in the analysis. 18<br />

studies (which included 13 from the controlled analysis) were taken from the validity<br />

analysis <strong>of</strong> 70 criterion measures, 3 were series uncontrolled studies (examined in the<br />

analysis <strong>of</strong> 163 case studies, Chapter 2). <strong>The</strong> remaining 7 studies were not discussed<br />

elsewhere: 3 were studies which examined judges predictions <strong>of</strong> patient status from their<br />

own criteria, 2 were related studies which examined judge reliability on interpreting self-<br />

images from paintings and 2 were specific reliability studies for the same test (the DDS).<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were three more controlled replications <strong>of</strong> studies by the same authors, all <strong>of</strong><br />

whose results cast doubt on their initial acceptable reliability. <strong>Study</strong> 9 was succeeded by<br />

study 10; study 37 was succeeded by study 38; and study 58 was succeeded by study<br />

59. Only the later studies are considered in the breakdown <strong>of</strong> elements. Those studies<br />

which are not marked unreliable and do not show discriminant characteristics are either<br />

186 Private communication, Anne Mills and S. Hacking 1998.<br />

121


eliability studies or studies which use comparisons <strong>of</strong> total scores rather than isolated<br />

elements. <strong>The</strong> total score comparisons are entered separately into the table as composite<br />

variables.<br />

In order to compare studies it was necessary to identify commonalities in<br />

variables between studies and classify them into categories. <strong>The</strong>se categories were<br />

tabulated into 14 different areas <strong>of</strong> drawing analysis. <strong>The</strong> tables show the original terms<br />

as far as possible. Terms are usually defined by the studies, and some produce booklets<br />

<strong>of</strong> rating criteria, but they do not exactly compare with each other. <strong>The</strong>se definitions are<br />

not reproduced as each is individual; a list would be lengthy, require specific vocabulary<br />

in some cases and serve little purpose. Different studies recorded similar elements,<br />

globally and through combinations <strong>of</strong> details; I have tried to reflect this in the text. <strong>The</strong>re<br />

is little information on unreliable categories, as they tend to be barely mentioned and do<br />

not appear on the variable lists, but those mentioned are presented in the tables, although<br />

these are much fewer than in reality. Reliability is presented according to the author's<br />

own evaluations, since this is a clinical decision, unless otherwise specified, as most<br />

present only their final statistic. Some reliabilities are quoted by their authors only as<br />

'better than chance' or 'acceptable', without figures. <strong>The</strong>se were included here, but not in<br />

the meta-analysis. Reliability tests are given by type, a key to which appears under<br />

every table. A discussion <strong>of</strong> the limitations <strong>of</strong> the reliability statistics appears separately<br />

at the end <strong>of</strong> the section. Discrimination analyses were simpler and test details are not<br />

presented. Studies here present a more objective approach than is usual in this literature.<br />

122


Studies which included reliability tests from controlled and uncontrolled analyses<br />

<strong>of</strong> literature on <strong>art</strong> tests 1974-1996<br />

Studies showing reliability only do not test discriminant properties.<br />

Divisions are for subcategories <strong>of</strong> like variables by heading or by 1st variable in<br />

bold; (o) objective (s) subjective (c) content (f) form. No.s are study No.s from<br />

Table <strong>of</strong> Authorities (Appendix 3), No.s below headings denote same term<br />

different study.<br />

Variables tested individually for reliability scores in bold, others are reliabilities<br />

for whole test.<br />

123


<strong>The</strong>matic variables<br />

Table 1: reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong> theme.<br />

<strong>The</strong>matic variables (<strong>Study</strong> No.)<br />

(o) objective (s) subjective (c) content (f)<br />

form<br />

presence <strong>of</strong> main image (48) (c) (o)<br />

(49)<br />

absence <strong>of</strong> focal configuration (56)<br />

unintelligible essential (36)<br />

omission <strong>of</strong> essential (36)<br />

presence <strong>of</strong> named elements (c) (o)<br />

house (56)<br />

1 dwelling or building, 2, 2+, estate (46)<br />

people present (48)<br />

(46)<br />

(21)<br />

(49)<br />

(3)<br />

animals present (48)<br />

(46)<br />

(49)<br />

tree present (48)<br />

(49)<br />

water landscape (48)<br />

(49)<br />

inanimate object (48) (s)<br />

(49)<br />

(33)<br />

% other<br />

ag't test<br />

(1-6) rel'y 1<br />

99<br />

77<br />

4 high<br />

90 5 .915<br />

90 5 .915<br />

4 high<br />

96<br />

100<br />

96<br />

3 .90<br />

77<br />

3 .96<br />

99<br />

96<br />

77<br />

96<br />

77<br />

93<br />

77<br />

124<br />

discriminates patient<br />

/nonpatient control<br />

(nf=no figures)<br />

NS<br />

(nf) Alzheimer's<br />

nf<br />

NS<br />

Organic**<br />

discriminates subgroups <strong>of</strong><br />

patients<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

Depression/ Organic**<br />

depressed p.d. **<br />

97<br />

77<br />

94 4 .96 severe mental retd'n **;<br />

acute/chronic psychosis*<br />

1 - reliability tests: 1 = % agreement; 2 = product moment correlation; 3 = correlation coefficient; 4 = association test Chi<br />

sq. or T; 5 = Kappa; 6 = Anova.<br />

** p


Discrimination: From 26 reliable variables, 8 were tested for discrimination properties<br />

by 5 studies; there were 4 differences. <strong>The</strong>re were no significant differences between<br />

schizophrenics and controls on absence <strong>of</strong> focal configuration (56). A similar variable,<br />

omission <strong>of</strong> essentials, was identified as a strongly significant and frequent predictor <strong>of</strong><br />

Alzheimer's disease against normal controls (36) and was distinct from unintelligibility,<br />

in which there was no difference in the same patient group. Between presence <strong>of</strong> named<br />

elements, people present were drawn more frequently by normal controls than patients<br />

with both depression and organic brain damage (21; this study probably lacks power as<br />

the numbers are very small) and this result is p<strong>art</strong>ly supported by similar findings from<br />

a study <strong>of</strong> depressed patients with a main diagnosis <strong>of</strong> personality disorder (3, but no<br />

subgroup numbers were given). <strong>The</strong> findings <strong>of</strong> study 46, that schizophrenics and other<br />

patients scored equally on countable named elements in pictures also support this<br />

differential score for depression. <strong>The</strong>re were differences between different p<strong>art</strong>itions <strong>of</strong><br />

schizophrenic patient groups from study 33, which distinguished duration <strong>of</strong> psychosis<br />

as long or short and severe from other grades <strong>of</strong> mental retardation (33), in the use <strong>of</strong><br />

inanimate objects, but neither compensation, nor power calculations were made for the<br />

process <strong>of</strong> multiple testing and unequal numbers.<br />

Summary: Objective thematic evaluation <strong>of</strong> content is reliable. Normal controls may<br />

draw more people than patients and Alzheimer's patients may omit more essential<br />

elements than both controls and patients. Use <strong>of</strong> inanimate objects may distinguish<br />

125


etween grades <strong>of</strong> the same diagnosis, p<strong>art</strong>icularly in retardation, but the evidence is very<br />

weak for all suggestions.<br />

Content<br />

Table 2: reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong> content.<br />

content variables (study No.)<br />

(o) objective (s) subjective (c) content<br />

(f) form<br />

paucity content (36) (s)<br />

general poor content (36, composite v)<br />

bizarre content (38) (s)<br />

(36)<br />

(33)<br />

incongruity (36)<br />

fused/hybrid image (36)<br />

morbid content (36) (s)<br />

hopelessness (75)<br />

suspicion (36)<br />

masklike drawing (33)<br />

personal content (s)<br />

expressive (44)<br />

(36)<br />

symbols (38)<br />

original (38)<br />

problem solving (21)<br />

identification <strong>of</strong> themes (50) (s)<br />

continuity <strong>of</strong> themes (50)<br />

several ideas (38)<br />

related ideas (38)<br />

interest self (75) (s)<br />

interest family (75)<br />

% other<br />

ag't test 1<br />

(1-6) rel'y<br />

126<br />

discriminates patient/<br />

nonpatient control<br />

(nf = no figures)<br />

90 5 .915 Alzheimer's freq.<br />

(nf)<br />

Alzh'rs **<br />

2 NS 90<br />

5 .915<br />

70 4 .566<br />

90 5 .915<br />

90 5 .915<br />

90 5 .915<br />

2 .94<br />

90 5 .915<br />

NS 4 NS<br />

3 .63<br />

90 5 .915<br />

2 NS<br />

2 > .60<br />

3 .92<br />

own criteria NS<br />

own criteria<br />

NS 2<br />

NS 2<br />

>.60<br />

2 .94<br />

2 .94<br />

Alzheimer's freq<br />

(nf)<br />

nf<br />

nf<br />

nf<br />

depressed **<br />

nf<br />

nf<br />

NS<br />

patients **<br />

NS<br />

depressed **<br />

depressed **<br />

discriminates subgroups <strong>of</strong> patients<br />

paranoid from non-p schizophrenia*;<br />

mild from other retarded<br />

schizophrenics**; retarded from nonr<br />

schizophrenics*<br />

schiz. coping techniques*<br />

depressed **<br />

1 - reliability tests: 1 = % agreement; 2 = product moment correlation; 3 = correlation coefficient; 4 = association test Chi<br />

sq. or T; 5 = Kappa; 6 = Anova.<br />

** p


implied contradictions between studies measuring similar variables. <strong>The</strong> studies using<br />

overall scores showed high agreement (mostly over 90%), although they were results <strong>of</strong><br />

5 different types <strong>of</strong> measurement and so, difficult to compare. Reliable subcategories<br />

were: paucity <strong>of</strong> content (36) and interest in self and family (75). Personal content and<br />

morbid content were similar variable types which seemed reliable if rated globally<br />

(subjective decisions such as masklike drawing [33] or symbols [38] were unreliable), but<br />

there may have been large variations in terms. Bizarre content was rated by 3 studies, 2<br />

indicating high agreement between 3 raters (36, 33), but study 38 found the same term<br />

unreliable between 2 raters. Since study 33 used a similar patient group, the most likely<br />

difference would be in definition. <strong>The</strong>re was poor agreement for the theme subcategory,<br />

in identification and continuity and in 2 variables from study 38, where related ideas were<br />

reliable, which is <strong>of</strong> questionable value if it is difficult to determine if there is more than<br />

one idea (several ideas, 38 unreliable). It is more likely that this variable measures some<br />

other quality <strong>of</strong> the picture such as internal relations between elements but it was not<br />

clear and therefore was rejected as ambiguous. 5 subcategories were therefore reliable.<br />

Discrimination: From 10 reliable variables tested for discrimination properties, 8<br />

showed results. Schizophrenia groups were distinguished in paranoia and retardation<br />

from non paranoic and non retarded, and p<strong>art</strong>icularly, mild retardation from other grades<br />

in the presence <strong>of</strong> bizarre content in their paintings (33). This point was supported by<br />

qualitative reports from study 46, <strong>of</strong> more bizarre work by schizophrenics than other<br />

patients, and better results from paranoid than other schizophrenics. Within<br />

schizophrenic groups, the coping strategy <strong>of</strong> 'sealing over' promoted the use <strong>of</strong> more<br />

127


personally expressive images than 'integration' (44). Poor content, a totalised score <strong>of</strong> 11<br />

separate variables, was a significant predictor <strong>of</strong> Alzheimer's disease against normal<br />

controls (36), within which, paucity <strong>of</strong> content was one <strong>of</strong> two variables which always<br />

occurred and bizarre content was identified as a frequent and significant indicator (36).<br />

<strong>The</strong> suggestion from study 46, which noted qualitatively that schizophrenics used more<br />

personal and self-centred content than other patients, was not picked up generally in this<br />

table although study 38 also noted, but did not pick up more originality from<br />

schizophrenics than normal controls. <strong>The</strong>re are indications that depressed patients use<br />

less problem solving strategies (21) than other patients. Some support is given from the<br />

finding that depressed patients, produced more hopelessness (75), and also differed from<br />

normal controls in their disinterest in self and family (75).<br />

Summary: Subjective judgement <strong>of</strong> global content is reliable, but not when referenced<br />

to the image. This indicates that what is interpreted has little relation to the picture,<br />

except in the differentiation <strong>of</strong> Alzheimer's disease, where pictures were poorer in content<br />

and more bizarre than normal controls. This could, however, reflect the physiological<br />

signs, such as control <strong>of</strong> the media, rather than intentional imagery, as indicated by the<br />

lack <strong>of</strong> specificity in 'bizarreness', such as confusion or hybrid imagery. <strong>The</strong>re are<br />

indications <strong>of</strong> differences within patient groups for personal content, but the evidence is<br />

weak because <strong>of</strong> small numbers and in the vague definition <strong>of</strong> 'expression' (44). Large<br />

differences were reported between depressed patients, normal and patient controls by 3<br />

studies on what seems reflective <strong>of</strong> situational despair or disinterest.<br />

128


Body Detail<br />

Table 3: reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong> body detail in<br />

picture.<br />

Body detail variables (study No.)<br />

(o) objective (s) subjective (c) content<br />

(f) form<br />

emotional indicators (c) (o)<br />

incest markers (9)<br />

incest markers (10 187 )<br />

sex difference (75)<br />

incest markers (57)<br />

trees (c) (o)<br />

knotholes (52)<br />

broken limbs or damaged trunk (52)<br />

leafless (52)<br />

size <strong>of</strong> figure (c) (o)<br />

self (75)<br />

family (75)<br />

figure relationships (s)<br />

self-other distance (75)<br />

self isolation (75)<br />

family isolation (75)<br />

identify self image (58) (s)<br />

(59) 189<br />

% other<br />

ag't test 1<br />

(1-6) rel'y<br />

96<br />

96<br />

96<br />

2 Mod<br />

2 NS<br />

2 .94<br />

5 .71<br />

2 .94<br />

2 .94<br />

2 .94<br />

2 .94<br />

2 .94<br />

129<br />

discriminates patient/<br />

nonpatient control<br />

(nf= no figures)<br />

stat. not clinical<br />

depressed **<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

dissociative<br />

disorders 30%<br />

freq. 188 (nf)<br />

depressed **<br />

depressed **<br />

depressed **<br />

depressed **<br />

depressed **<br />

discriminates subgroups <strong>of</strong> patients<br />

94.3<br />

61.9 unreliable 54% (5 soc.wkers); 78%<br />

(5 health pr<strong>of</strong>) 93.4% (2 Art ther)<br />

1- reliability tests: 1 = % agreement; 2 = product moment correlation; 3 = correlation coefficient; 4 = association test Chi<br />

sq. or T; 5 = Kappa; 6 = Anova.<br />

** p


to trees (52); size <strong>of</strong> figure (75); figure relationships (75) which all scored over 90%<br />

percentage agreement or correlation between 2 or 3 raters. <strong>The</strong> fact that neither <strong>of</strong> these<br />

measures gives an adequate assessment <strong>of</strong> true agreement is illustrated in the remaining<br />

subcategories, by two pairs <strong>of</strong> studies using the same tests with better controls and more<br />

raters. Studies 58 and 59, which identified self images, initially obtained high agreement<br />

between 2 <strong>art</strong> therapist raters, but, subsequently, using different raters, obtained 54-78%.<br />

<strong>The</strong> author's claim <strong>of</strong> acceptable reliability nonetheless for identification without<br />

reference to the patient, is clearly questionable, since a likely explanation <strong>of</strong> these figures<br />

is that subjective identification <strong>of</strong> the meaning <strong>of</strong> images probably depend on the rater's<br />

acquaintance with the patient's personal imagery. Studies 9 and 10 reported but did not<br />

quote 'moderate correlations' between 2 raters for their first study, whereas their second<br />

study using 4 raters achieved such poor reliability that the authors admitted serious<br />

doubts about their previous results. <strong>Study</strong> 57, which used the same patient groups, in<br />

similar numbers achieved good reliability as measured by the Kappa statistic; it seems<br />

likely that definition problems in study 10 were avoided in study 57 which focused<br />

purely graphic features. <strong>Study</strong> 75 achieved a very high correlation with another specific<br />

variable, sex differences. <strong>The</strong> emotional indicators subcategory was therefore judged<br />

reliable for specific graphic elements.<br />

Discrimination: From 11 variables between 3 studies tested for discrimination<br />

properties, 8 results showed significant differences. Differences between controls and<br />

depressed patients were shown in size <strong>of</strong> drawing, relationships between and sex<br />

differences in figures (75), but not among 74 graphic features measured from figure<br />

130


drawings by sex abused adolescents (57). Although no test was applied, trauma<br />

indicators from drawings <strong>of</strong> trees with damages found that psychotics drew more broken<br />

limbs or damaged trunks than normal controls (52).<br />

Summary: emotional indicators became reliable when judged objectively on specific<br />

graphic features. Certain features may discriminate drawings <strong>of</strong> depressed or psychotic<br />

patients from normal controls, but they were not apparent in victims <strong>of</strong> sexual abuse, the<br />

expected client group which is most frequently exposed to this kind <strong>of</strong> measure. Studies<br />

9 and 10 reported most <strong>of</strong> their variables were 'statistically but not clinically significant'<br />

for drawings from victims <strong>of</strong> sexual abuse, i.e. they were rare in that group. This finding<br />

is consistent with the recent literature 190 ; most studies failed to establish sex markers and<br />

presence <strong>of</strong> genitalia were rare.<br />

Quality<br />

Reliability: 6 experimental studies and 1 study <strong>of</strong> validity between tests generated 15<br />

variables, which were grouped among 2 subjective subcategories <strong>of</strong> content appraisal and<br />

1 formal subcategory. All the tests for agreement were the same, so studies compared<br />

well. Reliable subcategories were: presence <strong>of</strong> specific elements <strong>of</strong> drawing sophistication<br />

(38, 35, 61, 31), and proportion <strong>of</strong> the image, in p<strong>art</strong>s (38) and overall (35, 38, 36, 20).<br />

190<br />

Comprehensively summarised by M.M. Trowbridge (1995) Graphic indicators <strong>of</strong> sexual abuse in<br />

children's drawings: a review <strong>of</strong> the literature, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.22(5):485-93.<br />

131


Table 4: reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong> Quality.<br />

Quality variables (<strong>Study</strong> No.)<br />

(o) objective (s) subjective (c) content<br />

(f) form<br />

aesthetic quality (38) (s)<br />

pleasing/good (14)<br />

poor (61)<br />

care (38)<br />

craftsmanship (38)<br />

drawing sophistication (c) (o)<br />

painterly/graphic (38)<br />

dry brush (38)<br />

brush strokes (38)<br />

redrawn line (35)<br />

corrections (61)<br />

drawing sophistication, form (31)<br />

drawing sophistication, space (31)<br />

proportion (s)<br />

object (38)<br />

p<strong>art</strong>s (38)<br />

overall impairment (35)<br />

poor graphic quality (36, composite)<br />

quality copy (20)<br />

% other<br />

ag't test 1<br />

(1-6) rel'y<br />

2 NS<br />

own<br />

2 .97 191<br />

2 NS<br />

2 NS<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 .84<br />

2 .97<br />

2 .77<br />

2 .77<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 .84<br />

2 .78-.92<br />

132<br />

discriminates patient/<br />

nonpatient control<br />

(nf= no figures)<br />

common prefs 192 NS.<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

brain damage**<br />

Alzheimer's**<br />

discriminates subgroups <strong>of</strong><br />

patients<br />

NS<br />

Alzheimer's 193<br />

1 - reliability tests: 1 = % agreement; 2 = product moment correlation; 3 = correlation coefficient; 4 = association test Chi<br />

sq. or T; 5 = Kappa; 6 = Anova.<br />

** p


interpretation. Equally, the variation in scores for craftsmanship or care probably reflect<br />

individual differences, which would vary with raters' experience, so this subcategory was<br />

unreliable.<br />

Discrimination: From 9 reliable variables tested for discrimination properties, there<br />

were 4 significant results. <strong>The</strong>re were no differences between pictures by controls and<br />

brain damage (35) or schizophrenics (38) on elements <strong>of</strong> drawing sophistication, although<br />

coverage <strong>of</strong> this area was very poor, nor in proportion <strong>of</strong> objects or p<strong>art</strong>s <strong>of</strong> the pictures<br />

(38) for schizophrenia. Overall, quality <strong>of</strong> the drawing from a 9 variable composite, most<br />

discriminated Alzheimer's disease (36) and brain damage (35) from controls and among<br />

grades in the corresponding deterioration <strong>of</strong> the image or copy with dep<strong>art</strong>ure from<br />

normality (longer illness and loss <strong>of</strong> brain weight in Alzheimer's (20); damage to the left<br />

brain (which is more noticeable in normal operations) against that <strong>of</strong> the right and controls<br />

[35]).<br />

Summary: Subjective personal judgements <strong>of</strong> quality were unreliable. Reliability can<br />

be achieved when the criteria are defined as elements <strong>of</strong> the drawing. Although non-<br />

significant, study 38 noted trends towards more graphic work by schizophrenics and less<br />

proportion between objects and p<strong>art</strong>s. Global quality was a good discriminator variable<br />

for brain disease or damage, but there were indications that the coverage <strong>of</strong> this area was<br />

patchy.<br />

133


Line<br />

Table 5: reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong> line quality.<br />

Line quality variables (<strong>Study</strong> No.)<br />

(o) objective (s) subjective (c) content (f)<br />

form<br />

counted elements <strong>of</strong> line (f)<br />

number <strong>of</strong> lines (38)<br />

number <strong>of</strong> angles (35)<br />

angle production (35)<br />

% rightangles (35)<br />

% acute angles (35)<br />

obtuse angle (35)<br />

length (48)<br />

(49)<br />

long stroke (38)<br />

short stroke (38)<br />

degree short strokes (38)<br />

predominance long short (38)<br />

varied length (38)<br />

line quality (48) (f)<br />

(49)<br />

(21)<br />

heavy/light brush (38)<br />

predominance thick/thin (38)<br />

description <strong>of</strong> lines (f)<br />

reinforced (36)<br />

1 stroke (38)<br />

joined (35)<br />

crossed (35)<br />

jagged (38)<br />

broken (38)<br />

splintered (38)<br />

quavery (38)<br />

shaky (36)<br />

tremor (35)<br />

sketchy (33)<br />

curve (38)<br />

straight (38)<br />

straight/curved predominance (38)<br />

straight/curved variation (38)<br />

purpose <strong>of</strong> line (c) (s)<br />

aimless (36)<br />

enclosure (48)<br />

(49)<br />

(38)<br />

(46, composite)<br />

degree outline (38)<br />

for texture (38)<br />

for fill in (38)<br />

for shade (33)<br />

other marks (c) (o)<br />

blotches for line (38)<br />

blotches/form, mass, decor'n, texture (38)<br />

smears (38)<br />

dabs (38)<br />

degree dabs (38)<br />

dabs for form, mass, decor'n, texture (38)<br />

% other<br />

ag't test 1<br />

(1-6) rel'y<br />

99<br />

77<br />

97<br />

77<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 .84<br />

2 .84<br />

2 .84<br />

2 .84<br />

2 .84<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 NS<br />

2 >.60<br />

3 .32 NS<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 >.60<br />

90 5 .915<br />

2 NS<br />

2 .84<br />

2 .84<br />

2 NS<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 >.60<br />

90 5 .915<br />

2 .84<br />

82 4 .63<br />

2 NS<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 NS<br />

2 >.60<br />

90 5 .915<br />

98<br />

77<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 NS<br />

85 2 .68<br />

2 NS<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 NS<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 NS<br />

2 NS<br />

134<br />

discriminates pat.<br />

/nonpat. control<br />

(nf= no figures)<br />

NS<br />

brain damage **<br />

left b. damage**<br />

right b.damage*<br />

left b.damage*<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

nf<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

nf<br />

brain damage **<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

nf<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

discriminates subgroups <strong>of</strong><br />

patients<br />

NS<br />

left b.d. from r.+ control**<br />

right b.d. from left+control*<br />

left b.d. from right+control*<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

R. from L. brain damage **<br />

Grades retarded schiz 2 **<br />

Retarded schiz from non R.*<br />

NS<br />

Schizophreniform/nuclear s.*<br />

1 - reliability tests: 1 = % agree; 2 = product moment corr.; 3 = correlation coefficient; 4 = association test Chi sq./t; 5 = Kappa; 6 = Anova.<br />

2 - mild to moderate mental retardation in schizophrenia, distinguishes amongst grades.<br />

** p


Reliability: 5 experimental studies and 2 reliability studies generated 54 variables, which<br />

were grouped between 3 formal, 1 subjective and 1 objective content subcategory.<br />

Although only 5 variables between 3 studies actually gave individual figures for the<br />

reliable items (the others gave the averaged test agreement statistic), the variables and<br />

statistics within subcategories were congruous for the majority <strong>of</strong> studies. 13 <strong>of</strong> 14<br />

unreliable variables came from one exploratory study (38), which used large numbers <strong>of</strong><br />

variables, sometimes promoting confusion amongst similar terms. It must be remembered<br />

that few studies report their measurements <strong>of</strong> unreliable variables, so these measures<br />

must take on more weight in discussion than the minority <strong>of</strong> studies they seem to<br />

represent. <strong>The</strong> most reliable subcategories were: counted elements <strong>of</strong> lines (48, 35, 49,<br />

38), except for predominance <strong>of</strong> short or long lines (38), which is confusing, considering<br />

that long and short lines were identifiable. This same problem occurs in the subcategory<br />

<strong>of</strong> description <strong>of</strong> line. Presence <strong>of</strong> straight or curved line and variation <strong>of</strong> straight or<br />

curved line was reliable, but predominance was not. It is likely to be due to difficulties<br />

in relative judgement, which puts the objectivity <strong>of</strong> some <strong>of</strong> the other categories in doubt:<br />

reinforced lines were reliable (36), but not lines made with one stroke (38). Taken<br />

together it seems that polarisation in quality <strong>of</strong> line may be discerned, by relation to the<br />

whole and considering only the extremes. However, this technique would tend to allow<br />

the judgement to vary according to variation in individual style and range <strong>of</strong> marks. This<br />

subcategory was considered reliable for broad distinctions <strong>of</strong> presence. Joined or crossed<br />

(35), broken or splintered lines (38) were scored reliably present as were shaky or sketchy<br />

lines (38, 36, 33, 35), but not jagged (38) (which probably blurred with splintered).<br />

135


Purpose or use <strong>of</strong> line contained one unreliable variable among 8, fill-in (38), which was<br />

probably confused with the texture variable <strong>of</strong> the same study. Shade (33), a similar<br />

variable, achieved acceptable reliability. <strong>Study</strong> 21 achieved a correlation coefficient <strong>of</strong><br />

only 0.32 on quality <strong>of</strong> line between 3 raters, whereas three other studies achieved<br />

acceptable correlations and high agreements between 2 raters. <strong>The</strong> author <strong>of</strong> study 21<br />

admits unresolved problems in definition and reports previous results <strong>of</strong> (0.73-0.95), so<br />

the quality subcategory was considered reliable. <strong>The</strong> other marks subcategory (13<br />

variables, all from study 38) was generally unreliable where decisions <strong>of</strong> degree were<br />

made between similar variables; blotches, but not dabs (although they could be marked<br />

present) or smears, could be reliably differentiated as used for form, mass, decoration<br />

and texture (but not as used for line) so reliability was achieved only for presence <strong>of</strong><br />

blotches within the image.<br />

Discrimination: From 27 reliable variables tested for discrimination properties, there<br />

were only 5 differences reported among 3 reliable subcategories. <strong>The</strong>re were differences<br />

within patient groups <strong>of</strong> left or right brain damage and between patients with left brain<br />

damage and controls in difficulty <strong>of</strong> producing angles and in number <strong>of</strong> acute angles,<br />

within patient groups and between left brain damage and controls in right angles, but no<br />

difference in obtuse angle production (35). <strong>The</strong>re were no differences among these<br />

groups or among schizophrenics and controls in number (35, 38) <strong>of</strong> lines, or among<br />

schizophrenics and controls in length or quality <strong>of</strong> lines or presence <strong>of</strong> substitute marks<br />

(38). Within groups, nuclear schizophrenics were reported to use more shading than<br />

those with schizophreniform illnesses (33), but there were no differences in purpose <strong>of</strong><br />

136


lines between controls and Alzheimer's patients (36), schizophrenics and other patients<br />

(46) or schizophrenics and controls (38). Differences among diagnostic groups were<br />

shown in, shaky lines or tremor, probably betraying physiological damage, discriminating<br />

brain damage from normal controls (35). Sketchy line discriminated mild from other grades<br />

<strong>of</strong> schizophrenic retardates and schizophrenic retardates from normal schizophrenics<br />

(33).<br />

Summary: <strong>The</strong> reliability may vary with the range <strong>of</strong> line expressed and its relative<br />

extremes. Overall, line does not seem to be a discriminable dimension against normal<br />

controls for schizophrenia or Alzheimer's disease. Line does show up logical difficulties<br />

in control, p<strong>art</strong>icularly in the drawing <strong>of</strong> angles by patients with brain damage. This<br />

probably reflects loss <strong>of</strong> fine motor control rather than a distortion in concept <strong>of</strong> the<br />

image. <strong>The</strong> deterioration <strong>of</strong> controlled line the further towards retardation in<br />

schizophrenia may point towards a theory <strong>of</strong> regression for that group.<br />

Shape Table 6: reliability statistics and discriminating variables<br />

Shape variables (study no.)<br />

(o) objective (s) subjective (c) content<br />

(f) form<br />

% other<br />

ag't test 1 (1-<br />

6) rel'y<br />

paucity shape/form (36) (f) 90 2 .915 nf<br />

differentiation <strong>of</strong> line/shape (48) (f)<br />

(49)<br />

variation in form (38)<br />

variation in mass (38)<br />

shape dimensions (f)<br />

dominant shape (69)<br />

size <strong>of</strong> shapes (69)<br />

regularity <strong>of</strong> shape (69)<br />

similarity <strong>of</strong> shapes (69)<br />

flat shape (36)<br />

repeated forms (38) (f)<br />

3+ abstract forms mannerism (56)<br />

3+ same shape (69)<br />

4 shape/detail stereotype (56)<br />

98<br />

77<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 >.60<br />

137<br />

discriminates patient/<br />

nonpatient control (nf=<br />

no figures)<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

4 Acc.<br />

4 Acc.<br />

4 NS<br />

4 Acc.<br />

90 5 .915 nf<br />

2 >.60<br />

6 Acc.<br />

4 Acc.<br />

6 Acc.<br />

NS<br />

patients/schizophrenic*<br />

NS<br />

discriminates subgroups <strong>of</strong> patients<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

Schizophrenic/patients*<br />

NS<br />

NS


Shape variables (study no.)<br />

(o) objective (s) subjective (c) content<br />

(f) form<br />

delineated form by mass, contour (f)<br />

(38)<br />

delineated form by line (38)<br />

% other<br />

ag't test 1 (1-<br />

6) rel'y<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 NS<br />

138<br />

discriminates patient/<br />

nonpatient control (nf=<br />

no figures)<br />

NS<br />

discriminates subgroups <strong>of</strong> patients<br />

1- reliability tests: 1 = % agreement; 2 = product moment correlation; 3 = correlation coefficient; 4 = association test Chi<br />

sq. or T; 5 = Kappa; 6 = Anova.<br />

** p


Summary: Shape does not appear to distinguish any group <strong>of</strong> patients reliably. <strong>The</strong>re<br />

are ambiguities in terms from the literature which are not shown in the reliability tests,<br />

possibly reflecting individualistic research which shows itself in lack <strong>of</strong> congruency in<br />

type <strong>of</strong> variables between studies.<br />

139


Colour<br />

Table 7: reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong> colour.<br />

Colour variables (study no.)<br />

(o) objective (s) subjective (c) content<br />

(f) form<br />

number <strong>of</strong> colours (38) (f)<br />

(14)<br />

(75)<br />

(44)<br />

(36)<br />

colours used (14) (f)<br />

actual colour (38)<br />

colourtype (48)<br />

(49)<br />

mixed colour (f)<br />

blend (48)<br />

blend (49)<br />

muddy (38)<br />

mixed on surface (38)<br />

mixed colour (38)<br />

pure colour(38)<br />

thick (38)<br />

watery (38)<br />

dominant hue (69) (f)<br />

prominence (21)<br />

light/dark tone (69)<br />

brighter tone (69)<br />

brightness (69)<br />

brightness (38)<br />

dominant tone (69)<br />

consistency <strong>of</strong> colour (38) (f)<br />

(69)<br />

1/6+ 1 colour (56)<br />

masses 1 colour (38)<br />

decoration or outline in colour (38)<br />

colour fit (21) (c) (s)<br />

idiosyncratic colour (48)<br />

(49)<br />

colour harmony (69)<br />

colour relations (3)<br />

% other<br />

ag't test 1<br />

(1-6) rel'y<br />

2 NS<br />

own criteria nf<br />

2 .94<br />

3 .63<br />

90 5 .915<br />

own criteria nf<br />

2 NS<br />

98<br />

77<br />

92<br />

77<br />

97<br />

77<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 NS<br />

2 NS<br />

2 >.60<br />

4 Acc.<br />

3 .90<br />

4 Acc.<br />

4 Acc.<br />

4 Acc.<br />

2 >.60<br />

4 Acc.<br />

2 NS<br />

4 NS<br />

4 Acc.<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 >.60<br />

3 .86<br />

4 Acc.<br />

3 .83<br />

140<br />

discriminates patient/<br />

nonpatient control (nf= no<br />

figures)<br />

patients less (nf)<br />

depressed **<br />

Alzheimer's less (nf)<br />

patient pref.Red/Black(nf)<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

discriminates subgroups <strong>of</strong> patients<br />

schiz. coping strats 194 *<br />

psychotic depressives 195 **<br />

patient groups * 196<br />

neurotics *<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

neurotics *<br />

NS<br />

NS NS<br />

NS<br />

pers. disorder; dep. less (nf)<br />

1 - reliability tests: 1 = % agreement; 2 = product moment correlation; 3 = correlation coefficient; 4 = association test Chi<br />

sq. or T; 5 = Kappa; 6 = Anova.<br />

** p


applied between studies but there was high congruency <strong>of</strong> variables within subcategories.<br />

10 variables between 2 studies gave an item reliability score and there were 6 unreliable<br />

variables although these were spread over 4 subcategories. Reliable subcategories were:<br />

brightness <strong>of</strong> hue and tone (69, 21, 38) which were undifferentiable in most studies so<br />

are grouped together here. (study 69 did differentiate them and found evaluations <strong>of</strong><br />

these elements highly correlated); coloured detail (38), and colour fit (21, 48, 49, 3, 69).<br />

4 studies with the same term definition (75, 14, 36, 44) achieved high reliability on<br />

number <strong>of</strong> colours, but correlations between raters were very low in study 38. This<br />

study also found low correlations for colours used, found reliable by 3 other studies (14,<br />

48, 49, although two <strong>of</strong> these used no figures). <strong>The</strong> raters for study 38 were <strong>art</strong> trained<br />

and it is likely that there were too many delineations <strong>of</strong> colour in this study. <strong>The</strong>refore<br />

these subcategories are considered reliable when not too complicated. Colour mix was<br />

reliable on premixed colour (38, 48, 49), on surface mixing (38), muddy or watery colour<br />

(but not thick or pure, which probably denotes difficulty in decisions on relative<br />

consistency)(38). Consistency <strong>of</strong> (i.e. the most prevalent) colour was found unreliable<br />

by 2 studies (38, 69) and the reliability for amount <strong>of</strong> single colour was only 'acceptable'<br />

in study 56. This was probably due to global assessment <strong>of</strong> the whole picture, whereas<br />

specifics, such as detail in decoration or outline or masses were reliable. This category<br />

is therefore considered reliable for specific details.<br />

Discrimination: From 21 reliable variables tested for discriminant properties, there were<br />

8 significant results among 3 subcategories. General psychiatric patients, Alzheimer's<br />

patients and depressed patients (14, 36, 75) all scored less than normal controls on<br />

141


number <strong>of</strong> colours. Furthermore within coping strategies <strong>of</strong> schizophrenic groups,<br />

'integrators' scored more than 'sealers-over'(44). <strong>The</strong>re were no tests on colours used, but<br />

patients were reported to use more red and black (14) than normal controls. Neurotics<br />

were differentiated from other patients on 2 highly correlated variables; tone and hue (69).<br />

<strong>Study</strong> 38 found no differences between schizophrenics and normal controls among 4<br />

mixed colour variables and 2 details in colour variables. 2 studies found no differences<br />

for colour fit (69, 21) but a tendency to less appropriate colour by depressed patients<br />

with personality disorders was reported (3) although there were no test results.<br />

Psychotic depressives were differentiated from 5 patient groups on dominant hue (69),<br />

which concurred with initial findings <strong>of</strong> significant difference between 4 patient groups<br />

and controls using the same test statistic (21). <strong>The</strong> author <strong>of</strong> <strong>Study</strong> 21 reports no<br />

significance for this result because the Sheffé test did not isolate a single group, but I have<br />

disregarded this post-hoc analysis: the test procedure increases the power <strong>of</strong> the results,<br />

but the small numbers and overlap between groups in each <strong>of</strong> these two studies (21<br />

N=25; 69 N=33), make complex statistical procedures inappropriate and limit the<br />

comparability <strong>of</strong> the results, so results from study 69 and initial findings from study 21<br />

are here considered equivalent and valid.<br />

Summary: Normal controls generally use more and brighter colours than all patient<br />

groups although there could be more subtle inter-group differences amongst<br />

schizophrenics. Prominent or dominant colours or tones may <strong>of</strong>fer discrimination<br />

between patient subgroups but clear distinctions between groups have not yet been made.<br />

142


Reality<br />

Table 8: reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong> reality.<br />

reality variables (study no.)<br />

(o) objective (s) subjective (c)<br />

content (f) form<br />

overall reality (c) (o)<br />

reality (21)<br />

degree verisimilitude (38)<br />

representative (48)<br />

(49)<br />

abstract (48)<br />

(49)<br />

abstract symbol (48)<br />

(49)<br />

verisimilitude masses & forms (38)<br />

verisimilitude objects (38)<br />

ambiguous shapes (36)<br />

reality in content elements (c) (s)<br />

omissions (38)<br />

additions (38)<br />

distortion (38)<br />

faulty recall (36)<br />

displacement (35)<br />

place error (33)<br />

unusual place (48)<br />

(49)<br />

logic (21) (c) (o)<br />

light source (38)<br />

connections:path, door; correctly<br />

placed path, door, window (46) 198<br />

horizon/ground line (c) (o)<br />

ground line (48)<br />

(49)<br />

base line (46)<br />

horizon (56)<br />

(46)<br />

% other<br />

ag't test 1<br />

(1-6) rel'y<br />

77<br />

77<br />

88<br />

77<br />

93<br />

77<br />

3 .88<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 >.60<br />

90 5 .915<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 >.60<br />

90 5 .915<br />

2 .84<br />

56 4 .56<br />

99<br />

77<br />

96<br />

3 .92<br />

2 >.60<br />

100<br />

77<br />

96<br />

4 good<br />

96<br />

discriminates patient/<br />

nonpatient control (nf= no<br />

figures)<br />

organic dis.**<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

nf<br />

143<br />

discriminates subgroups <strong>of</strong> patients<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

Alzheimer's (more) nf.<br />

R/L brain damage** R.brain-d from L.brain-d 197<br />

Acute/Chronic schizophrenia*;<br />

grades in mental-ret'd. schiz.**;<br />

non-ret'd/retarded in schiz's.**<br />

organics **<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

depressed from mania+organic**<br />

1- reliability tests: 1 = % agreement; 2 = product moment correlation; 3 = correlation coefficient; 4 = association test Chi<br />

sq. or T; 5 = Kappa; 6 = Anova.<br />

** p


subcategories were congruous and all overall percentages showed good-very high<br />

agreements with no unreliable variables. Reliable subcategories were: recognisable content<br />

(48, 21, 49, 38, 36), horizon or base line (56, 46, 48, 49) and logical connections between<br />

named elements (46, 21). Although measures <strong>of</strong> placement showed good agreement<br />

generally in addition, absence, or distortion (38), placing errors (33), a term applied to<br />

placement <strong>of</strong> the central figure in the middle <strong>of</strong> the picture, showed much lower<br />

percentage agreement than the other studies (0.56). However, acceptable significance<br />

levels (p


omissions, distortions or additions <strong>of</strong> elements (38), although the same type <strong>of</strong> dep<strong>art</strong>ure<br />

from reality (evidence <strong>of</strong> faulty recall) was more marked in Alzheimer's disease than in<br />

controls (36 or 35). <strong>The</strong> displacement variable showed significant differences between<br />

left brain damaged patients and normal controls or right brain damaged patients (35), also<br />

between a patient group p<strong>art</strong>itioned 3 ways (33): (a) non retarded schizophrenic from<br />

retarded schizophrenics; (b) pr<strong>of</strong>ound from other grades <strong>of</strong> mental retardation; and<br />

between acute and chronic schizophrenia.<br />

Summary: <strong>The</strong> polarisation <strong>of</strong> representation <strong>of</strong> reality and logic between controls and<br />

organic brain damage seems to be supported by findings <strong>of</strong> no differences between<br />

schizophrenics, other patients and controls on corresponding indices. It is thus<br />

reasonable to suppose that the results for Alzheimer's disease, as it affects the brain, may<br />

share some <strong>of</strong> the qualities <strong>of</strong> organic brain damage. Displacement <strong>of</strong> main images seems<br />

to strongly indicate general abnormality.<br />

Space<br />

Reliability: 11 experimental studies, 2 reliability studies and 1 validity study generated<br />

36 variables which were grouped between two subjective, one objective content<br />

subcategory, and one formal subcategory. <strong>The</strong>re were 6 variables between 6 studies<br />

which gave item reliabilities and only one unreliable variable in the table. Reliable<br />

subcategories were: coverage <strong>of</strong> the paper (49, 44, 48, 21, 75, 35, 38, 36); relations<br />

between the main elements (3, 35, 61); presence or absence <strong>of</strong> perspective or indicators<br />

145


(35, 46, 36, 38, 33); and indicators <strong>of</strong> dimensionality (56, 61, 36, 46, 38). <strong>The</strong> exclusion<br />

<strong>of</strong> rotation (21) did not affect the other subcategory variables.<br />

Table 9: reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong> space.<br />

Spacial variables (study no.)<br />

(o) objective (s) subjective (c) content<br />

(f) form<br />

picture space used (f)<br />

(48)<br />

(49)<br />

(44)<br />

(21)<br />

(38)<br />

empty space (75)<br />

neglect (35)<br />

impoverishment (46, composite)<br />

neglect (36)<br />

spacial relations (35) (s)<br />

(3)<br />

expansive/constrictive (61)<br />

presence <strong>of</strong> perspective (35) (c) (o)<br />

(46)<br />

depth (38)<br />

proportion (36)<br />

distorted perspective (36)<br />

perspective elements<br />

building top page (46)<br />

size errors (33)<br />

distance small (46)<br />

distance systematic (46)<br />

foreshortening: all elements, in roads,<br />

progressively in roads, systematically<br />

in roads (46)<br />

overlap (35)<br />

(46)<br />

+1 overlap (46)<br />

lines overlapping (36)<br />

organisation <strong>of</strong> space (46, composite)<br />

dimensions (s)<br />

2 dimensional (primitive) (56)<br />

3 dimensional (61)<br />

2 sides building (46)<br />

2 different angles <strong>of</strong> building (46)<br />

transparencies (38)<br />

rotation (21)<br />

birds eye view (36)<br />

worms eye view (36)<br />

% other<br />

ag't test*<br />

(1-6) rel'y<br />

92<br />

77<br />

3 .63<br />

3 .92<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 .94<br />

2 .84<br />

96<br />

90 5 .915<br />

2 .84<br />

3 .89<br />

2 .97<br />

2 .84<br />

96<br />

2 >.60<br />

90 5 .915<br />

90 5 .915<br />

96<br />

70 4 .62<br />

96<br />

96<br />

96<br />

2 .84<br />

96<br />

96<br />

90 5 .915<br />

96<br />

96<br />

4 Acc.<br />

2 .97<br />

2 >.60<br />

3 NS<br />

90 5 .915<br />

90 5 .915<br />

146<br />

discriminates patient/<br />

nonpatient control<br />

(nf= no figures)<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

depressed **<br />

brain damage**<br />

Alzheimer's (more) nf<br />

brain damage**<br />

patients less (nf)<br />

brain damage**<br />

NS<br />

nf<br />

nf<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

nf<br />

patients *<br />

discriminates subgroups <strong>of</strong> patients<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

right from left b.damage**<br />

Schiz. from patients**<br />

NS<br />

depressed pers.dis (nf)<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

grades ret'n in schizophrenia*<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

* - reliability tests: 1 = % agreement; 2 = product moment correlation; 3 = correlation coefficient; 4 = association test Chi sq. or<br />

T; 5 = Kappa; 6 = Anova.<br />

** p


<strong>of</strong> elements, differentiated controls from mixed patients (56) but not within<br />

schizophrenics/mixed patients (46, 56). <strong>The</strong> perspective subcategory did not differentiate<br />

between schizophrenics and other patients either (46) or between left/right brain damage<br />

(35) or between schizophrenics with and without mental retardation (33). <strong>The</strong>re were<br />

no differences either, between controls and schizophrenics on depth perspective (38) or<br />

size errors (33) which did distinguish severe mental retardation in schizophrenia from<br />

other grades (33). Brain damaged patients generally (left or right) were distinguishable<br />

from controls by overall perspective but not by strategy (overlap). Patients with brain<br />

damage (35) or personality disorder (3) used less spacial relations than controls. Empty<br />

space, apparently, distinguished depressed patients (75), Alzheimer's patients (36) and<br />

brain damage (35) from controls. Furthermore, study 46 reports, impoverishment in<br />

pictures by schizophrenics compared to other patients though both patient groups were<br />

equally poor against normal children. <strong>The</strong>se results, however, were not picked up by 2<br />

studies which reported that picture space used neither distinguishes between patient<br />

groups <strong>of</strong>: mania, depressed, organics, schizophrenia and controls (21), schizophrenics<br />

and controls (38).<br />

<strong>The</strong>re seem two obvious sources <strong>of</strong> confusion: Most significant findings were<br />

from measures <strong>of</strong> neglect or emptiness (which could be taken to mean incompleteness).<br />

Measures which gave non-significant results as proportions <strong>of</strong> media coverage were<br />

typical <strong>of</strong> highly controlled studies, but which used very small group sizes (each group<br />

(21, N=25; 38, N=33) so probably suffer lack <strong>of</strong> power.<br />

147


Summary: <strong>The</strong>re are unresolved definition ambiguities <strong>of</strong> the qualities measured.<br />

Neglect measures appear to distinguish patients against controls and schizophrenia from<br />

patients although coverage <strong>of</strong> paper does not. Mixed patients use more 2 dimensional<br />

space than controls although there are no specific differences in schizophrenia. Patients<br />

with brain damage use less spatial relations than controls and share some perspective<br />

abnormalities <strong>of</strong> mental retardation, but the qualification mentioned previously in reality<br />

above (study 33), may apply to both mental retardation and brain damage; limitations on<br />

measurements <strong>of</strong> drawing qualities show that severe retardates make less errors because<br />

there is observably less on the paper, so the utility <strong>of</strong> this form <strong>of</strong> measurement may be<br />

compromised.<br />

Energy Table 10: reliability statistics and discriminating variables for Energy.<br />

energy variables (study No.)<br />

(o) objective (s) subjective (c) content<br />

(f) form<br />

energy (21) (c) (s)<br />

(3)<br />

tension (38)<br />

balance <strong>of</strong> motion (69)<br />

motion (44)<br />

(48)<br />

(49)<br />

motion shown thru line (38) (c) (s)<br />

motion in objects (38)<br />

motion shown through colour (38)<br />

main subject <strong>of</strong> picture (c) (o)<br />

energy in self (75)<br />

energy in family (75)<br />

human action (46)<br />

graphic human action (46)<br />

1, 2, 3 buildings, function visible (46)<br />

community building (46)<br />

good representation bldg function (46)<br />

% other<br />

ag't test 1<br />

(1-6) rel'y<br />

97<br />

77<br />

96<br />

96<br />

96<br />

96<br />

96<br />

3 .60<br />

3 .89<br />

2 >.60<br />

4 Acc.<br />

3 .63<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 NS<br />

2 .94<br />

2 .94<br />

148<br />

discriminates patient/<br />

nonpatient control<br />

(nf= no figures)<br />

organic **<br />

patients less (nf)<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

depressed **<br />

depressed **<br />

discriminates subgroups <strong>of</strong> patients<br />

depressed from organic** 199<br />

depressed pers. dis. (less) nf<br />

paranoid from non (schiz) *<br />

coping strategies schiz. 200 (p


Reliability: 7 experimental and 2 reliability studies generated 19 variables which were<br />

grouped between 2 subcategories <strong>of</strong> subjective and 1 objective subcategory <strong>of</strong> content<br />

evaluation. <strong>The</strong>re were 3 significant variables independently tested for item reliability<br />

between 3 studies, and only one unreliable variable appeared in the table. Reliable<br />

subcategories were: overall energy (21, 3; 48, 44 in movement, 38 in tension and 69 in<br />

balance <strong>of</strong> motion); specific energy in the main subjects <strong>of</strong> the picture (75 in family or self,<br />

46 in action or function); and motion conveyed by form (line and objects), but not by<br />

colour (38).<br />

Discrimination: From 15 variables tested for discrimination properties, there were 6<br />

significant results between 2 subcategories. <strong>The</strong>re was no great concordance <strong>of</strong> items<br />

within subcategories. Neither specific function <strong>of</strong> the main picture elements nor human<br />

energy distinguished schizophrenics from other patient groups (46) but human energy<br />

(self or family) did discriminate depressed patients from controls (75), giving support to<br />

the findings <strong>of</strong> discrimination in global energy ratings between depressed patients,<br />

controls and patients with organic mental disorder (21). <strong>The</strong>re were also indications that<br />

a secondary diagnosis <strong>of</strong> depression may be discriminable (3). <strong>The</strong>re were no significant<br />

differences in global tension or balance <strong>of</strong> motion between schizophrenics and controls<br />

(38, 69), but within patient groups, paranoid schizophrenics were distinguished from<br />

other patient groups (69) and schizophrenics who integrated experiences were<br />

distinguished from those who sealed over (44).<br />

149


Summary: <strong>The</strong> effect which distinguished depressed patients from controls and organics<br />

on global energy is supported by the discrimination <strong>of</strong> depressed patients from controls<br />

on specific human energy. Although there is probably another energy factor which does<br />

not occur under the main elements <strong>of</strong> the picture which discriminated between patient<br />

groups.<br />

Detail<br />

Table 11: reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong> pictorial detail.<br />

Pictorial detail variables (study No.(s)<br />

(o) objective (s) subjective (c) content<br />

(f) form<br />

omission <strong>of</strong> detail (33) (c) (o)<br />

missing detail (56)<br />

presence <strong>of</strong> detail (75)<br />

(35)<br />

(44)<br />

(21)<br />

presence <strong>of</strong> 14 details: civilised<br />

objects, 2 civ objects, roads, 2 roads,<br />

path, sidewalk, window, windows,<br />

door, garden, nature, relief, vegetation,<br />

shadows (46)<br />

amount <strong>of</strong> detail (38)<br />

superfluous detail (c) (o)<br />

(56)<br />

(35)<br />

(36)<br />

(61)<br />

decoration (38)<br />

external detail (35)<br />

words (48)<br />

word script (56)<br />

words on building (46)<br />

words (49)<br />

extra letters (36)<br />

% other<br />

ag't test 1<br />

(1-6) rel'y<br />

63 4 .59<br />

96<br />

4 Acc.<br />

2 .94<br />

2 .84<br />

3 .63<br />

3 .80<br />

2 >.60<br />

4 Acc.<br />

2 .84<br />

90 5 .915<br />

2 .97<br />

2 NS<br />

2 .84<br />

100<br />

4 v.good<br />

96<br />

77<br />

90 5 .915<br />

150<br />

discriminates patient/<br />

nonpatient control<br />

(nf= no figures)<br />

patients **<br />

depressed **<br />

left brain damage**<br />

organics **<br />

NS<br />

discriminates subgroups <strong>of</strong> patients<br />

mild from other grades schiz'c<br />

retardation**; schizophreniform from<br />

nuclear schiz*; retardation from nonret.<br />

schiz'cs. **<br />

NS<br />

Left brain d. from R. **<br />

schiz. coping groups **<br />

organics and mania<br />

1 - reliability tests: 1 = % agreement; 2 = product moment correlation; 3 = correlation coefficient; 4 = association test Chi<br />

sq. or T; 5 = Kappa; 6 = Anova.<br />

** p


one unreliable variable, decoration (38), which may have been confused with other similar<br />

elements. <strong>The</strong> other items were congruent within their subcategories and gave high overall<br />

reliabilities. Reliable subcategories were: the presence or absence <strong>of</strong> detail (56, 46, 61, 75,<br />

35, 36, 44, 21, 33) and superfluous detail (48 56 35 46 49 36 61).<br />

Discrimination: From 28 variables tested for discrimination properties, there were 6<br />

significant results which occurred only in the objective presence or absence <strong>of</strong> details<br />

subcategory. Superfluous detail, which included words was rated universally non<br />

significant by 5 studies and in support, study 46 included in its qualitative report, the<br />

comment that words and microscopia are equally common in children, schizophrenics<br />

and patient controls. All studies measuring global elements found differences. 2 studies<br />

reported within patient group results: between the 'integration' and 'sealing over' coping<br />

strategies <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia (44), and within 3 p<strong>art</strong>itions <strong>of</strong> schizophrenic and retarded<br />

subjects (study 33). 4 studies reported results against normal controls: Depression was<br />

discriminated from controls (75) and left brain damage from controls (35). However,<br />

while study 21, supported the brain damage/control difference, their control scores<br />

overlapped those <strong>of</strong> mania, and did not discriminate depression or schizophrenia on this<br />

index. <strong>The</strong> solution probably lies in the results <strong>of</strong> the fourth study which supports most<br />

results, that mixed psychiatric patients were discriminable from normal controls (56), but<br />

not schizophrenia, so, the findings <strong>of</strong> no differences in 75 counted detail elements<br />

between schizophrenics and other patients (46) are also supported.<br />

151


Summary: Striking variation in global subjective evaluation <strong>of</strong> detail indicates that what<br />

is measured is not detail, but some element <strong>of</strong> completeness. <strong>The</strong> variety <strong>of</strong> patient<br />

groups distinguished, including the patient/non-patient distinction, indicate that missing<br />

detail is a good predictor <strong>of</strong> patient status, although brain damage patients were right/left<br />

differentiable, but right scores overlapped those <strong>of</strong> controls.<br />

Complexity<br />

Table 12: reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong> complexity.<br />

complexity/differentiation variables<br />

(study no.) (o) objective (s) subjective<br />

(c) content (f) form<br />

regression/complexity (c) (s)<br />

simplification (35)<br />

childlike (56)<br />

developmental (21)<br />

simple/complex (38)<br />

differentiation (46, composite)<br />

differentiation (c) (o)<br />

variation in building detail (46)<br />

complexity in dwelling (46)<br />

difference in: structure <strong>of</strong> building - 2,<br />

or 3 types; dwellings, 2 different or<br />

personalised; heights, widths, doors,<br />

number or kind <strong>of</strong> windows (46)<br />

% other<br />

ag't test 1<br />

(1-6) rel'y<br />

96<br />

96<br />

96<br />

2 .84<br />

4 poor<br />

3 .88<br />

2 >.60<br />

152<br />

discriminates patient/<br />

nonpatient control (nf= no<br />

figures)<br />

brain damage**<br />

organic or mania**<br />

NS<br />

discriminates subgroups <strong>of</strong> patients<br />

left from r. brain damage**<br />

depressed from organic **<br />

schiz's from patients **<br />

1 - reliability tests: 1 = % agreement; 2 = product moment correlation; 3 = correlation coefficient; 4 = association test Chi<br />

sq. or T; 5 = Kappa; 6 = Anova.<br />

** p


Discrimination: From 16 variables tested for discrimination properties there were 2<br />

significant results which both fell into the global regression/complexity subcategory.<br />

Controls were differentiated from brain damage (35), which result was also supported by<br />

study 21, differentiating brain damage from 4 patient and a control group but which found<br />

mania scores overlapped the brain damage (21). Depression was also differentiated from<br />

brain damage in the same study but not schizophrenia. <strong>The</strong>re were no differences<br />

between schizophrenics and controls (38) in another study and no differences in any <strong>of</strong><br />

the elemental scores for differentiation between schizophrenia and other patients (46).<br />

Summary: <strong>The</strong>re were global but not elemental significant differences in elements for<br />

schizophrenia and other patients. This indicates an additive effect which should be<br />

differentiable by logistic regression. Other global elements differentiated brain damage<br />

from controls.<br />

Control<br />

Table 13: reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong> control.<br />

somatic signs (study no.)<br />

(o) objective (s) subjective (c) content<br />

(f) form<br />

perseveration (35) (c) (s)<br />

(21)<br />

(33)<br />

pers. in line/form (36)<br />

pers. in theme (36)<br />

% other<br />

ag't test 1<br />

(1-6) rel'y<br />

2 .84<br />

3 .54 NS<br />

NS 4 NS<br />

90 5 .915<br />

90 5 .915<br />

153<br />

discriminates patient/<br />

nonpatient control<br />

(nf= no figures)<br />

brain damage**<br />

NS<br />

Alzheimer's (more) nf.<br />

discriminates subgroups<br />

<strong>of</strong> patients<br />

1 - reliability tests: 1 = % agreement; 2 = product moment correlation; 3 = correlation coefficient; 4 = association test Chi<br />

sq. or T; 5 = Kappa; 6 = Anova.<br />

** p


Reliability: Perseveration is a non-<strong>art</strong> term from diagnostic criteria. It was rated by 4<br />

studies; the two with good reliability gave composite test scores (35), by structure and<br />

theme (36). Another 2 studies used individual item ratings which were unreliable.<br />

Perseveration is not an objective definition because a subjective decision <strong>of</strong> intent is<br />

required to rate it. This category was considered unreliable for the remaining variables.<br />

Summary: <strong>The</strong>re are indications <strong>of</strong> effect for studies with consistent definitions <strong>of</strong><br />

terms, in brain damage and controls in 2 studies but there are questions <strong>of</strong> content validity<br />

which have not yet been addressed.<br />

Composition<br />

Table 14: reliability statistics and discriminating variables for category <strong>of</strong> composition.<br />

composition variables (study No.)<br />

(o) objective (s) subjective (c) content<br />

(f) form<br />

orientation <strong>of</strong> picture (35) (f)<br />

tilt (48)<br />

(49)<br />

centre <strong>of</strong> focus (69) (f)<br />

dominance <strong>of</strong> image (38)<br />

top/bottom (69)<br />

left/right (69)<br />

right/left/top/bottom (36)<br />

structural relations (c) (s)<br />

rhythm (38)<br />

serial elements used structurally (69)<br />

relation <strong>of</strong> pic to frame (38)<br />

integration (21) (c) (s)<br />

(48)<br />

(49)<br />

compositional integration (3)<br />

general integration (3)<br />

unity (38)<br />

organisation (75)<br />

disorganisation (33)<br />

incoherence (56)<br />

fragmented gestalt (36)<br />

disorganisation (36, composite)<br />

balance (38) (c) (o)<br />

symmetrical balance (38)<br />

equilibrium (69)<br />

imbalance (56)<br />

% other<br />

ag't test 1<br />

(1-6) rel'y<br />

99<br />

77<br />

2 .94<br />

4 Acc.<br />

2 >.60<br />

4 Acc.<br />

4 Acc.<br />

90 5 .915<br />

2 >.60<br />

4 NS<br />

2 NS<br />

3 .94<br />

94<br />

77<br />

3 .80<br />

3 .91<br />

2 NS<br />

2 .94<br />

4 NS<br />

4 Acc.<br />

90 5 .915<br />

2 >.60<br />

2 >.60<br />

4 Acc.<br />

4 v.good<br />

154<br />

discriminates patient/<br />

nonpatient control (nf= no<br />

figures)<br />

NS NS<br />

NS<br />

Alzheimer's (more)nf<br />

NS<br />

organic/manic **<br />

patients (less) nf.<br />

patients (less) nf.<br />

depressed **<br />

NS<br />

nf<br />

Alzheimer's **<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

patients more **<br />

discriminates subgroups <strong>of</strong><br />

patients<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

organic from manic **<br />

depressed/non-d pers.dis(nf)<br />

depressed/non-d pers.dis(nf)<br />

1 - reliability tests: 1 = % agreement; 2 = product moment correlation; 3 = correlation coefficient; 4 = association test Chi<br />

sq. or T; 5 = Kappa; 6 = Anova.<br />

NS<br />

NS<br />

NS


** p


(including schizophrenics) against controls (56). Both studies are equally well controlled,<br />

but study 38 used very small numbers and is therefore open to type 2 error, the authors<br />

qualitatively note that schizophrenics' pictures frequently show less balance and<br />

symmetry, although this did not show statistically. This review therefore accepts the<br />

results from study 56. Depression was distinguished from controls on organisation (75),<br />

but not by study 21, which distinguished controls, organics and mania amongst 2 other<br />

patient groups (depression and schizophrenia) (21). <strong>The</strong> findings <strong>of</strong> no significance<br />

between schizophrenia and other patients, and mixed patients against controls tends to<br />

support both results (21, 75) by suggesting overlaps between certain groups (especially<br />

depression, schizophrenia and controls) and reduces the chance <strong>of</strong> the confusion between<br />

a diagnostic and a patient effect. <strong>Study</strong> 3 noted but did not test less compositional and<br />

general integration in pictures by patients with personality disorder than controls, and<br />

study 36 used a composite variable organisation, encompassing its derivatives listed in<br />

this table, which distinguished patients with Alzheimer's disease from controls.<br />

Summary: <strong>The</strong> composition category is generally non discriminatory for schizophrenia<br />

although there are indications <strong>of</strong> lack <strong>of</strong> balanced work. <strong>The</strong> most discriminable diagnosis<br />

was Alzheimer's or brain damage on central variables and organisational, p<strong>art</strong>icularly<br />

integration for brain damage. Depression also distinguished integration, but there were<br />

indications depressed scores might overlap with schizophrenia.<br />

Summary <strong>of</strong> reliability study<br />

156


<strong>The</strong>re were more reliable elements than unreliable, but the studies examined here do not<br />

represent the major p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the literature in their clear, and mostly objective, definitions<br />

<strong>of</strong> search criteria. Only 27% <strong>of</strong> the controlled test situations are represented here and<br />

1.8% from the case studies. <strong>The</strong>re were 15 formal and 37 content categories, split<br />

between 21 subjective and 16 objective decisions. 5 subcategories were found to be<br />

unreliable, all <strong>of</strong> them subjective.<br />

Unreliable elements <strong>of</strong> pictures seemed to predominate in interpretation, fine<br />

distinctions between two similar elements and global judgements. Raters could not<br />

identify symbols, themes or the continuity <strong>of</strong> themes between pictures, whether several<br />

ideas were expressed, differentiate ordinary from bizarre content, identify childlike<br />

elements, incest markers or self images or use their own criteria for patient versus non<br />

patient status judgements. <strong>The</strong>y could not describe a painting, whether it was unified,<br />

organised or coherent, nor decide whether elements were used structurally, agree on care<br />

or craftsmanship (although they recognised quality). Detail decisions were inconsistent:<br />

although raters could identify length <strong>of</strong> lines they could not tell if p<strong>art</strong>icular types<br />

predominated, whether lines were jagged or used for fill in, whether shapes were more<br />

regular than other shapes, amount <strong>of</strong> single colours, whether colours were thick or pure<br />

(although they could differentiate watery or mixed colour) and the consistency or<br />

intensity <strong>of</strong> colour. <strong>The</strong>y did not agree on errors <strong>of</strong> size and placing, on omission, lack<br />

<strong>of</strong> detail or decoration, perseveration, whether motion was conveyed by colour, line by<br />

blotches or whether dabs were used for form, mass, texture or decoration (although they<br />

could for blotches).<br />

157


<strong>The</strong> studies are difficult to sum up collectively in any meaningful way as each<br />

study examines a different selection <strong>of</strong> qualities, which makes them difficult to compare<br />

or replicate and the final interpretation comes as a synthesis <strong>of</strong> the study. Some <strong>of</strong> the<br />

definitions seem unclear, especially those which deal with global categories and some<br />

studies dealing with objective qualities provide a huge unwieldy instrument where<br />

alternative terms are difficult to differentiate 201 . Most conspicuously, there are<br />

considerable problems as to the authors' use <strong>of</strong> statistics (which will be discussed next).<br />

Mostly, the statistical bases on which the reliability is calculated contain serious<br />

methodological flaws.<br />

Measures <strong>of</strong> agreement<br />

Agreement between categorical assessments compares the ability <strong>of</strong> different raters to<br />

classify subjects into one <strong>of</strong> several groups. <strong>The</strong> reason studies use 2 or more raters is<br />

usually to see if the raters agree well enough for one to replace another or for raters to be<br />

used interchangeably. One consideration which would improve the quality <strong>of</strong> some <strong>of</strong> the<br />

studies reported here would be the definition <strong>of</strong> what is meant by agreement, also the<br />

degree <strong>of</strong> agreement.<br />

Most reported measures <strong>of</strong> reliability gave an 'overall agreement' statistic, which<br />

took the mean <strong>of</strong> the summed agreement percentages for each element. It is not possible,<br />

<strong>of</strong> course, in many cases, to measure these quantities directly and the decision on what<br />

constitutes good enough agreement must lie with the clinical conditions. However, 6<br />

201<br />

W.L. Wadlington and H.J. McWhinnie (1973) <strong>The</strong> development <strong>of</strong> a rating scale for the study <strong>of</strong> formal<br />

aesthetic qualities in the paintings <strong>of</strong> mental patients, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.1(3-4):210-20.<br />

158


studies did not even quote figures, 4 because they were too poor (50, 12, 10, 14). <strong>The</strong><br />

other 3 reported categories <strong>of</strong> 'very good' to 'acceptable' agreement (9, 11, 39; study 39<br />

reported that 3 raters own criteria judgements <strong>of</strong> patient status, from 200 paintings was<br />

10% better than chance but gives no other figures, although we know (study 14) that own<br />

criteria judgements are based on widely differing individual values. Significant agreement<br />

is reported, but with no indication <strong>of</strong> how this was arrived at). No study justified their<br />

cut <strong>of</strong>f points for 'good agreement' and these varied considerably from study to study.<br />

Worse, perhaps than no figures, a good percentage <strong>of</strong> these reliability studies are<br />

mis-analysed 202 . In p<strong>art</strong>icular, the correlation between the values reported by individual<br />

raters or groups <strong>of</strong> raters is calculated in 9 studies (75, 35, 37, 38, 31, 10, 44, 21, 3), with<br />

a high value <strong>of</strong> r interpreted as an indication <strong>of</strong> good agreement. Correlation is an<br />

inappropriate analysis, firstly because the correlation coefficient is a measure <strong>of</strong> the<br />

strength <strong>of</strong> linear association between two variables, not agreement. Agreement is<br />

assessed directly. Secondly, there may be a high degree <strong>of</strong> correlation when the agreement<br />

is clinically poor, as recognised by the actions <strong>of</strong> the authors <strong>of</strong> study 37, who used 7<br />

terms with correlations below 0.31 because agreement was clinically high (but didn't drop<br />

any clinically low ones). A high value <strong>of</strong> r can be obtained because, as for studies 3, 37,<br />

38 and 31, there is large variation between subjects. <strong>The</strong> authors <strong>of</strong> study 37/38<br />

recognised large differences between subjects in 37. <strong>The</strong>y used a much more rigidly<br />

defined group <strong>of</strong> subjects for study 38 and a higher cut <strong>of</strong>f point for the correlation, to<br />

202<br />

Much <strong>of</strong> the information here is quoted from D.G. Altman (1994) Practical Statistics for Medical<br />

Research , (4th reprint, original 1991), London: Chapman Hall.<br />

159


indicate greater agreement (even though their earlier study had recognised the correlation<br />

was not a good indicator <strong>of</strong> good agreement). It is clearly not reasonable to assess<br />

agreement by a statistical method that is highly sensitive to the choice <strong>of</strong> the sample <strong>of</strong><br />

subjects. Similarly a famous and well quoted study 203 (33) incorrectly judged agreement<br />

by a _ 2 test which is also a test <strong>of</strong> association.<br />

Another incorrect analysis appears in a well quoted study using the comparison<br />

<strong>of</strong> means by a paired t-test, which is a hypothesis test (69). Similarly <strong>Study</strong> 56 204 used<br />

60 judges in groups <strong>of</strong> 10, to rate 5 pictures and compared the variation between scores<br />

<strong>of</strong> 0-10 agreements between groups, using Friedman's Anova, which although it is a<br />

category ranking test, is yet another test <strong>of</strong> association. <strong>The</strong>y found few significant<br />

differences and drew up a table <strong>of</strong> poor to good agreement categories.<br />

Methods cannot be deduced to agree well because they are not significantly<br />

different. A high scatter <strong>of</strong> differences may well lead to a crucial difference in means<br />

(bias) being non significant. Using this approach, worse agreement decreases the chance<br />

<strong>of</strong> finding a significant difference and so increases the chance that the methods will appear<br />

to agree. Despite the authors' claims <strong>of</strong> good statistical agreement in study 69, most <strong>of</strong><br />

the discussion reported their difficulties with the measure seriously affected their study<br />

results and recommended a shorter form for better reliability.<br />

<strong>The</strong> simplest approach is to see how many exact agreements exist. 7 studies<br />

reported percentage agreement by tables <strong>of</strong> elements or overall agreement. <strong>The</strong><br />

203 S.R. Kay (1978) Qualitative differention in human figure drawings according to schizophrenic subtype,<br />

Perceptual Motor Skills , V.47:923-32.<br />

204 S. Russell Lacy et al. (1979) An experimental study <strong>of</strong> pictures produced by acute schizophrenics<br />

160


percentage agreement figures look reasonably high but can be unreliable when more raters<br />

are added (see for example, study 48 and 49: 95.7% agreement for 2 raters decreased to<br />

77% for 29; studies 58 and 59: 94.3% agreement for 2 raters decreased to 61% for 10;<br />

studies 9 and 10: 'good' agreement for 2 raters decreased to 'poor' agreement for 4). <strong>Study</strong><br />

46 and study 61 report figures <strong>of</strong> '0.96' and '0.97' respectively, which, it is assumed,<br />

represents percentage agreement as there is no other information.<br />

<strong>The</strong> DDS 205 merits some consideration, under this heading, as one <strong>of</strong> few tests<br />

which attempt to validate, reliably rate their instrument and encourage replications.<br />

Described as a "standardised evaluation supported by extensive research" 206 , only 3<br />

interrater studies have been included in this analysis: study 48 207 reports agreement<br />

scores from 77-100% over 23 categories, giving 95.7% overall after "2 months training"<br />

<strong>of</strong> the 2 main authors rating 30 sets <strong>of</strong> drawings by undescribed subjects. <strong>Study</strong> 49<br />

reports only 77% agreement between 29 naive raters performing the same measurements.<br />

<strong>Study</strong> 52 reports 96% agreement between raters <strong>of</strong> 4 details in tree drawings, by 30<br />

patients with post traumatic dissociative disorder and 30 controls, taken from the DDS<br />

rating guide and protocol. Other studies used peculiar methods and were not included in<br />

this analysis.<br />

subjects British J. Psychiatry , V.134:195-200.<br />

205 Diagnostic Drawing Series, Cohen et al (1988) op.cit.<br />

206<br />

B.M. Cohen, A. Mills, A.K. Kijak (1994) An Introduction to the DDS: a standardized tool for<br />

diagnostic and clinical use, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.11(2):105-10.<br />

207 Mills et al (1993) Reliability and Validity studies, op.cit.<br />

161


Two weaknesses lie in the simple calculation <strong>of</strong> agreement; there is no account <strong>of</strong><br />

where in the table the agreement was and secondly we would expect some agreement<br />

between raters by chance. A more reasonable answer is obtained by considering<br />

agreement in excess <strong>of</strong> the amount by chance, which is only attempted by one study (39),<br />

and lacks other figures. <strong>The</strong> best approach to this type <strong>of</strong> problem is that adopted by<br />

studies 44 and 36, the kappa statistic, which may be interpreted as the chance corrected<br />

proportional agreement, but it is important to show the raw data (which they don't). In<br />

support <strong>of</strong> this statement, Neale's application <strong>of</strong> the DDS to children 208 , found a much<br />

lower level <strong>of</strong> reliability than that reported by Mills 209 : only 12 variables reached<br />

significance using the Kappa measure <strong>of</strong> agreement between 2 raters.<br />

Conclusion<br />

Generally, specific decisions are more reliable than global.<br />

Content decisions were most reliable where objective, because they were more specific<br />

and probably easier to define and rate. Most categories contained subjective decisions<br />

and largely where these were global they were unreliable. This is p<strong>art</strong>icularly exemplified<br />

in the category <strong>of</strong> Quality, where personal judgement was not reliable because experience<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> was clearly an influence on aesthetic appeal. Global subjective decisions became<br />

more reliable when not referenced to the image, but suspicions must arise that the rating<br />

208<br />

E.L. Neale (1994) <strong>The</strong> Children's DDS, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.11(2):119-26, but not included in this review<br />

because the subjects were children.<br />

209 Mills et al. (1993) op.cit.<br />

162


has then little to do with the <strong>art</strong>work itself. <strong>The</strong> tables show discriminatory properties<br />

for drawing areas.<br />

Alzheimer's disease and brain damage tended to be discriminable on similar indices in<br />

7/13 categories: omitting more essentials, poor quality, bizarre content, poor reality and<br />

logic and poor integration, although brain damage was p<strong>art</strong>icularly distinguishable in<br />

drawing angles, so it is reasonable to suppose they share similar qualities which are<br />

p<strong>art</strong>icularly apparent in their <strong>art</strong>work.<br />

Normal controls were also discriminable in 8/10 categories, generally supplying more<br />

complete paintings without morbid content.<br />

Schizophrenics or psychotics paintings were discriminable from controls through empty<br />

space in the picture, on their inclusion <strong>of</strong> emotional indicators (which two characteristics<br />

also distinguished within types), and in global measurement <strong>of</strong> regression.<br />

It is also worth noting that against all commonly believed theory, emotional indicators<br />

drawn as body details did not distinguish sex abuse, but thought disorder.<br />

This organisation <strong>of</strong> the literature has helped to distinguish reliable categories <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>art</strong> variables from non-reliable by contrasting similar variables measured by different<br />

studies. However, the observed validity <strong>of</strong> the drawing procedure is delimited by the<br />

clinical relevance and reliability <strong>of</strong> the selected drawing features. <strong>The</strong>re are limitations to<br />

those studies; they use small numbers, their interpretation <strong>of</strong> the figures is questionable,<br />

by and large they are unreplicated, and those few which are replicated seem to produce<br />

inconsistent results. This is consistent with the overall findings reported at the beginning<br />

<strong>of</strong> this chapter. It is now necessary to further investigate the utility <strong>of</strong> the classifications<br />

163


<strong>of</strong> drawing areas and to quantify whether reported discriminations <strong>of</strong> patients are<br />

clinically meaningful and experimentally sound.<br />

164


Chapter 3: Positive Thinking: what are the common<br />

psychiatric characteristics <strong>of</strong> paintings?<br />

Chapter 3 summarises the review to identify the central importance <strong>of</strong> developing<br />

systematic, content-free assessments <strong>of</strong> psychiatric patients' paintings.<br />

Firstly the findings from the literature review showed the kinds <strong>of</strong> measurement<br />

which had been employed and had the best repeatability for a suitable test. <strong>The</strong>se<br />

selected papers will now be subjected to a further analysis designed to make some sense<br />

<strong>of</strong> their contradictions and put them on an equal footing, so as to find out what kind <strong>of</strong><br />

effects to expect. <strong>The</strong>n, the development <strong>of</strong> a novel instrument (the DAPA) for such<br />

assessments is presented.<br />

Meta Analysis <strong>of</strong> reliable studies identified by the literature review<br />

Analysis <strong>of</strong> tabulated categories and variables from the empirical literature was performed<br />

in 2 ways:<br />

1. <strong>The</strong> reliability and the validity <strong>of</strong> this classification system was qualitatively<br />

assessed in Chapter 2, by discussion and comparison between all studies on<br />

similar variables. This process <strong>of</strong> simplifying and displaying common themes<br />

was exploratory.<br />

2. A meta analysis was performed on studies that conformed to the minimum<br />

scientific criteria for <strong>quantitative</strong> work to complement the discursive assessment.<br />

165


Is Meta Analysis appropriate for this literature<br />

<strong>The</strong> discursive summary was not as informative as it might have been either with respect<br />

to summarised significance levels or with respect to summarised category tables, because<br />

it reflected the conclusions <strong>of</strong> the studies, which tend to provide equivocal answers to<br />

imprecise questions. Research environments are difficult to control, common definitions<br />

are not always available nor accepted, and methods, techniques and sampling<br />

characteristics vary from study to study. This situation is made more difficult by the<br />

proliferation <strong>of</strong> studies that address common research questions (e.g. is there a difference<br />

between pictures by abnormal groups and pictures by normal controls), but do not report<br />

essentials such as definitions or reliability <strong>of</strong> variables, sample sizes, statistical methods<br />

(many report 'significant' results, but not the number <strong>of</strong> tests, which variables were tested<br />

and how many dropped), or even fully report the characteristics <strong>of</strong> their experimental<br />

group. Furthermore their literature reviews are notorious for depending on the subjective<br />

judgments, preferences and biases <strong>of</strong> the reviewers. Conflicting interpretation <strong>of</strong> the<br />

evidence is common and consistent 210 .<br />

<strong>The</strong>re is no lack <strong>of</strong> literature but the study area shows what Rosenthal 211 calls<br />

poor cumulation, lack <strong>of</strong> orderly development building directly on the older work. Each<br />

study seems to replicate the same process to produce conflicting results which can lead<br />

210<br />

See for instance: E. Ulman and B.I. Levy (1974) An Experimental Approach to the Judgement <strong>of</strong><br />

Psychopathology from Paintings, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.8:3-12 (reprinted 1975, 1984 and 1992) although<br />

their results showed that health workers scored no differently and some people with no experience <strong>of</strong><br />

psychiatric paintings were more accurate, they concluded that diagnostic classification <strong>of</strong> pictures was a<br />

skill which could be taught and this opinion has been related through the later literature as a proven fact.<br />

211<br />

Robert Rosenthal (1984), Meta Analytic Procedures for Social Research , Beverley Hills, CA: Sage, p.9-<br />

10.<br />

166


to no acceptable answers but conclude with calls for further research. This literature,<br />

despite its heavy emphasis on qualitative reports, is an appropriate candidate for meta-<br />

analysis.<br />

Meta analysis is the application <strong>of</strong> statistical procedures to collections <strong>of</strong><br />

empirical findings from individual studies for the purpose <strong>of</strong> integrating, synthesizing and<br />

making sense <strong>of</strong> them. A common metric aggregates diverse statistics across studies, and<br />

standardised methods help to produce an unbiased assessment <strong>of</strong> the reliability <strong>of</strong> a<br />

variable measured across studies. It addresses five methodological difficulties which have<br />

been identified with traditional literature reviews 212<br />

(1) selective inclusion <strong>of</strong> studies <strong>of</strong>ten based on the reviewer's impressionistic view <strong>of</strong> the<br />

quality <strong>of</strong> the study;<br />

(2) differential subjective weighting <strong>of</strong> studies in the interpretation <strong>of</strong> a set <strong>of</strong> findings;<br />

(3) misleading interpretations <strong>of</strong> study findings;<br />

(4) failure to examine characteristics <strong>of</strong> the studies as potential explanations for disparate<br />

or consistent results across studies;<br />

(5) failure to examine moderating variables in the relationship.<br />

Selection <strong>of</strong> studies for Meta-analysis<br />

<strong>The</strong> first criterion for analysis <strong>of</strong> absolute differences between groups was that terms<br />

should be reliable. From the pool <strong>of</strong> 51 controlled studies, 35 employed no assessment<br />

or indication <strong>of</strong> reliability, therefore any bias would be overwhelming. Because weighting<br />

212<br />

Summarised by Frederic M. Wolf (1986), Meta Analysis: <strong>quantitative</strong> methods for research synthesis ,<br />

Beverley Hills, CA: Sage, p.10.<br />

167


techniques which compensate for unreliability <strong>of</strong> variables 213 require information which<br />

is <strong>of</strong>ten unavailable from these studies, and because there was considerable variability in<br />

definition <strong>of</strong> terms, I have decided that this technique would require too many estimations<br />

<strong>of</strong> quantities which are not predictable in this range <strong>of</strong> studies and therefore be unreliable<br />

and inappropriate 214 .<br />

Method <strong>of</strong> selection <strong>of</strong> studies<br />

31 studies from the pool <strong>of</strong> controlled studies, series uncontrolled studies, validity and<br />

reliability studies employed more than one rater. <strong>The</strong> numbers assigned to them in<br />

Chapter 2 215 were retained through the further elimination procedures and eventual<br />

analysis. 6 studies (11, 12, 39, 43, 65, 66) were eliminated because they reported 'hit<br />

rates' rather than reliability between raters: that is, they compared the rating with the<br />

actual diagnostic group, but not between raters. 25 studies were retained for further<br />

analysis.<br />

Replication studies which tested the reliability <strong>of</strong> former studies and proved<br />

them unreliable (9, 14, 50, 10, 37) were eliminated and those which did not compare<br />

groups on diagnosis (58, 59, 48, 49, 31, 20, 61), together with 2 studies which included<br />

no figures for analysis (3, 52; study 3 did provide some figures for suicide groups <strong>of</strong><br />

patients with personality disorder against non-suicides, but gave no indication <strong>of</strong> the<br />

213<br />

J.E. Hunter, F.L. Schmidt and G.B. Jackson (1982), Meta Analysis: cumulating research findings<br />

across studies , Beverley Hills, CA: Sage.<br />

214 Rosenthal (1984) also considers this procedure too burdensome.<br />

215 see Table <strong>of</strong> Authorities for numbers assigned to all control, validity and reliability studies, Appendix<br />

3. Studies selected for meta analysis are marked (M).<br />

168


ange <strong>of</strong> tests administered. <strong>The</strong>y also presented figures for patients cross classified by<br />

diagnosis, but they gave no group numbers or indication <strong>of</strong> the range <strong>of</strong> tests performed).<br />

This left a total <strong>of</strong> 11 studies (69, 56, 75, 35, 46, 44, 21, 57, 38, 36, 33) for meta analysis<br />

<strong>of</strong> variables between group categories <strong>of</strong> measurable drawing elements.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were 5 further dep<strong>art</strong>ures from the tabulated discursive elements between<br />

these 11 studies:<br />

1. Unreliable variables were removed from the analysis.<br />

2. Non significant results were included in the analysis. <strong>The</strong> procedure for<br />

studies which did not quote figures for non-significant variables (studies<br />

38, 69, 56 216 , 35, and 44) was to assume a significance value <strong>of</strong> 1.000 217<br />

(<strong>Study</strong> 38 did note frequent signs and these are marked and noted in the<br />

table).<br />

3. <strong>Study</strong> 44 reported p levels only for their positive results, and these were<br />

converted to Z scores and then into effect sizes, along with results from<br />

tests other than Chi-square or t-tests, which were converted directly to<br />

effect sizes 218 .<br />

4. <strong>Study</strong> 36 and 46 listed but did not test individual variables (they were<br />

grouped and discussed with the other tests under the tabulated areas <strong>of</strong><br />

216 <strong>Study</strong> 56 also reported significant results from other tests on covered space, yellow, blending and<br />

variety <strong>of</strong> colour, but gave no reliability figures or indication <strong>of</strong> the range <strong>of</strong> tests which proved<br />

nonsignificant, so these results were not included.<br />

217 Recommended by Rosenthal (1984) op.cit. p.33.<br />

218 Rosenthal (1984) op.cit. Recommended procedure to convert t or _ 2 to effect size - using broad theory:<br />

Test <strong>of</strong> = Size <strong>of</strong> x Size <strong>of</strong> p.20-21 gives examples <strong>of</strong> relationships and equations.<br />

significance Effect <strong>Study</strong><br />

169


drawing evaluation with comments from the studies where frequent and<br />

significant predictors were indicated). Tests performed are included here<br />

under category headings as a total or category score. <strong>Study</strong> 57 provided<br />

only a total score which was included as a single variable in the analysis.<br />

5. Each study tested differences between patients and normal controls or<br />

between a defined group <strong>of</strong> patients and another group <strong>of</strong> patient<br />

controls, although there were some which tested both. In order to assess<br />

differences between the effect size <strong>of</strong> patient/nonpatient and<br />

patient/patient, each result was calculated separately (since there were<br />

two tests done).<br />

This analysis was not intended as a confirmatory statement, but as a synthesis and an<br />

integration <strong>of</strong> the tabulated information which tried to assess the reliability <strong>of</strong> research<br />

findings across several studies. It takes into account 4 criticisms <strong>of</strong> meta analyses<br />

identified by Glass 219 :<br />

1. No logical comparison can be made between dissimilar techniques<br />

<strong>of</strong> measurement and operational definitions.<br />

Tabulation <strong>of</strong> the information from several studies into similar areas goes some way<br />

towards analogising definitions <strong>of</strong> variables and subjects from studies which used<br />

different measuring techniques, that were previously too dissimilar in scope to compare<br />

equally.<br />

219<br />

Gene V. Glass, Barry McGraw, Mary L. Smith (1981) Meta Analysis in Social Rese arch , Beverley<br />

Hills, CA: Sage.<br />

170


2. Results are uninterpretable between studies using poor designs and<br />

those with good.<br />

Rather than making statistical compensation for poor studies, only studies which used<br />

reliability measures for their terms were used. Design otherwise was equally poor.<br />

3. Published research is biased in favour <strong>of</strong> significant findings<br />

because nonsignificant findings are rarely published: the 'File<br />

Drawer Problem'. 220<br />

Checks were made on unpublished controlled studies through private correspondence;<br />

there is every reason to believe that unknown unpublished studies <strong>of</strong> other types than<br />

the two which are used in this review would show similar conflicting viewpoints, poor<br />

design and statistical rigour as those published which are subject to peer review. <strong>The</strong>re<br />

does not seem to be a lack <strong>of</strong> published nonsignificant findings, so there may be little bias<br />

in this field. A test was applied following procedures from Wolf (1986) 221 which<br />

estimated how many additional studies with nonsignificant results would be necessary<br />

to reverse the conclusion drawn, providing some estimate <strong>of</strong> the robustness and validity<br />

<strong>of</strong> the findings.<br />

4. Multiple results used from the same study bias or invalidate the<br />

meta analysis and make the results appear more reliable than they<br />

really are because they are not independent.<br />

220<br />

Not everyone is agreed on whether this point exists: a recent discussion <strong>of</strong> the various issues as to how<br />

to estimate the proportion <strong>of</strong> conflicting evidence, in M.T. Bradley, R.D. Gupta (1997), Estimating the<br />

Effect <strong>of</strong> the File Drawer Problem in Meta Analysis, Perceptual and Motor Skills , V.65(2_:719-22. I<br />

followed Rosenthal's recommended checks (1984) op.cit. p.107-110.<br />

221<br />

Wolf (1986) Meta Analysis, op.cit. <strong>The</strong> 'fail-safe N' p.37-39, which was simpler than the calculation<br />

from Rosenthal.<br />

171


<strong>The</strong>re are few answers to this criticism which do not lead to possible inferential errors,<br />

and it is a question <strong>of</strong> judgement in using a procedure which is appropriate for the sample<br />

and reflects the classification and variety <strong>of</strong> variables encompassed.<br />

Wolf 222 describes a method using only the most significant results from each<br />

study, but this technique lends itself to type I error. Rosenthal's 223 adaption <strong>of</strong> the<br />

Stouffer method, i.e. averaging between significance levels transformed to z-scores to<br />

produce one variable per study, are clearly not appropriate here because the standard<br />

deviations and direction <strong>of</strong> significance <strong>of</strong> studies must be similar, and the variables non-<br />

correlated, so that they can be weighted equally 224 . Furthermore, although 4 from 11<br />

studies show a very high effect size aggregated in this way and 3 more show a small to<br />

medium effect, 3 studies did not actually report the large numbers <strong>of</strong> non-significant<br />

statistics, <strong>of</strong> which their study mostly consisted, and therefore estimates <strong>of</strong> p=1 would<br />

probably contribute to <strong>art</strong>ificially low averages resulting in a much too conservative<br />

estimate.<br />

Kulik, Rosenthal and others recommend using separate analyses for each variable,<br />

when each study uses several <strong>of</strong> the same type <strong>of</strong> variables. <strong>The</strong>re were two main<br />

problems which made this type <strong>of</strong> analysis difficult in this study: (1) <strong>The</strong>re were tests<br />

between patients and normal controls and between patients and patient controls; and (2)<br />

there was no way <strong>of</strong> knowing how similar variables with similar terms, which were<br />

tabulated together, actually were. Few studies contributed variables to most <strong>of</strong> the<br />

222 Wolf (1986) op.cit. p.46.<br />

223 Rosenthal (1984) op.cit. p.72.<br />

172


tabulated drawing areas, so some studies measured areas completely neglected by others<br />

and there were no tables to which all studies contributed. <strong>The</strong> p<strong>art</strong>ition into tabulated<br />

areas was useful for the qualitative p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the analysis, but more than one variable from<br />

most studies described aspects within the same area, and there were no grounds to<br />

assume these variables were correlated.<br />

Glass et al. 225 used multiple tests from the same studies in a single analysis, but<br />

this technique is not popular 226 , since multiple results are said to inflate the sample size<br />

and effects beyond the number <strong>of</strong> individual studies 227 and increase the power <strong>of</strong> the<br />

meta-analysis. <strong>The</strong> studies analysed here though were unrepresentative <strong>of</strong> the majority<br />

<strong>of</strong> studies in this field, but their variables are typical <strong>of</strong> the kind <strong>of</strong> constructs usually<br />

employed.<br />

<strong>The</strong> practical answer, to eliminate the variables that did not fit clearly affects the<br />

type <strong>of</strong> study included and the results; if the results within a study are averaged, it<br />

precludes analytical examination <strong>of</strong> differences and similarities for different categories <strong>of</strong><br />

outcome and tends to increase the chance <strong>of</strong> type I error 228 . Furthermore, meta analytic<br />

methods have recently been criticised on this basis, that treatment trials pay less<br />

224 Rosenthal (1984) op.cit. p.33; Wolf (1986) op.cit. p.36-37.<br />

225<br />

M. Smith & G. Glass, (1980) Meta Analysis <strong>of</strong> research on class size and its relationship to attitudes<br />

and instruction, Am. Educational Research J. , V.17:419-33; Glass et al., (1981) op.cit.<br />

226 for example: Rosenthal, (1984) op.cit.; Wolf (1986) op.cit.<br />

227<br />

J. Kulik (1983), Review <strong>of</strong> G.V. Glass et al. (1981) op.cit. Evaluation News , V.4:101-5, but the studies<br />

represented here comprise only a tiny proportion <strong>of</strong> the field.<br />

228 M. Strube (1985) Combining and comparing significance levels from non-independent hypothesis tests,<br />

Psychological Bulletin V.97:334-341.<br />

173


attention to overall effect sizes than the difference between individual studies 229 . <strong>The</strong><br />

complex issue <strong>of</strong> which set <strong>of</strong> statistics to use is largely a matter <strong>of</strong> judging the structure<br />

<strong>of</strong> the data and applying as fair a method as possible 230 . This problem was dealt with<br />

empirically by coding the characteristics for each study and comparing the different<br />

p<strong>art</strong>itions <strong>of</strong> the variables 231 as tabulated in the qualitative analysis.<br />

Tables were prepared using 2 techniques:<br />

(1) all variables were included to produce a single aggregated case for each<br />

experimental group in the analysis;<br />

(2) the identified tabulated areas were compared on effect size and subsets <strong>of</strong> the<br />

most significant tables, which retained all studies in the analysis were aggregated<br />

for each type <strong>of</strong> control group;<br />

Other criticisms focus on interaction effects, <strong>of</strong> which 2 are taken into account in this<br />

analysis; small sample sizes and weighting for size <strong>of</strong> study 232 .<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were 4 questions to be answered:<br />

Is the <strong>art</strong>-test a discriminable dimension for psychiatric diagnosis for (1) patients<br />

from non-patients; and (2) within patient groups.<br />

229<br />

David Healy (1998), Commentary: meta analysis <strong>of</strong> trails comparing anti-depressants with active<br />

placebos, British J. Psychiatry , V.17:232-4; David Sohn (1997), Questions for meta analysis,<br />

Psychological Reports , V.81(1):3-15.<br />

230<br />

R. Rosenthal (1998) Meta analysis: concepts, corollaries and controversies, in J.G. Adair and D.<br />

Bellanger (eds.) Advances in Psychological Science , V.1:371-384.<br />

231<br />

Rosenthal (1984) op.cit; Wolf (1986) op.cit. and using procedures for calculation from Carol Taylor<br />

Fitzgibbon and Lynn Lyons Morris (1987), How to Analyse Data , Beverley Hills, CA: Sage, Chapter 7.<br />

Meta Analysis pp.132-145.<br />

232 <strong>The</strong> major criticism <strong>of</strong> bias in meta analyses <strong>of</strong> treatment methods, Wolf (1986) op.cit.<br />

174


Do different variables show agreement in effect sizes? and if not: (3) which<br />

drawing area showed greater effect; and (4) was there a greater effect for form or<br />

content, objective or subjective variables?<br />

Statistical Procedure<br />

Mathematical procedures were facilitated by the use <strong>of</strong> SPSS, version 4 for DOS.<br />

Treatment <strong>of</strong> the data<br />

Effect sizes (_) were calculated for each variable from z scores, Chi-square or t-<br />

tests as described, between patient/normal controls and between patient/patient<br />

controls from the results given in the papers. Cohen's d, 233 the usual statistic was<br />

adopted throughout, with a confidence interval <strong>of</strong> 95% to indicate a significant<br />

relationship, if zero was not encompassed, at the 0.05 level.<br />

All reliable variables were included to produce a single aggregated case for each<br />

drawing area according to control group (1) patient/nonpatient and (2)<br />

patient/patient;<br />

Effect sizes <strong>of</strong> the identified tablulated drawing areas were aggregated and<br />

compared between patient/nonpatient and patient/patient controls and subsets<br />

<strong>of</strong> the most significant tables, which retained all studies in the analysis were also<br />

compared;<br />

Form and content areas were compared on aggregation <strong>of</strong> effect size as were<br />

subjective and objective areas.<br />

233<br />

J. Cohen (1977) Statistical Power Analyses for the Behavioral Sciences , New York: Ac.Press, p.20,<br />

methods described in Wolf (1986) op.cit.<br />

175


<strong>The</strong> 'File Drawer Problem' (refer back to [3] criticisms <strong>of</strong> meta-analysis), was dealt with<br />

in the manner recommended by Wolf (1986) using Orwin's (1983) fail-safe N 234 , selecting<br />

d=0.2 (small effect) as the criterion value. This provided an estimate <strong>of</strong> the number <strong>of</strong><br />

variables necessary to reverse the conclusion <strong>of</strong> a significant relationship and thus <strong>of</strong> the<br />

robustness <strong>of</strong> the validity <strong>of</strong> the findings.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were 2 compensations made:<br />

(1) <strong>The</strong> Hedges and Olkin 235 adjustment for small sample sizes, which is appropriate<br />

since some groups were under 10 members;<br />

(2) the weighted _ (wd) technique 236 which produces an unbiased estimate <strong>of</strong> effect size<br />

for the corrected group sizes.<br />

Validity <strong>of</strong> drawing area Classification<br />

<strong>The</strong> usual procedure to test the validity <strong>of</strong> my assignment <strong>of</strong> variables to the<br />

drawing area categories would be a test <strong>of</strong> equivalence <strong>of</strong> proportion to indicate the<br />

homogeneity <strong>of</strong> effect size for each variable and their relation 237 . However, this was<br />

inappropriate here because the vast majority <strong>of</strong> the non-significant results were not<br />

available, so the assumption <strong>of</strong> p=1 creates a false disparity between the significant and<br />

non-significant findings, imposing hetereogeneity.<br />

234 using Wolf's (1986) op.cit. p.39, suggestion from Orwin (1983) fail-safe N for the average effect size<br />

that can be obtained, selecting d=0.2 (small effect) as the criterion value:<br />

Nfs. = No. <strong>of</strong> variables with an effect size


Results<br />

Row 1 <strong>of</strong> Table 1 presents the results <strong>of</strong> an aggregated analysis <strong>of</strong> the total number <strong>of</strong><br />

variables included in the analysis which related to performance by patients against normal<br />

controls and row 2 against patient controls. It can be seen that the confidence levels do<br />

not encompass zero, so we can assume a significance level beyond chance expectation<br />

(p


Table 2. All variables for 11 studies from the tabulated 15 different areas <strong>of</strong> drawing<br />

measurement.<br />

Drawing<br />

Area 239<br />

1<br />

2*<br />

3<br />

4<br />

5<br />

6<br />

7<br />

8<br />

9*<br />

10*<br />

11*<br />

12<br />

13<br />

14<br />

15<br />

ES1: Effect size for<br />

patient/normal controls<br />

(confidence intervals)<br />

and p values.<br />

0.0000 (-.2922; 0.2922) NS<br />

0.8795 (0.6362; 1.1228) p


(2) For variables discriminating between patient groups but not between patients and<br />

controls, diagnosed groups <strong>of</strong> psychiatric patients differed in: thematic content or subject<br />

<strong>of</strong> the picture; used less or different qualities <strong>of</strong> line, less or different colour groups, their<br />

pictures depicted reality or abstraction and were more or less complex.<br />

Table 3 shows the aggregation <strong>of</strong> all the variables over 14 drawing areas<br />

p<strong>art</strong>itioned at whether they were categorised as content, subjective or objective, or<br />

form 241 . Subjective variables seemed to produce the largest effect, but there were<br />

demonstrable if small effects for the other two types <strong>of</strong> variable.<br />

Table 3. Aggregated results for patients/all controls by form or content variables<br />

Variable type Effect Size (_)<br />

Subjective content 1<br />

Objective content 2<br />

Form variables 3<br />

Confidence limits<br />

for _ (all significances<br />

p


Less than a third <strong>of</strong> the drawing areas both discriminated patients from controls<br />

and within diagnostic group. Five drawing areas, which measured subjective content<br />

information, were sensitive to general differentiation <strong>of</strong> patients and normal controls, but<br />

were insensitive to diagnostic differentiation. Five drawing areas which measured<br />

objective information were sensitive to specific diagnostic differentiation, but were<br />

insensitive to general discriminations between patients and normal controls. This<br />

suggests a major conceptual flaw in constructs common across the field <strong>of</strong> investigation<br />

explaining the schematics and cognitive constructs <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>work, such as in the description<br />

<strong>of</strong> the work <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy and in psychological drawing tests relating to <strong>art</strong>istic<br />

<strong>psychopathology</strong>.<br />

Furthermore the failure to include non-significant findings in the results and<br />

conclusions <strong>of</strong> much <strong>of</strong> the literature suggests that current applications <strong>of</strong> research<br />

findings to the assessment <strong>of</strong> psychiatric <strong>art</strong>work lack construct validity and predictive<br />

validity.<br />

Form and Content variables<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was a demonstrable form effect and it was significant, but low. Against all<br />

predictions from <strong>art</strong> theory, observational case and series studies and narrative personal<br />

experiences by therapists, p<strong>art</strong>icularly <strong>of</strong> schizophrenics, there was a failure to show<br />

specific effect <strong>of</strong> form superior to content. However, there were twice as many content<br />

variables; formal variables were <strong>of</strong>ten inappropriate for the eventual type <strong>of</strong> analysis,<br />

were discordant and patchy.<br />

180


Conclusion<br />

<strong>The</strong> type <strong>of</strong> analysis used was the most appropriate for the data; all indications<br />

were that other methods would have introduced more methodological discrepancies.<br />

Despite everything, there are strong indications <strong>of</strong> an effect which differentiates<br />

not only patients from controls (which could be predicted from environment), but also,<br />

different diagnostic classes <strong>of</strong> patients. <strong>The</strong> large observational literature and reports<br />

from <strong>art</strong> therapists predict much larger effects, suggesting very serious measurement<br />

problems.<br />

Summary <strong>of</strong> the findings <strong>of</strong> the literature review<br />

Generally, it seems that constructs common across the field <strong>of</strong> investigation into the<br />

schematics and cognitive constructs <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>work, such as in the description <strong>of</strong> the work <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>art</strong> therapy and in psychological drawing tests are only slightly related to <strong>art</strong>istic<br />

<strong>psychopathology</strong>. Failure to demonstrate previous published systematic studies<br />

suggests that current applications <strong>of</strong> the assessment <strong>of</strong> psychiatric <strong>art</strong>work therefore lack<br />

construct validity and predictive validity. Thus we cannot talk about psychopathic <strong>art</strong><br />

or <strong>of</strong> defined marker areas.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se findings support the introductory statement that we know nothing about<br />

the phenomenology <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> from psychiatric patients, and it is shown all through the<br />

review that, contrary to popular opinion, there has been a complete failure to understand<br />

the system. It is clear that there is an urgent need for immediate work on the design <strong>of</strong><br />

more suitable, sensitive and psychodynamic evaluation measures for psychiatric <strong>art</strong>work.<br />

181


Case study and controlled research has largely ignored cautions and tried to<br />

codify symbols or signs, relationships between, and distortion <strong>of</strong>, images by relating<br />

explanations to a theory <strong>of</strong> the unconscious in an apparent attempt to produce an<br />

analogical translation that was largely unsuccessful.<br />

Case studies <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>ists were more concerned with the debate as to intentionality<br />

and generalisability <strong>of</strong> illustrations <strong>of</strong> what has been seen as <strong>psychopathology</strong> to other<br />

psychiatric populations. This has also been largely unsuccessful.<br />

<strong>The</strong> general direction <strong>of</strong> the impressionistic or discursive literature indicates that<br />

only the patient has access to signs. <strong>The</strong> job <strong>of</strong> the therapist is to gain access to that<br />

very personal language and delicately manipulate it. But unless the language is universal,<br />

or at least common to a p<strong>art</strong>icular group, which most research insists that it isn't, it is<br />

futile to try and ch<strong>art</strong> it except retrospectively to extract general principles for the<br />

ch<strong>art</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> another individual course. This has now become the general direction <strong>of</strong> case<br />

study research.<br />

Research based on the idea that <strong>art</strong> is healing in itself, because it produces a<br />

remote tangible communication outside the patient but from the patient to himself, rather<br />

than from the therapist, tended toward the 'nurturing' case study. <strong>The</strong> emphasis on the<br />

therapeutic relationship relies on the assumption <strong>of</strong> communication through the <strong>art</strong>work<br />

as the main outcome for the integrity <strong>of</strong> the therapy as <strong>art</strong>-related. <strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> was not<br />

found to be inherently related to the therapy but provided a receptive atmosphere, which<br />

could presumably be achieved in other ways.<br />

182


<strong>The</strong> common focus has been in the interpretation <strong>of</strong> the meaning <strong>of</strong> a message but<br />

there are no agreements as to what form the message takes. Problems occur when the<br />

associations <strong>of</strong> the rater or therapist are not clearly distinguished from those <strong>of</strong> the<br />

patient, which likely alternative explanation is largely ignored by the literature. It is<br />

dangerous to make assumptions about content, especially when dealing with psychosis.<br />

<strong>The</strong> nature <strong>of</strong> the illness affects the verbal expulsions <strong>of</strong> psychotic people and therefore<br />

ratings based on what they say may be inconsistent and fallacious. <strong>The</strong> distinction<br />

between response and record is important because it is a primary assumption that what<br />

is measured comes from the patient, otherwise discriminatory properties could be due to<br />

other related issues than to what is described, thus producing erratic results.<br />

<strong>The</strong> majority <strong>of</strong> the literature is <strong>of</strong> poor quality especially the reliability and<br />

consistency <strong>of</strong> terminology in previous studies because though what is measured across<br />

studies is described similarly, for example: line quality, heaviness <strong>of</strong> line, <strong>art</strong>iculation,<br />

what is understood by the term digresses from study to study. If we cannot assume<br />

studies measure the same aspects because they use the same terms, studies, or variables<br />

within studies, are not comparable.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was a demonstrable and significant form effect, although it was low.<br />

Content measures, such as bizarre imagery, disconnections, inappropriate or disordered<br />

colour, perseveration, inessential detail and subject matter require judgments <strong>of</strong> intention<br />

or meaning, are difficult to validate, difficult to define, unreliable and not specific to<br />

psychiatric populations. Formal characteristics may be seem to have differentiating<br />

183


potential for psychiatric populations, and have the advantage <strong>of</strong> being easier to define and<br />

rate 242 .<br />

<strong>The</strong> next stage, the development <strong>of</strong> a test takes on the problems <strong>of</strong> reliability,<br />

content validity and ambiguity <strong>of</strong> definition <strong>of</strong> the objective phenomena for the purpose<br />

<strong>of</strong> testing these findings. It dismissed, as far as possible, interpretation by the rater <strong>of</strong><br />

the intention <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong>ist, used simple terms relative to the work which were widely<br />

distinct from each other and the minimum number <strong>of</strong> categories to describe the work. <strong>The</strong><br />

Descriptive Assessment for Psychiatric Art (DAPA), uses a method which fragments the<br />

<strong>art</strong> object into a collection <strong>of</strong> mostly formal variables. This research tries to answer one<br />

important question which unfolded, but has not been fully answered from the review; is<br />

psychopathological evidence from <strong>art</strong>work sensitive?<br />

242 For example, Wadlington and McWhinnie (1973) revised their unreliable 18 variable scale which relied<br />

on aesthetic (and therefore content based) terms, to 4 formal dimensions which indicated reliable and<br />

distinguishable categories.<br />

184


Chapter Four: <strong>The</strong> Descriptive Assessment for Psychiatric Art (DAPA)<br />

<strong>The</strong> ideal Characteristics for a new test: development <strong>of</strong> the DAPA<br />

Differentiation <strong>of</strong> Form and Content<br />

Studies <strong>of</strong> <strong>psychopathology</strong> have stressed the importance <strong>of</strong> distinguishing between form<br />

and content 243 . Jaspers 244 explains:<br />

In all psychic experience there is a subject and an object. This objective element<br />

conceived in its widest sense we call psychic content and the mode in which the<br />

subject is presented with the object (be it as a perception, a mental image or<br />

thought) we call the form.<br />

Thus in describing how a subject is presented with the object (the content), we are<br />

concerned with the description <strong>of</strong> the form, or mode <strong>of</strong> presentation <strong>of</strong> the content; i.e.<br />

how the picture is made. Rating <strong>of</strong> content occurs when raters judge what is represented<br />

(e.g. 'unnatural' colour, abstraction, groundlines, movement, integration).<br />

<strong>The</strong>ory and justification for each DAPA Scale<br />

<strong>The</strong> purpose <strong>of</strong> this section is to present a table <strong>of</strong> mostly formal characteristics, likely<br />

to be reliable and valid, based on the literature analysis, which are said to exist in the<br />

<strong>art</strong>work <strong>of</strong> psychiatric patients in order to define the parameters <strong>of</strong> a test which premises<br />

are understood universally and can be compared <strong>quantitative</strong>ly. Positive characteristics<br />

243<br />

Karl Jaspers (1963) General Psychopathology , 7th Edition: translated, Manchester University Press;<br />

Andrew Sims (1988) Symptoms In <strong>The</strong> Mind: an introduction to descriptive <strong>psychopathology</strong> , London:<br />

Tindall.<br />

244 Jaspers (1963) op.cit., p.59.<br />

185


only are discussed (indicators which have been noted, rather than contradictory tests<br />

which have reached no significance) with no weight given to one characteristic above<br />

another.<br />

Content: themes, quality <strong>of</strong> picture and specific details<br />

Table 1 simplifies under three headings the positive findings and notations <strong>of</strong> all case and<br />

controlled studies considered in this review relating to the content <strong>of</strong> the picture. Most<br />

<strong>of</strong> these qualities could not be related to specific diagnoses and therefore could only be<br />

used in a limited way to differentiate patients from controls 245 . <strong>The</strong>y have a common<br />

negativity, except for a few manic patients (but these were small numbers n=5) and an<br />

odd finding for depressive pictures, <strong>of</strong> happy and complete work (but this may be related<br />

to treatment stage).<br />

Content characteristics were simplified to positive or negative content. <strong>The</strong> aim<br />

<strong>of</strong> the test, to eliminate interpretation <strong>of</strong> intentions, meant totally excluding content<br />

which would be a major omission and probably rather pig-headed. <strong>The</strong> reliability <strong>of</strong> an<br />

acknowledged ownership <strong>of</strong> an impression by the judge was probably better than a guess<br />

as to what the patient intended.<br />

245 as did study 31 on distorted figures and 56 on lack <strong>of</strong> detail (except schizophrenics).<br />

186


Table 1: to show the simplified positive findings from case and control studies reviewed<br />

relating to content <strong>of</strong> picture.<br />

Type <strong>The</strong>me or subject matter Quality <strong>of</strong> picture Detail<br />

Psychosis/ Schizophrenia<br />

Brain Damage<br />

Mania<br />

Depression<br />

Anxiety<br />

Personality<br />

Disorder<br />

1. case studies<br />

2. controlled studies<br />

3. <strong>art</strong>ists studies<br />

threatening; religious or conflict 1<br />

illustrating symptoms 13<br />

problem solving 3 ,<br />

separation, individuation 1<br />

repeated numbers 1<br />

disturbed images 1<br />

symbols 1<br />

mild/bizarre content 21<br />

energy/ global tension 2<br />

personal 2 /(expressive<br />

3 1<br />

originality 2<br />

use <strong>of</strong> inanimate objects 2 .<br />

subject broken, disruption 1<br />

Archetypal images 1<br />

less reality/logic 2<br />

no people 2<br />

(Alzheimers + cancer metaphors <strong>of</strong> loss 1 )<br />

(Alzheimer's: poor/lack <strong>of</strong> content ( 2 )<br />

bizarre content 2 )<br />

(autism mouth 1 )<br />

'plus phenomena' 1<br />

rapid expansive euphoric 1<br />

'minus phenomena' 1<br />

self disclosure personal information 1<br />

expressive 3<br />

sombre gloomy themes 12<br />

disinterest /less human energy self or family 2<br />

mutilated, twisted, distorted, or aggressive figures 13<br />

complete happy scenes with symbols, yellow sun, flowers 1<br />

(improvement: impressionistic, realistic with fantasy images 1 )<br />

less problem solving 2 less global energy 2<br />

twisted or distorted aggressive figures 3<br />

(anorexia good quality subject matter 1 )<br />

more people 1<br />

187<br />

fragmented 13 ,<br />

nonintegrated 3 ; ossified,<br />

static 1<br />

balance <strong>of</strong> pic 12<br />

displacement 2<br />

simplification<br />

/regression 2 ,<br />

simplistic imagery 2<br />

(aphasic 1 ; dementia 1 )<br />

poor quality 2<br />

(Alzheimer's 2 ; also poor<br />

organisation)<br />

fragmented 3 ,<br />

nonintegrated 3 low<br />

quality <strong>of</strong> execution 1<br />

disorganised 2<br />

wild random uncontrolled<br />

incoherent drawings,<br />

unconnected 1<br />

more logic than other<br />

patients 2<br />

less compositional 2 and<br />

general integration<br />

developmental<br />

indicators 1<br />

damage to trees,<br />

broken limbs 1<br />

lack <strong>of</strong> detail 1<br />

simple shapes/<br />

omissions 1<br />

proportion errors,<br />

disconnections 1<br />

lack <strong>of</strong> detail (left<br />

brain damage 2 )<br />

(Alzheimers: faulty<br />

recall 2 ;<br />

essentials omitted 2 )<br />

1, 2<br />

less detail<br />

smaller drawing, odd<br />

relationships between<br />

sexes, few sex diffs in<br />

figures 2 ;<br />

depression as<br />

secondary diagnosis is<br />

discriminable 1<br />

<strong>The</strong> DAPA Category <strong>of</strong> Emotional tone: <strong>The</strong> emotional tone <strong>of</strong> the section <strong>of</strong> picture<br />

under examination, as it is perceived by the rater, is recorded as positive, neutral or<br />

negative. Positivity is not a popular scale <strong>of</strong> content measurement and certainly does not<br />

cover the content spectrum mentioned above, This method has the advantage that


specific areas <strong>of</strong> positivity could be compared and that these impressions are distinct<br />

from the other variables.<br />

Measurements <strong>of</strong> Colour from the literature analysis<br />

<strong>The</strong> evidence suggests that normal controls generally used more colours than all patient<br />

groups. However, few studies examined the colours used, but employed some grouping<br />

system in the rating: darks and lights, warm and cool, tonal variations and monochromatic<br />

variations. <strong>The</strong>se divisions are all relative to the picture itself, since red and green can be<br />

seen as 'dark' together, whereas with other colours, such as brown or black, they can seem<br />

'lighter'. Dark can also be relative to the intensity <strong>of</strong> use and therefore these terms are<br />

ambiguous, may require special knowledge <strong>of</strong> the behaviour <strong>of</strong> tones, and contain layers<br />

<strong>of</strong> judgement decisions. <strong>The</strong> terms 'gloomy' and 'sombre' colours were <strong>of</strong>ten used as an<br />

indication <strong>of</strong> depression, but no evidence supports such associations. Previous research<br />

has recommended formal factors 246 , hue, value, chroma and tonality as likely to contain<br />

differentiating information. Whereas the basic hue <strong>of</strong> a colour is discernable, the other<br />

three qualities require a trained eye and may be too sensitive for an initial study. Most<br />

<strong>of</strong> what they convey can be summed up in the quality <strong>of</strong> intensity. P<strong>art</strong>icular associations<br />

between intensity and hue may be better dealt with in the analysis, as decisions such as<br />

dominant or prominent colour lend themselves to misinterpretations and to the subjective<br />

associations <strong>of</strong> judges.<br />

246<br />

W.L. Wadlington and H.J. McWhinnie (1973), <strong>The</strong> development <strong>of</strong> a rating scale for the study <strong>of</strong><br />

formal aesthetic qualities in the paintings <strong>of</strong> mental patients, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.1(3-4):201-20.<br />

188


<strong>The</strong> DAPA category <strong>of</strong> colour: <strong>The</strong> DAPA describes colour in the presence or absence<br />

<strong>of</strong> 9 basic hues within each section examined: Red, Yellow, Orange, Green, Blue, Brown,<br />

Purple, White and Black. Only the media is scored, not the paper.<br />

Intensity is coded seperately as high, neutral or low.<br />

Measurements <strong>of</strong> Line from the literature analysis<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were more different categories <strong>of</strong> line than any other, some were mixed up with<br />

shape, the definition <strong>of</strong> which was ambiguous. No differences were shown between<br />

normal controls and schizophrenia or Alzheimer's disease, but there were suggestions that<br />

physiognomic difficulties in control would probably affect results although some<br />

definitions were understood emotionally rather than visually and some were visual<br />

transformations <strong>of</strong> expected symptomatology. <strong>The</strong> studies which attempted to interpret<br />

the patients intentions through assessing the purpose <strong>of</strong> the line showed the most<br />

variable reliability and results. <strong>The</strong> definition <strong>of</strong> line quality, which typically rated<br />

internal relations within pictures; i.e. lines were rated relative to other lines within the<br />

picture, was probably easiest to compare objectively because people tend to judge the<br />

relative extremes. It seems reasonable to suppose that a simple note <strong>of</strong> internal variability<br />

<strong>of</strong> line <strong>of</strong> the picture would be comparable with that <strong>of</strong> another picture and would be<br />

difficult to invest with meaning.<br />

<strong>The</strong> DAPA Category <strong>of</strong> Line: <strong>The</strong> DAPA describes 'Line' using 3 levels <strong>of</strong> quality:<br />

Heavy, varied and thin. Development <strong>of</strong> the measure modified the coding method slightly<br />

from the first study and line is now coded separately as drawn or painted. <strong>The</strong> DAPA<br />

189


Rating Guide (Hacking & Foreman 1994) describes the division into painted line and<br />

drawn line when estimating variability <strong>of</strong> thickness. In the first study, line was a<br />

summary <strong>of</strong> painted and drawn lines.<br />

Measurement <strong>of</strong> Space in the literature<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were two elements to space; coverage <strong>of</strong> the paper with the media and pictorial<br />

space (the picture plane). Content based judgements seem to be sensitive to skill in that<br />

patients were distinguishable from controls on their lesser use <strong>of</strong> virtual picture planes<br />

(perspective) and it is hard to imagine how the associations and culture <strong>of</strong> the rater do<br />

not contribute to the judgement <strong>of</strong> spacial relations between drawn objects. Formal<br />

assessments <strong>of</strong> the amount <strong>of</strong> paper covered were affected by variability <strong>of</strong> definition <strong>of</strong><br />

cover <strong>of</strong> the paper (where the space was considered p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the composition in one case<br />

and not in another, according to the rater's assessment <strong>of</strong> the intention) which found no<br />

differences in patients and controls. Neglect measures did distinguish patients from<br />

controls and schizophrenia from mixed patients. <strong>The</strong>refore a measure which objectively<br />

defined media cover <strong>of</strong> the paper without reference to the image would break new ground.<br />

<strong>The</strong> DAPA category <strong>of</strong> Space: <strong>The</strong> DAPA area category records how much paper<br />

within each section examined remains uncovered by media by qu<strong>art</strong>ers: up to 10%; 10-<br />

25%; 25-55%; 55-80% and 80-100%.<br />

190


Measurements relating to Form from the literature<br />

This element covers a composite <strong>of</strong> variables which indicate mass, shape and focus.<br />

Many disorders indicate that fragmentation, disorganisation or degeneration <strong>of</strong> the image<br />

takes place in psychiatric illness. Lack <strong>of</strong> or dissolution <strong>of</strong> form indicating disassociation<br />

has been graphically portrayed by <strong>art</strong>ists as symptomatic <strong>of</strong> depression and other<br />

psychiatric illness, but not <strong>of</strong> stress disorder such as anorexia. Interpretation <strong>of</strong><br />

regression from childlike forms has been inconclusive. Repetition <strong>of</strong> forms is said to<br />

differentiate schizophrenia from other patients and normal controls, but repetitions are<br />

hard to identify without aesthetic training and probably vary considerably. <strong>The</strong> simple<br />

consideration <strong>of</strong> presence <strong>of</strong> form should differentiate at least patients from controls,<br />

with a possibility <strong>of</strong> exclusion <strong>of</strong> stress disorder. <strong>The</strong> other characteristics require<br />

judgements about skill or intention.<br />

<strong>The</strong> DAPA Category <strong>of</strong> Dominant Form: Global judgements commonly try to make<br />

an aesthetic assessment <strong>of</strong> the composition. Evidence also suggested that displacement<br />

<strong>of</strong> main images strongly indicated general abnormality. Wadlington and McWhinnie's 247<br />

concept <strong>of</strong> significant form seemed to encompass the main elements <strong>of</strong> this commonality.<br />

<strong>The</strong>ir attempt to define it was incoherent, p<strong>art</strong>ly because it depended on <strong>art</strong>istic<br />

terminology. It is adapted here, perhaps simplistically, as dominant form; the dominant<br />

shape and the simple assessment as to where it lies. Dominant form was a later addition<br />

to the measure and was used for the main study but was absent in the first study.<br />

247 Wadlington & McWhinnie (1973) op.cit.<br />

191


Table 2: Summary table <strong>of</strong> expected characteristics from the literature.<br />

Positivity <strong>of</strong><br />

Emotional<br />

Tone<br />

Colours by<br />

hue<br />

Intensity<br />

by strength<br />

high-low<br />

Drawn line by<br />

thickness<br />

PREDICTIONS<br />

1. All patients v Controls<br />

2. Difference to other patients<br />

3. Prediction <strong>of</strong> order<br />

1. More negative<br />

2. Schizophrenics and depressives negative> all<br />

patients<br />

3. Negativity: Schizophrenics>brain<br />

damage/disorder>other disorders><br />

personality disorder>control.<br />

Depressives mixed (most -ve) (some +ve)<br />

1. Less and darker colours; more red and black<br />

p<strong>art</strong>icularly, possibly brown, green, blue.<br />

2. Schizophrenics and depressives: red, black><br />

other patients; Schizophrenics and<br />

depressives 1 : No. colours< other patients.<br />

3. Black/brown: Depression>schizophrenia><br />

other groups> stress disorder>control;<br />

Yellow control>patients<br />

1 Depressives may be variable<br />

1. Darker colours, lower intensity<br />

2. Schizophrenics and depressives brain<br />

injury<br />

Empty Space 1. More<br />

2. Brain damage/disorder > schizophrenics<br />

3. Brain damage/disorder> depressed/<br />

schizophrenia >other patients> controls<br />

Largest most<br />

dominant<br />

Form<br />

1. Less form in total<br />

2. Depression


Objectives <strong>of</strong> the test<br />

<strong>The</strong> main objective <strong>of</strong> the Descriptive Assessment for Psychiatric Artwork<br />

(DAPA) was to describe an object produced directly by the patient (the painting)<br />

so that relations with another such product, by a different patient were<br />

comparable along the same indices.<br />

Subsidiary objectives were:<br />

to find suitable dimensions <strong>of</strong> a picture so as to include as much information as<br />

possible without making the scale unwieldy.<br />

that the scales should be valid; they would measure the attributes for which they<br />

were designed.<br />

that the scales be reliable; several raters would not disagree significantly on<br />

scoring.<br />

Hypothesis<br />

that the test would be useful, understandable and applicable outside the <strong>art</strong><br />

therapy pr<strong>of</strong>ession.<br />

1 Formal variables will distinguish paintings by different diagnostic groups <strong>of</strong><br />

psychiatric patients.<br />

2 If people with similar diagnoses use a similar characteristic style in their paintings,<br />

then the majority <strong>of</strong> people in a given group should make pictures which are more<br />

similar to those who suffer the same diagnostic psychiatric disorder.<br />

193


Previous methods <strong>of</strong> assessing atomistic elements <strong>of</strong> pictures have mostly dealt with the<br />

whole picture at once which can be complex because quite <strong>of</strong>ten not all <strong>of</strong> the picture is<br />

the same and therefore focus is directed to p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the picture. <strong>The</strong> grid overlay helps to<br />

deal with the totality <strong>of</strong> the picture elements whilst avoiding the domination <strong>of</strong> the<br />

assessment by the content.<br />

<strong>The</strong> DAPA Rating Guide (figure 3), gives a detailed explanation <strong>of</strong> the procedure<br />

and method for rating (helpsheets and rating sheet with a copy <strong>of</strong> the DAPA guide can<br />

be found in Appendix 4). <strong>The</strong> illustration below gives an example <strong>of</strong> the use <strong>of</strong> the grid<br />

system. An element <strong>of</strong> the picture, on the left hand side, is difficult to interpret in the<br />

conventional sense, but no aspect <strong>of</strong> the DAPA procedure requires us to either translate<br />

it or ignore it. Red scores for each square are given below.<br />

Red Scores from top left, reading across to bottom right. Row 1: 0, 0, 1, 1, 1. Row 2:<br />

1, 1, 1, 1, 1. Row 3: 1, 1, 0, 0, 0. Row 4: 0, 0, 0, 0, 0.<br />

194


Dapa Rating Guide, Version 3. Method for rating according to the Descriptive<br />

Assessment for psychiatric <strong>art</strong>work.<br />

Methods Section<br />

Organisation <strong>of</strong> methods section: <strong>The</strong>re are 3 different research tests to be covered:<br />

1. Main <strong>Study</strong>, which describes a study <strong>of</strong> spontaneous paintings with no<br />

prescribed subject in non-test conditions by 109 subjects, 83 <strong>of</strong> whom were<br />

psychiatric patients; instruments and procedures are described in detail.<br />

2. Interrater study (experimenter and 6 additional raters).<br />

3. First <strong>Study</strong>, which describes a study <strong>of</strong> self-portraits in standardised conditions<br />

by 50 subjects, 39 <strong>of</strong> whom were psychiatric patients.<br />

This chapter is organised into 3 sections. <strong>The</strong> Main study is presented first because<br />

extended explanations are more appropriately discussed within the greater rigour <strong>of</strong> the<br />

main study rather than in the initial more experimental study. <strong>The</strong> complete interrater<br />

and initial studies are presented next, thus references and comparisons are more<br />

comprehensible.<br />

Section 1. Main <strong>Study</strong><br />

Permission and ethics: This project was submitted and passed by the North<br />

Staffordshire Medical Ethics Committee 1995. Permission to access patients notes and<br />

to use paintings for research was sought from consultant psychiatrists in charge <strong>of</strong> the<br />

cases because most patients were not volunteers and there were ethical issues in some<br />

195


cases as to whether informed consent was appropriate. <strong>The</strong>re was no involvement by<br />

S.H. with the patient, for the purposes <strong>of</strong> this research retrospective paintings were<br />

viewed as information kept as notes on the patient, therefore they were owned by the<br />

hospital and formed p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the records. Consent forms were used for access to casenotes<br />

where patients were already in a painting group and staff agreed to save their paintings<br />

for the project. <strong>The</strong>se were accompanied by written explanations <strong>of</strong> the project for staff<br />

and patients, and instructions on how to treat the paintings after collection, but there<br />

were no refusals in these groups (samples <strong>of</strong> consent form with explanatory sheets are<br />

attached, Appendix 5). <strong>The</strong>re were no changes to the usual ward routine for the patient.<br />

Demographic information<br />

Patient Selection: P<strong>art</strong>icipants were drawn from mixed sex wards <strong>of</strong> psychiatric<br />

hospitals serving an industrial community in North Staffordshire, England. <strong>The</strong> hospitals<br />

accept the full range <strong>of</strong> adult psychiatric disorder requiring admission. 130 subjects for<br />

the study came from nine wards treating in-patient substance abusers, psychotic<br />

disorders, depressive disorders and personality disorders. Seven wards were sited in a<br />

large country mental hospital, which included a rehabilitation unit housed separately<br />

within the grounds <strong>of</strong> the main hospital building. <strong>The</strong> study encompassed the whole<br />

population <strong>of</strong> the hospital's recreational <strong>art</strong> programme. <strong>The</strong> substance abuse unit was<br />

sited in the City General hospital and the other ward was a community based mental<br />

health centre, which dealt with adjustment, anxiety and affective disorders.<br />

196


Controls: Staff members and nursing students from all p<strong>art</strong>icipating wards agreed to act<br />

as controls and completed their paintings together with the patient groups, so that control<br />

conditions were identical to those <strong>of</strong> the patients.<br />

Table 1: Demographics for subjects from all experimental groups<br />

TOTAL Sex M Sex F Age Mean Age S.D.<br />

Entire Population 109 49 60 46.17 16.50<br />

Affective Disorder 18 3 15 58.33 13.02<br />

Schizophrenia 35 22 13 57.8 13.69<br />

Personality<br />

Disorder<br />

9 3 6 32.78 7.51<br />

Drug Abuse 24 15 9 35.96 11.62<br />

Controls (staff) 23 6 17 34.83 9.81<br />

Sample: Table 1 shows demographic statistics: 86 psychiatric patients (43 males, 43<br />

females) formed the patient group. 23 staff members (6 males, 17 females) formed the<br />

control group and painted alongside the patients (49 males, 60 females). <strong>The</strong> mean age<br />

<strong>of</strong> the controls compared well with the other groups although there were more females.<br />

Choice <strong>of</strong> patients<br />

It was not possible, due to the numbers <strong>of</strong> patients available, to randomise groups and<br />

all suitable available patients 248 within the main county hospital at the time were involved<br />

in the study, most patients were in chronic stages <strong>of</strong> illness. <strong>The</strong> patient group available<br />

was quite large and permitted the comparison <strong>of</strong> general effects against specific effects.<br />

248 Those who were able and willing to paint, for whom there were facilities, and were not on a locked ward<br />

(for security reasons). Paintings were done on the ward and in the occupational therapy dept. 10 wards<br />

from the main hospital and 2 from the city general hospital were involved in the study.<br />

197


It was decided to use a large mixed group <strong>of</strong> general psychiatric patients who were split<br />

into several diagnostic groups rather than one smaller group <strong>of</strong> schizophrenics as previous<br />

research has done.<br />

Exclusions: Two wards which were asked to produce pictures for the study did not:<br />

only 4 patients from the traumatic brain injury unit produced the required number <strong>of</strong><br />

paintings as <strong>art</strong> groups were irregular with frequent changes <strong>of</strong> staff and patients, so they<br />

were excluded on numbers. Wardstaff withdrew a chronic schizophrenia group from the<br />

study due to misgivings about confidentiality because their <strong>art</strong>groups were dynamically<br />

rather than recreationally based. <strong>The</strong>re were 2 patients on whom information was not<br />

available (short stay patients, untraceable); and one patient who withdrew from the<br />

study. <strong>The</strong> other exclusions were on diagnostic criteria which are shown in Table 5 under<br />

procedures below.<br />

Unequal group sizes<br />

Patients: because diagnosis was only taken after the <strong>art</strong>work had been collected,<br />

numbers fluctuated and there were also overlaps between wards, especially with<br />

schizophrenics.<br />

Non-patients: All the non-patients were staff members or student nurses who worked<br />

with the group in the same conditions and whose paintings had been saved along with the<br />

patients. All staff members involved agreed to contribute their paintings. Most <strong>of</strong> the<br />

control paintings came from the groups which agreed to save their paintings weekly<br />

because many staff paintings, especially those <strong>of</strong> transient student nurses, were excluded<br />

198


ecause they were difficult to identify and single, and even those <strong>of</strong> regular staff had not<br />

been saved for long.<br />

Measures<br />

Age and sex were recorded and their means are shown in Table 1.<br />

Four additional demographic variables were taken from the files 249 , but on examination,<br />

discarded:<br />

1. Marital status had changed <strong>of</strong>ten for some patients and there were signs to<br />

suggest that files were <strong>of</strong>ten inaccurate and insensitive to patient's outside<br />

relations, especially when the patient had been hospitalised for a long time. This<br />

variable was discarded.<br />

2. IQ was not recorded because it was not routinely tested, the researcher was not<br />

clinically involved with patients and had no training for such measurement. IQ<br />

measures are known to be unreliable with psychiatric populations, because the<br />

illness itself interferes with the verbal component. Some studies use<br />

developmental indicators from the pictures themselves, but there is strong<br />

evidence to suggest that the scoring procedure is inconsistent in psychiatric<br />

illness 250 .<br />

3. Drugs was discarded: Only a handful <strong>of</strong> patients were not taking serious<br />

medication and some patients' medication fluctuated <strong>of</strong>ten and the date <strong>of</strong><br />

249<br />

Forms were developed so that information taken would be standardised and comparable. A sample form<br />

is included in Appendix 5.<br />

250 see introduction: projective tests.<br />

199


paintings was not reliable enough to relate to it; all were inpatients, but some<br />

were in hospital for one or two days only and there may have been other<br />

influences, especially in the substance abuse group.<br />

4. Chronicity <strong>of</strong> patients: <strong>The</strong>re were three variables to determine chronicity: new<br />

admission, previous admissions, and whether the patient had ever been sectioned.<br />

It was initially decided to examine and compare work in both acute and chronic<br />

stages <strong>of</strong> illness. <strong>The</strong> advantage <strong>of</strong> examining work by patients in acute stages<br />

<strong>of</strong> illness, as well as the traditional chronic groups, is the comparison <strong>of</strong> indicators<br />

which have been identified as distinguishing psychiatric illness that may be due<br />

to the institutional environment. However, patients were difficult to classify,<br />

even as inpatients, as their status fluctuated. Most <strong>of</strong> the wards which<br />

p<strong>art</strong>icipated actually contained patients with chronic illnesses, especially<br />

schizophrenia. However, in three out <strong>of</strong> four wards which were identified<br />

initially as dealing with acute illnesses (drug unit, rehabilitation unit, and<br />

community mental health unit) casenotes revealed that the majority <strong>of</strong> frequent<br />

attenders (3 paintings or more) had previous admissions and few were new to the<br />

hospital. Another acute unit, for traumatic brain injury was excluded. This<br />

variable was discarded.<br />

200


Instruments<br />

Diagnostic Tests: What is the ICD10(DCR)<br />

<strong>The</strong> International criteria for Diagnoses developed by the <strong>World</strong> Health Organisation<br />

(WHO) is the most widely used classification system in the world. Its development is<br />

detailed elsewhere 251 . It is recognised as a standardised, easy to apply system with<br />

excellent reliability between raters. <strong>The</strong> DSM 252 system, which is more popular in <strong>art</strong>-<br />

therapy literature represents North American modifications <strong>of</strong> the ICD sections; DSMs<br />

must be compatible with ICD. <strong>The</strong> main differences are in the higher rate <strong>of</strong> multiple<br />

coding <strong>of</strong> selected disorders encouraged by the use <strong>of</strong> DSM-III. Collaborative<br />

development <strong>of</strong> ICD-10 with DSMIV has now aligned these even more closely 253 .<br />

Because <strong>of</strong> the diagnostic procedure, and the experience <strong>of</strong> the clinician, for this research,<br />

the ICD-10 classifications provided the simplest and most reliable procedures.<br />

Reliability: All ICD-10 clinical diagnoses are organised into 10 two-character groups<br />

(e.g. F2 for all Schizophrenic disorders). <strong>The</strong>se two-character groups are subdivided into<br />

3 character groups (e.g. F20 for schizophrenia), which in turn are subdivided into 4<br />

character codes (e.g. F20.0 for paranoid schizophrenia). Interrater agreement can be<br />

251<br />

J.E. Cooper (1988), <strong>The</strong> structure and presentation <strong>of</strong> contemporary psychiatric classifications with<br />

special reference to ICD9 and 10. British Journal Psychiatry , V.152 (suppl.1):21-28. N. S<strong>art</strong>orius (1991),<br />

<strong>The</strong> classification <strong>of</strong> mental disorders in the Tenth Revision <strong>of</strong> the International Classification <strong>of</strong> Diseases.<br />

European Psychiatry , V.6:315-322. Details <strong>of</strong> planning, organisation and conduct <strong>of</strong> international field<br />

trials are also provided elsewhere, WHO DMH Mental behavioral and developmental disorder ("clinical<br />

descriptions and diagnostic guidelines"), in International Classification <strong>of</strong> Diseases , Tenth Revision (June<br />

1987 Draft for Field Trials). Geneva, WHO, 1987. J. Burke (1988), Field trials <strong>of</strong> the 1987 draft <strong>of</strong><br />

chapter V (F) <strong>of</strong> ICD-10. British J. Psychiatry , V.152 (suppl.1):33-57.<br />

252<br />

American Psychiatric Association (1987), Diagnostic and statistical manual <strong>of</strong> mental disorders (3rd ed.<br />

rev.), Washington D.C.:Author.<br />

253<br />

D.A. Regier, C.T. Kaelber, M.T. Roper, D.S. Rae, N. S<strong>art</strong>orius (1994), <strong>The</strong> ICD-10 Clinical Field<br />

Trial for Mental and Behavioral Disorders: Results in Canada and the United States, Am. J. Psychiatry ,<br />

V.151(9):1340-1350.<br />

201


computed on any <strong>of</strong> these levels. Groups <strong>of</strong> 2 character and 3 character codes combine<br />

related categories, and kappa coefficients at these levels tend to be higher than for more<br />

specific 4 character categories, since they ignore some differences in diagnostic<br />

disagreement 254 . For the purpose <strong>of</strong> this research, subjects were classified by 3 character<br />

groups although diagnoses were actually made at the 4 character level, which is usual for<br />

clinicians, as shown in table 10.<br />

<strong>The</strong> ICD-10 DCR (Diagnostic Criteria for Research, 1993) version was used for this<br />

research 255 , which is consistent and compatible with the Clinical Descriptive version and<br />

other national classification systems (e.g. DSM-IV) 256 . <strong>The</strong> criteria are explicit, easy to<br />

apply and are specified in more detail than in the more narrative equivalent statements<br />

in the clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines from which they were derived. Very<br />

high interrater agreements have been reported 257 , which was important for this research<br />

as reliability <strong>of</strong> diagnoses was not statistically investigated. <strong>The</strong> criteria contain<br />

descriptions <strong>of</strong> symptoms and other attributes <strong>of</strong> the patients, together with statements<br />

about the frequency, relative importance and duration <strong>of</strong> symptoms. Exclusion and<br />

inclusion statements are also provided.<br />

254 Ibid.<br />

255<br />

Division <strong>of</strong> Mental Health <strong>of</strong> the <strong>World</strong> Health Organization (1993), International Criteria for Diagnoses<br />

in the Mental and Behavioral Disorders: Diagnostic Criteria for Research (ICD-10 DCR), Geneva: Author.<br />

256<br />

N. S<strong>art</strong>orius, T. Bedirhan Ustun, A. Korten, J.E. Cooper, J. van Drimmelen (1995), Progress toward<br />

achieving a common language in psychiatry, II: results from the international field trials <strong>of</strong> the ICD10<br />

diagnostic criteria for research for mental and behavioral disorders, Am. J. Psychiatry , V.152(10):1427-<br />

1437.<br />

257 Although in trials, numbers were low for some diagnostic groups. <strong>The</strong> population was not randomly<br />

selected and the range <strong>of</strong> possible diagnoses were more limited than the field trials for the ICD-10 clinical<br />

202


Accommodation <strong>of</strong> this project to identified weaknesses <strong>of</strong> the ICD-10 DCR in<br />

interrater agreement.<br />

For some categories, such as those dealing with certain polymorphic psychotic disorders<br />

or milder forms <strong>of</strong> affective disorders, the criteria have been rated difficult and reliability<br />

was low 258 . Difficulties usually came from the fact that two conditions were coded in<br />

combination, a situation which was avoided in this research, since subjects with<br />

secondary diagnoses were excluded. Difficulties in distinction between related states or<br />

in fine differentiation <strong>of</strong> levels were addressed by limiting the classification to 3 characters<br />

which allowed combination <strong>of</strong> related categories. Most subjects were in chronic state and<br />

hospitalised, so the somewhat low agreements on polymorphic syndromes (eg acute<br />

psychotic disorders) or milder categories, where distinction between normal and case was<br />

vague, were avoided. Categories showing lowest kappa values tended to present the<br />

greatest difficulty. <strong>The</strong>re were obvious weaknesses in diagnosing psychiatric illness<br />

from casenotes. <strong>The</strong> notes did not usually contain a formal ICD-10 designation for the<br />

patient, but they did usually present a detailed description <strong>of</strong> symptoms and<br />

observations on the patient by an experienced psychiatrist and other staff. Patients who<br />

p<strong>art</strong>icipated in this research had major psychiatric dysfunction, and patients with<br />

secondary diagnoses were dropped. For this research, where there were difficulties,<br />

ambiguities or different conclusions from that recorded in the notes, DF consulted with<br />

the psychiatrist in charge <strong>of</strong> the case. <strong>The</strong>se cases were rare.<br />

descriptions.<br />

258 S<strong>art</strong>orius et al (1995), common language in psychiatry, op.cit. Although these tests were between<br />

countries worldwide and low reliability mostly reflected North American familiarity with the DSM-111.<br />

203


<strong>The</strong> DAPA (Descriptive Assessment for Psychiatric Artwork)<br />

<strong>The</strong> DAPA was piloted at Post Graduate School <strong>of</strong> Medicine, Dept. <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry, Keele<br />

University. Details <strong>of</strong> the instrument and the initial study were published in 1996 259 and<br />

the first study is described in section 3 <strong>of</strong> this chapter; Section 2 details the interrater<br />

reliability <strong>of</strong> the instrument.<br />

<strong>The</strong> DAPA test procedure uses a rating sheet for standardisation 260 , and the rating guide<br />

describes categories as objectively as possible in observable and simple terms with<br />

examples for comparison to minimise imposed interpretation by the rater. A full list <strong>of</strong><br />

operational definitions appears in the DAPA Rating Guide (Hacking and Foreman 1994:<br />

which appears at the beginning <strong>of</strong> Chapter 4, also appendix 4)<br />

Application - Scoring procedure<br />

<strong>The</strong> six categories are measured by a grid drawn on a transparent overlay, <strong>of</strong> 20 squares,<br />

consisting <strong>of</strong> 5 columns and 4 rows. <strong>The</strong> grid fits over the whole painting, corner to<br />

corner. Its absolute size is therefore defined by the size <strong>of</strong> painting. For each square<br />

respectively, 9 colours are recorded as present/absent (1,0); brightness or density (colour<br />

intensity) <strong>of</strong> the media is recorded as high, medium, low (3,2,1); line is recorded as thick,<br />

varied, or thin (3,2,1); the percentage space covered by the media is scored in qu<strong>art</strong>ers:<br />

10, 25, 55, 100%; subjective emotional tone is scored as it appears to the rater as<br />

259<br />

S. Hacking, D. Foreman, J. Belcher (1996), <strong>The</strong> Descriptive Assessment for Psychiatric Artwork<br />

(DAPA): a new way <strong>of</strong> quantifying paintings by psychiatric patients, J. <strong>of</strong> Nervous and Mental Disease ,<br />

V.184(7):425-430.<br />

204


positive, neutral or negative for each square (3,2,1); and Dominant form is recorded for<br />

each square as present or absent. This produces 20 scores for each <strong>of</strong> 15 variables.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se twenty scores are averaged for each variable over one painting. To eliminate any<br />

further effect <strong>of</strong> content, all the paintings by one individual were again averaged providing<br />

a subject pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong> 15 scores.<br />

Development: Appendix 4 shows early versions, labelled 1 and 2, and the developed<br />

version <strong>of</strong> the DAPA (version 3) used in the main study and the reliability study. <strong>The</strong><br />

initial study used version 2.<br />

Subordinate Analyses<br />

Treatment <strong>of</strong> the Data: Procedures to approximate a normal distribution were taken on<br />

the raw data and to counteract any confounding variables.<br />

Not all the subjects completed all the paintings. <strong>The</strong> non-parametric equivalent<br />

<strong>of</strong> ANOVA, the Kruskal Wallis test was performed on the categorical variables <strong>of</strong><br />

diagnostic group by number <strong>of</strong> paintings to determine bias, as content in the early<br />

paintings would therefore be emphasised. Additionally correlations were calculated<br />

between all variables to show association with number <strong>of</strong> paintings done.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were more females in the control group, so the ANOVA procedure was<br />

used to determine interactions <strong>of</strong> sex with significant variables.<br />

260 Sample in Appendix 4, version 3 <strong>of</strong> DAPA.<br />

205


<strong>The</strong> extremes <strong>of</strong> the distribution for each variable were examined for consistent<br />

outliers which could be eliminated, to compact the data and decrease noise; tables can be<br />

found in appendix 2; from Chapter 5 (figs 3a-f and tables/figs 1a-l).<br />

Analysis <strong>of</strong> the paintings: Most <strong>of</strong> the raw data was rank-ordered or classificatory, but<br />

the final score for each subject was a ratio <strong>of</strong> the distribution <strong>of</strong> each variable over their<br />

output <strong>of</strong> paintings, fulfilling the conditions for parametric analysis. All calculations<br />

were done using the computerised Statistical Package for Social Sciences, version 4 for<br />

DOS.<br />

Analysis to differentiate groups and the importance <strong>of</strong> population distribution<br />

<strong>The</strong> Analysis <strong>of</strong> variance test procedure was the main differentiating analysis in this<br />

study. It allows cases to fall into different groups based on their values for one variable.<br />

<strong>The</strong> one-way Analysis <strong>of</strong> Variance has the advantage that it is a single analysis which<br />

compares all the data at once, rather than employing t-tests between different groups,<br />

which is problematic when there are a number <strong>of</strong> tests. But ANOVA gives a clumsy<br />

answer because it employs an omnibus approach which compares equality <strong>of</strong> population<br />

means for each test variable; it assumes to 'null hypothesis', that there is no difference<br />

between the 5 different diagnostic groups.<br />

<strong>The</strong> test compares the variation between the means <strong>of</strong> the groups with that<br />

expected from the variability between individuals in the groups. <strong>The</strong> comparison takes<br />

the general form <strong>of</strong> an F test (variance ratio test); if two normally distributed populations<br />

206


have equal variances, the ratio <strong>of</strong> the two sample variances has a sampling distribution<br />

called the 'F' distribution, that is, the ratio <strong>of</strong> sample variances or square <strong>of</strong> ratio <strong>of</strong> the<br />

sample standard deviations. <strong>The</strong>re are two values for degrees <strong>of</strong> freedom - for each<br />

variance, the first relates to the numerator and the second to the denominator. If the two<br />

standard deviations are markedly different, it is unlikely that the two samples come from<br />

populations with the same variance. ANOVA assumes two conditions and tests are<br />

discussed below:<br />

1. Each <strong>of</strong> the groups must be a sample from a normal population.<br />

2. <strong>The</strong> variances within groups must be equal.<br />

In practice the analysis <strong>of</strong> variance gives good results even if the normality assumption<br />

doesn't quite hold but because <strong>of</strong> the imbalance in the number <strong>of</strong> observations in each <strong>of</strong><br />

the groups, the equal variance assumption was important. Distributions <strong>of</strong> values for<br />

each variable were computed using the SPSS EXAMINE procedure.<br />

<strong>The</strong> analysis <strong>of</strong> variance test does not isolate p<strong>art</strong>icular differing groups: it can<br />

only tell us whether there is a difference and not where the difference lies. For this a<br />

multiple comparison test is used. Because there were no grounds for prior hypothesis,<br />

a planned comparison would be inappropriate with an omnibus test such as Anova.<br />

Having observed a statistically significant effect using the oneway procedure, the<br />

RANGE subcommand (Duncan procedure) was used to compare each group with every<br />

other group; significance levels are attached to those groups showing most difference.<br />

<strong>The</strong> comparisons use t-tests applied between independent samples for each variable,<br />

comparing each pair <strong>of</strong> means in turn.<br />

207


<strong>The</strong> difficulty with using t-tests is that multiple significance testing gives a high<br />

probability <strong>of</strong> a type 1 error (a false positive result) because the probability becomes<br />

much more than 5%. Duncan's multiple range test controls the overall type 1 error rate<br />

at no more than 5% using the Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. <strong>The</strong><br />

procedure is suitable for groups with uncorrected variances and can also be adjusted for<br />

unbalanced design 261 . <strong>The</strong> disadvantage <strong>of</strong> this and similar methods available on SPSS is<br />

that they are 'conservative' so that errors are on the side <strong>of</strong> safety (non-significance).<br />

<strong>The</strong>refore small numbers <strong>of</strong> group comparisons (up to 5) are recommended, with<br />

specified research objectives 262 . In addition, since it is likely that some <strong>of</strong> the measures<br />

for ANOVA are correlated: in real life we can assume some correlation between multiple<br />

tests, it is more likely that the Bonferroni estimate would be conservative, placing any<br />

suspicion on non-significant data.<br />

Discriminatory power between controls and patients<br />

This final analysis aims to give distinct answers to 2 direct questions;<br />

(i) Is the DAPA as effective as other <strong>art</strong> assessments; and<br />

(ii) can the DAPA practically discriminate between patients and controls.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se two questions need to be attacked differently because they are respectively<br />

conjectural and pragmatic; (i) is answered through the illustration <strong>of</strong> effect sizes from t-<br />

test results, using the methodology explained in Chapter 2, which derived effect sizes for<br />

261<br />

It is a popular misconception that groups must be orthogonal for comparison tests, R. West (1991),<br />

Computing for Psychol ogists (London: Harwood).<br />

262 D.G. Altman (1994),<br />

Practical Statistics for Medical Research , London, 3rd. reprint, original 1991:<br />

208


the most interpretable studies from 20 years review <strong>of</strong> the literature; and (ii) is answered<br />

through the interpretation <strong>of</strong> the discriminant analysis performed on the collected data<br />

from patients and controls.<br />

(i) Is the DAPA a better assessment than the other tests reviewed in Chapter 2?<br />

In order to contrast the effect from the DAPA with that <strong>of</strong> the general tenor <strong>of</strong> the<br />

literature, the basic differences in effect size between controls and patients on each<br />

variable from the DAPA were determined by another t-test. Each variable was treated<br />

as though it was independent, purely for the theoretical comparison. <strong>The</strong>se tests cannot<br />

be regarded in practice as independent, as there were obvious correlations in the data and<br />

so there was likely to be confounding errors, due to multicollinearity -one variable may<br />

be the main predictor, subsuming those correlated with it to insignificant contributions,<br />

thus true results for the DAPA should take account <strong>of</strong> direct relations between variables.<br />

(ii) Can the DAPA practically discriminate between patients and controls.<br />

Regression analysis was not applicable to this study because from the discussion <strong>of</strong><br />

results it was obvious that there could be interactions and correlations between one or<br />

more <strong>of</strong> the predictor variables. <strong>The</strong> more complicated regression techniques require more<br />

cases. <strong>The</strong> discriminant analysis is an older technique, but for 2 groups gives a similar<br />

result. Discriminant analysis avoids the problem <strong>of</strong> multicollinearity by setting a<br />

tolerance level which excludes variables that are highly correlated with each other. In<br />

Chapman and Hall, p.211.<br />

209


addition it leaves out other variables which are not necessarily non-discriminatory<br />

independently, but their ability to add to the discrimination having taken account <strong>of</strong> the<br />

other variables already used is judged low. It reduces the test to a subset <strong>of</strong> useful<br />

variables from a larger set <strong>of</strong> candidates and indicates to what extent a combination <strong>of</strong><br />

independent variables will allow discrimination between controls and patients.<br />

Discriminant analysis was used to find a combination <strong>of</strong> variables that<br />

classified a large proportion <strong>of</strong> subjects into the correct group as an instrument <strong>of</strong><br />

allocation or diagnosis for new subjects. <strong>The</strong> analysis used the same subject group<br />

detailed in table 1 (109 subjects: 23 controls and 86 patients). <strong>The</strong> method is robust and<br />

makes few assumptions, but two recommendations are suggested for the sake <strong>of</strong><br />

interpretation 263 :<br />

1. that most variables have a normal distribution with the same s.d. within each group<br />

(although some authorities have argued that discriminant analysis is robust for binary<br />

distributions); and<br />

2. five times as many subjects are recommended per group as variables.<br />

It was appropriate to consider this method, then, for 2 groups: patients and controls;<br />

subgroup analysis would require more cases. <strong>The</strong> Mahalanobis method, which is a<br />

variation on the stepwise method, was selected. Using SPSS, the steps <strong>of</strong> the calculation<br />

are clearly shown. <strong>The</strong> analysis works by finding a combination <strong>of</strong> variables that<br />

maximises the distance (D 2 ) between the groups. It may, perhaps, do slightly less well<br />

263 Altman (1994), op.cit. recommends that in order to correctly interpret the associated significance that<br />

variables should have a normal distribution, but generally authorities are mixed on whether this issue is<br />

critical.<br />

210


than the stepwise option at correctly classifying the subjects but there is a clearer<br />

indication <strong>of</strong> the extent to which p<strong>art</strong>icular variables contribute to the discrimination<br />

process.<br />

It was necessary to control for uneven numbers between groups as the patient<br />

group was nearly four times the size <strong>of</strong> the control group, otherwise there wouldn't be<br />

an even chance <strong>of</strong> allocation to either group. <strong>The</strong> PRIORS subcommand within the SPSS<br />

program adjusts the calculation for the sizes <strong>of</strong> the groups according to the prior<br />

probability <strong>of</strong> chance allocation to one group or another. Prior probability was thus set<br />

to 75/25%.<br />

Subordinate analyses<br />

Correlations between DAPA variables<br />

For exploratory analysis, the Pearson Product Moment Correlation (Pearson's r) is the<br />

most commonly used measure <strong>of</strong> linear association between continuous variables. Bi-<br />

variate scatter plots for the significant variables were generated to check the distribution<br />

<strong>of</strong> the data because there are restrictions on the validity <strong>of</strong> the associated hypotheses<br />

tests. <strong>The</strong> significance <strong>of</strong> the correlation coefficient is valid for random samples with at<br />

least one normally distributed variable. 2 tailed tests were used because, although<br />

hypotheses were made, there was little scientific reason to expect that correlations would<br />

be in any p<strong>art</strong>icular direction.<br />

Correlation is <strong>of</strong>ten used as exploratory analysis, but even with only a dozen<br />

variables here, 66 r values are produced. 5 populations are tested, thus 1 value in 20 will<br />

be significant at the 5% level purely by chance! This level <strong>of</strong> analysis was rather too<br />

211


nebulous for reasonable conclusions and two tailed tests <strong>of</strong> significance tightened up the<br />

data and allowed for type 1 errors. Much depends on the sample size, but significant<br />

values <strong>of</strong> r below .6 would not contribute much to a speculative general discussion 264 .<br />

Because there is little background knowledge, it would be imprudent to infer<br />

relationships from analysis <strong>of</strong> correlations alone. It was clear from previous studies 265<br />

that some exploratory analysis was necessary, as interrelationships between the variables<br />

could contribute to the explanation <strong>of</strong> difference in the results <strong>of</strong> this analysis from<br />

previous work or accepted knowledge. <strong>The</strong> secondary purpose, in further development<br />

<strong>of</strong> the test, was to identify any possible reduction <strong>of</strong> the scales to increase efficiency if<br />

some <strong>of</strong> the scales measured the same quantities.<br />

An analysis for the whole population as if they were the same diagnostic group<br />

may mislead because other differences in samples, or indeed a third unknown variable<br />

might influence ratings, bunching p<strong>art</strong>icular groups and inflating the correlation. In<br />

addition in large populations, very small correlations become significant. Correlations<br />

were calculated between reliable variables within each diagnostic group.<br />

Procedures<br />

264 Altman's (1994) op.cit. recommended method to modify over-enthusiasm may be adopted here; the<br />

calculation <strong>of</strong> 100r 2 on 0.7 gives 49%, the percentage <strong>of</strong> the variability <strong>of</strong> the data that is explained by the<br />

association <strong>of</strong> the two variables.<br />

265 For instance: the DDS (Cohen et al. 1988, op.cit) see my criticism <strong>of</strong> their results, Chapter 2;<br />

Wadlington and McWhinnie (1973), op.cit. found significant associations between scales similar to those<br />

used in the DAPA test.<br />

212


Procedure for diagnoses: ICD-10 diagnoses were made from case records by Dr. D.<br />

Foreman 266 , who is a Consultant psychiatrist with training in both general and child<br />

psychiatry. D.F. was blind to the originating ward and paintings while making the<br />

diagnoses. Disagreements were discussed with the consultant in charge <strong>of</strong> the case.<br />

Table 2 shows category definition <strong>of</strong> case assignment to 4 types; Affective Disorder<br />

(n=18); Schizophrenia (n=35); Personality Disorder (n=9); Drug Abuse (n=24).<br />

ICD-10 Diagnostic categories for research (1993) N ASSIGNED<br />

TO TYPE<br />

Moderate Depressive Episodes (Affective Disorder) (4 x F25.2; 3 x F25.1; 2 x<br />

F25.0, F33.3; 1 x F33.1, F32.8, F32.3, F32.1, F31.7, F31.6, F31.0). 18 3<br />

Schizophrenia (24 x F20.3; 4 x F20.0; 4 x F20.9; 2 x F20.5; 1 x F20.2). 35 1<br />

Personality Disorder: (3 x F60.3; 1 x F60.3 + F68.1; 3 x F10.2 + F60.3; 1 x<br />

F10.2 + F60.9; 1 x F45.0 + F61.1). 9 5<br />

Substance Abuse: (14 x F10.2; 3 x F19.2; 2 x F11.2, F18.2; 1 x F11.1, F13.2,<br />

F15.2) 24 2<br />

Table 3: 21 Patients who were excluded from the study<br />

Diagnostic type with ICD-10 category N<br />

Diagnosis <strong>of</strong> Affective Disorder with secondary diagnosis<br />

(1 x F33.4 + F00.1; 1 x F33.4 + F02.3; 1 x F31.3 + F70.1; 1 x F33.2 + F22.0)<br />

Diagnosis <strong>of</strong> Schizophrenia with secondary diagnosis: (1 x F20.0 + G20.0; 1 x F20.3 +<br />

F70.1; 1 x F20.3 + F07.8; 1 x F20.9 + F70.1) 4<br />

Diagnosis <strong>of</strong> Organic Mental Disorder: (1 x F00.1 + F31.7; 1 x 0.1 +F25.1; 1 x F01.2; 1<br />

x F07.0) 4<br />

Diagnosis <strong>of</strong> Anxiety Disorder, Eating Disorder: (1 x F41.2 + F10.2; 1 x F10.2 + F50.3;<br />

2 x F50) 4<br />

Diagnosis <strong>of</strong> Mental Retardation: (2 x F71.1) 2<br />

Procedure for Painting Generation: All patients and staff who had attended <strong>art</strong><br />

groups run by nurses, as p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the recreational program <strong>of</strong> the hospital in the wards<br />

266 Dr. D.M. Foreman, M.Sc., MRC Psych., Clinician and Senior Lecturer, Psychiatry Dept., Post<br />

Graduate School <strong>of</strong> Medicine, Keele University. Dr. Foreman supervised this Ph.D. research.<br />

213<br />

4


identified were included in the study. <strong>The</strong> <strong>art</strong> groups took place at the same place and<br />

time each week in daylight hours for one hour a week. <strong>The</strong>y contained up to 8<br />

p<strong>art</strong>icipants who were either patients or staff from the ward. In each case the most recent<br />

10 paintings produced by the subject were used for the study, these varied in date<br />

because although the study was initially retrospective, paintings by patients were not<br />

kept by all wards 267 . Where it was a practise to dispose <strong>of</strong> the work, <strong>art</strong> groups agreed<br />

to keep and label the work for a specified period. Paintings were identified with a number<br />

by the nurses at the time and a separate sheet was provided a key to names and staff<br />

identification, which was kept separately until after the scoring period. S.H. collected all<br />

the paintings from the nurses weekly, but was unaware <strong>of</strong> diagnosis, which paintings<br />

were by staff members and unacquainted with the group. No themes or subjects were<br />

set 268 and the group leaders followed their own agendas or used free painting. Art<br />

materials were those available to the group leaders, pencil and paint, but were not<br />

standardised, although labelling instructions did indicate that all the basic colours should<br />

be available to the patients if possible. Most <strong>of</strong> the paper provided was white 17" x 12",<br />

but some used white 8" x 12".<br />

267 This is surprising if <strong>art</strong> work is considered as information relating to the patient, but actually few<br />

patients are given true <strong>art</strong> therapy due to the shortage <strong>of</strong> therapists. Most '<strong>art</strong> therapy' is given by nurses<br />

who have taken short courses. Paintings were discarded because they contribute to fire hazard and also<br />

fat-file disease: the patient's file gets thicker the longer the hospital stay.<br />

268 But where suggestions were asked for, S.H. made the recommendation <strong>of</strong> a self portrait.<br />

214


Summary<br />

After the pictures were drawn/painted and collected and measured according to the<br />

DAPA specifications above, there were 6 stages to the analysis <strong>of</strong> the study data, their<br />

applications are divided between results and discussion.<br />

RESULTS:<br />

• <strong>The</strong> data was transformed, so that the distribution <strong>of</strong> the majority <strong>of</strong> variables<br />

approximated normal. Variables with low frequencies were excluded from the<br />

analysis.<br />

• A variance analysis was applied to 13 scores representing 6 measured dimensions <strong>of</strong><br />

paintings obtained from the DAPA procedure to determine whether the instrument<br />

differentiated between controls and 4 patient groups <strong>of</strong> different diagnostic types.<br />

• <strong>The</strong> Duncan procedure identified where differences lay.<br />

• <strong>The</strong> discriminant analysis determined the predictive value <strong>of</strong> differences between<br />

patients and controls.<br />

• Subordinate correlational analysis was used to determine the within cells correlations<br />

for the 13 variables from the DAPA procedure, to assess the validity <strong>of</strong> the<br />

categories.<br />

• T-tests for each variable were calculated using the same procedure to determine effect<br />

sizes for comparison with the best studies from the literature.<br />

215


DISCUSSION<br />

• <strong>The</strong> validity <strong>of</strong> the DAPA categories was examined, strengths and limitations <strong>of</strong> the<br />

procedure.<br />

• <strong>The</strong> results were explained and compared and contrasted with those detailed in the<br />

literature review and the results <strong>of</strong> the pilot study, in which the content was<br />

controlled.<br />

• <strong>The</strong> interactions and correlations <strong>of</strong> the 13 variables from the DAPA procedure were<br />

discussed and compared with impressions from the literature taking account <strong>of</strong> the<br />

limitations <strong>of</strong> the procedure for the effect sizes produced by the t-test.<br />

• <strong>The</strong> discriminant procedure was discussed and compare the final outcome with<br />

previous investigations and predictions.<br />

Section 2: Reliability <strong>Study</strong><br />

An abbreviated version <strong>of</strong> this study, together with the first study (section 3) appeared<br />

in an <strong>art</strong>icle by S. Hacking, D. Foreman and J. Belcher (1996) 269 .<br />

Purpose <strong>of</strong> the <strong>Study</strong><br />

<strong>The</strong> amount <strong>of</strong> rating in this set <strong>of</strong> studies was considerable. A little over 1000 paintings<br />

were rated for the main study and the first study by the author for this research. It was<br />

necessary to find out if the rater's judgement <strong>of</strong> pictures was a contributing factor to<br />

significant differences in discriminating variables. <strong>The</strong>re are various ways <strong>of</strong> conducting<br />

inter-rater reliability tests on human subjective judgements. <strong>The</strong>re is nothing unscientific<br />

269 S. Hacking, D. Foreman and J. Belcher (1996), <strong>The</strong> Descriptive Assessment for Psychiatric Art: a new<br />

216


about subjective judgments but care is needed in making them credible. It is essential to<br />

be able to demonstrate that at least two people can independently come to fairly similar<br />

judgments. Multiple rating <strong>of</strong> the whole test would be time consuming, costly and<br />

impractical since it is better to employ more than two people; two people closely<br />

involved can reach an understanding which is not apparent in the written material and<br />

over hundreds <strong>of</strong> ratings, the significance <strong>of</strong> small deviations decreases. <strong>The</strong> more people<br />

employed, the greater the risk <strong>of</strong> individual variation on rating areas which are not clearly<br />

defined.<br />

Inter-rater reliability statistics judge the extent to which agreement is reached,<br />

usually by reporting the correlation between raters, together with a summary <strong>of</strong> the<br />

agreements and disagreements on level <strong>of</strong> rating. This study took a small sample <strong>of</strong><br />

pictures (1%) from the total amount <strong>of</strong> pictures previously rated by the author, and used<br />

a relatively large number <strong>of</strong> judges. <strong>The</strong> object was to find out if several people<br />

independently could give the same or similar scorings using the DAPA scale to the<br />

scoring given by the author; if so, it can be reasonably deduced that the author's rating<br />

was consistent within the test categories.<br />

Choice <strong>of</strong> Subjects<br />

Raters: 7 first year <strong>art</strong> students (6 females 1 male 19-24 yrs.) from a local University<br />

were recruited as raters. <strong>The</strong>y had no previous knowledge <strong>of</strong> the DAPA or <strong>of</strong> other <strong>art</strong><br />

tests. Art students were chosen because the training would take some time, concentration<br />

way <strong>of</strong> quantifying paintings by psychiatric patients, J. Nervous and Mental Disease , V.184(7):425-9.<br />

217


and the test used terminology which would be familiar to them. 1 rater was dropped<br />

during training because his responses were inappropriate to the task. Raters received 2<br />

hours training with the rating guide used in the main study (Version 3, shown in Chapter<br />

3). This was in the form <strong>of</strong> a workshop, which took them through the guide, included<br />

explanations, examples <strong>of</strong> terms and the rating <strong>of</strong> one training painting before using the<br />

scale.<br />

Picture Selection: 25 paintings were selected by the author from paintings rated some<br />

months previously as p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> a study with large numbers <strong>of</strong> other such work. <strong>The</strong><br />

paintings showed a range <strong>of</strong> different subjects and treatment <strong>of</strong> media. <strong>The</strong>y were each<br />

by different long term psychiatric inpatients from a local mental hospital, or by staff<br />

from the same facility. Each rater on arrival selected one painting from the study pool<br />

and these paintings were used for the study (ratings made by the author for the selected<br />

paintings were retrieved at the analysis stage for the comparison and were not available<br />

at the time).<br />

Method<br />

Raters were given the opportunity to discuss the categories with the author in training<br />

as indicated. <strong>The</strong>y all rated the same training picture at the end <strong>of</strong> the training session to<br />

iron out any inconsistencies and misunderstandings, and then individually rated seven<br />

pictures, in series, in different orders, referring to the rating guide and helpsheet provided<br />

by the author. <strong>The</strong>y worked by themselves at separate desks so that they could not<br />

discuss their ratings, they changed over pictures with the next person until they had<br />

218


finished the set. <strong>The</strong>y were asked for their comments and suggestions. Most<br />

p<strong>art</strong>icipants took about 10 minutes for the whole process as described in the DAPA<br />

rating guide (Chapter 3) per painting by the end.<br />

Analysis: treatment <strong>of</strong> the data<br />

A point by point comparison <strong>of</strong> every category per gridsquare would have been<br />

ridiculously long and complicated. <strong>The</strong> propriety <strong>of</strong> different methods <strong>of</strong> agreement are<br />

discussed at the end <strong>of</strong> Chapter 2 (reliability). Altman 270 recommends Kappa for<br />

categorical variables, and warns against the misapplication <strong>of</strong> the correlation coefficient<br />

to measure agreement for categorical scales. However, a disadvantage <strong>of</strong> kappa is that it<br />

takes no account <strong>of</strong> disagreements and is not useful for numbers <strong>of</strong> raters, numbers <strong>of</strong><br />

categories, non-ordinal scales and large tables, <strong>of</strong>ten requiring considerable collapsing <strong>of</strong><br />

the data and thus not for this study. A categorical comparison was therefore both<br />

unwieldy and unnecessary, since it is the final mean score, used as continuous data,<br />

which is the hub <strong>of</strong> the DAPA process. <strong>The</strong> questionable propriety <strong>of</strong> the correlation co-<br />

efficient for analysis <strong>of</strong> agreement, as expounded by Altman, is that although it indicates<br />

linear association, how nearly the scores are ordered in the same way, it does not indicate<br />

whether the level <strong>of</strong> the scores have changed. This is important because ordinal<br />

positioning is not relational and therefore p<strong>art</strong>icularly makes nonsense <strong>of</strong> categorical<br />

correlations.<br />

270<br />

D.G. Altman (1994), Practical Statistics for Medical Research , London, 7th ed. originally 1991:<br />

Chapman Hall, p.284, and 409.<br />

219


How the data were structured<br />

Although ordinal scales are <strong>of</strong>ten treated as though they were interval scales, only very<br />

limited mathematical and statistical operations make sense using ordinal data. However,<br />

a common quasi-legitimate procedure 271 was applied to scores from the DAPA, both for<br />

the main tests and for the reliability test. Normality was assumed in the underlying<br />

distribution and the interval units <strong>of</strong> the scale were adjusted to match the average<br />

observation for the whole picture (over 20 squares), which makes sense, because there<br />

is a maximum limit for each variable over the painting (e.g. Range <strong>of</strong> Red = 0 - 1). Even<br />

though, pragmatically, the limitations <strong>of</strong> the measurement method must be taken into<br />

consideration in the final analysis, statistical advantages <strong>of</strong> parametric analysis allow<br />

more powerful techniques <strong>of</strong> comparison.<br />

<strong>The</strong> raw scores for colour and form for each picture (count <strong>of</strong> up to 20<br />

gridsquares) were scaled 0-20 and had two possible scores for each square, but the scores<br />

for Intensity, Painted Line, Drawn Line, and Emotional Tone were scaled 0-60 with 4<br />

choices and those for Space, 1-100 with 5 choices 272 . It was therefore necessary to align<br />

the scores on the same scaling points for comparability <strong>of</strong> repeated measurement. <strong>The</strong><br />

usual testing procedure reduces the scores to an average gridsquare rating over the whole<br />

set <strong>of</strong> paintings, but this was inappropriate to test the scoring procedure 273 . To keep the<br />

data as simple as possible, a count per picture for colours was used; scores from the 0-60<br />

scales, were divided by a factor <strong>of</strong> 3, and the 1-100, by a factor <strong>of</strong> 5.<br />

271 <strong>The</strong> same as that done with IQ data in order to convert it to an interval scale.<br />

272 <strong>The</strong>re weren't any zero scores (no marks at all).<br />

220


Measuring accuracy<br />

Altman's recommendation to plot the differences against the average score, for continuous<br />

measures, seemed clear and appropriate. Tables 1-15a (appendix 2) show rater's scores,<br />

discrepancy levels, the mean score for each subject and differences to that <strong>of</strong> the author.<br />

Plots <strong>of</strong> the distribution <strong>of</strong> the raters' scores (Figs 1-15 appendix 2) show concordance<br />

with the mean rating; for clarity the same minimum and maximum limits are used<br />

throughout. It must be remembered that the principal rating (R7 by the author) is not the<br />

true value, which is unknown, and for this purpose, the average <strong>of</strong> the mean trainee rating<br />

and that <strong>of</strong> the author 274 , acts as the best estimate for the unknown true value. A<br />

summary <strong>of</strong> these scores, the standard differences are tabulated below (table 16).<br />

Table 16: Standard deviation <strong>of</strong> the differences between the mean <strong>of</strong> the 6 trainee raters<br />

and the author ratings over 7 paintings<br />

S.Diff<br />

Variable Mean Std Dev Min Max<br />

RED -.02 1.35 -1.50 2.67<br />

YELLOW 1.12 1.21 .00 3.00<br />

ORANGE -.24 .42 -1.00 .00<br />

PURPLE -1.26 2.09 -5.33 1.00<br />

GREEN -.86 1.20 -3.00 .17<br />

BLUE -1.36 1.23 -3.00 .00<br />

BROWN -1.07 2.51 -5.83 2.17<br />

WHITE -.24 .63 -1.67 .00<br />

BLACK -.07 .69 -1.17 1.00<br />

INTENSITY 1.02 1.54 -.89 3.83<br />

PAINT LINE 1.21 1.26 -.33 2.72<br />

DRAW LINE -.06 .11 -.28 .00<br />

SPACE -.65 .68 -1.77 .00<br />

EM-TONE .17 2.01 -2.17 3.11<br />

FORM -.31 .68 -1.33 .67<br />

221<br />

95% limit <strong>of</strong> agreement;<br />

(expected differences by scale points);<br />

and likely direction <strong>of</strong> discrepancy<br />

-2.68 to 2.70 (


How well does the method agree?<br />

Table 16 shows a summary <strong>of</strong> the standard differences (S.diff) between the 6 trainee<br />

raters and the author's score (column 1), plotted in Figs 1-15 275 (Appendix 2). <strong>The</strong><br />

scatter <strong>of</strong> differences remains constant and does not increase with the mean, so the<br />

differences can be further investigated in the raw state 276 . A test <strong>of</strong> association would<br />

be inappropriate at this stage as we cannot deduce that methods agree well if they are not<br />

statistically different. <strong>The</strong>re were large variations in the sample which may have<br />

regressed the mean and may well lead to important differences seeming non-significant<br />

(see tables 1-15 accompanying figs. 1-15, appendix 2).<br />

<strong>The</strong> S.diff. can be used as a measure <strong>of</strong> agreement by itself, but it is more useful<br />

to construct a range <strong>of</strong> values from it, which covers the agreement between the raters for<br />

most cases.<br />

For reasonably symmetric distributions we can expect the range mean +-2 S.D.<br />

to include 95% <strong>of</strong> the observations. We can therefore take mean +-2S.Diff. as a 95%<br />

range <strong>of</strong> agreement for individual paintings. This range defines the 95% limits <strong>of</strong><br />

agreement (shown in Table 16, column 2). For the present data, from the 95% ranges,<br />

the author is expected to score within 1 point <strong>of</strong> scores from independent raters on drawn<br />

line, orange and white; within 2 on yellow, black, space, and form, with a slight bias<br />

towards higher scores; and within 3 points on a further 5 variables: red, green, blue,<br />

intensity, and painted line, with discrepancies likely in either direction. 3 variables have<br />

275 against Author+RATS/2.<br />

276 Rather than translation to logarithmic scores.<br />

222


wider ranges: purple and brown, which had 4 or 5 point limits, are mixed colours which<br />

are harder to define and therefore more variable to rate; raters tended to score lower. And<br />

emotional tone, which had 4 points limit, depended on the associations <strong>of</strong> the rater<br />

(discrepancies equal).<br />

So, how can we define good agreement?<br />

For this research, the standard deviation <strong>of</strong> the differences as a measure <strong>of</strong> agreement is<br />

appropriate because the subjects themselves define their own limitations. However, this<br />

does not facilitate comparison <strong>of</strong> the measure. Interpretation <strong>of</strong> agreement depends upon<br />

the circumstances, it is not possible to use statistics to define acceptable agreement; it<br />

is more important to quantify the variability <strong>of</strong> the individual data points. But it is still<br />

necessary to define some answer to the question: how well do the raters agree? within<br />

the larger context which gives it meaning <strong>of</strong>, how do we compare this agreement with<br />

other measures? It is a complicated question, and there are two components to the<br />

answer.<br />

(1) <strong>The</strong> mean difference is an estimate <strong>of</strong> the average bias <strong>of</strong> the raters relative to the<br />

principal rater. Here the means are mostly negligible and we can say that agreement is<br />

excellent on average.<br />

(2) It is essential to consider the agreement for an individual painting for which purpose<br />

the standard deviation <strong>of</strong> the differences (S.diff; Table 16) can be used.<br />

A rough comprehensive overall statistic can be deduced from examination <strong>of</strong> the<br />

raw data (middle columns <strong>of</strong> tables 1-15, appendix 2). A total 480 out <strong>of</strong> 630 decisions<br />

223


- 76% - <strong>of</strong> the scales were rated within one scale point <strong>of</strong> the author's score; the number<br />

<strong>of</strong> exact agreements was 58% (365); and a further 10% within 2. <strong>The</strong>se figures are well<br />

within the expected limits <strong>of</strong> agreement shown in Table 16 (column 5) for 86% <strong>of</strong> the<br />

data. Ordering <strong>of</strong> the most reliable to least reliable DAPA categories (table 18 below) was<br />

based on the above considerations.<br />

Table 18. Ordering <strong>of</strong> DAPA variables for Reliability<br />

Most reliable categories<br />

Variable / agreement within 1<br />

point <strong>of</strong> R7score<br />

Best Agreements limits<br />

Draw-Line 88% .2<br />

Form 91% 1.5<br />

White 95% 1<br />

Orange 88% 1<br />

Black 86% 1.5<br />

Green 86% 2.5<br />

Moderate Agreement<br />

Purple* 81% 4<br />

Yellow 79% 2<br />

Red 78% 3<br />

Space 73% 1.5<br />

Lower Agreement<br />

Brown* 67% 5<br />

Blue 60% 2.5<br />

Paint-Line 55% 2.5<br />

Intensity 50% 3<br />

Em-Tone+ 40% 4<br />

over 2<br />

points<br />

diff's.<br />

2%<br />

2%<br />

5%<br />

12%<br />

4%<br />

7%<br />

14%<br />

14%<br />

17%<br />

8%<br />

21%<br />

23%<br />

26%<br />

31%<br />

39%<br />

*the actual figures within 1 point for brown and purple are acceptable because there were some wide fluctuations<br />

in the estimate which brings the mean figure up.<br />

+Emtone was a more subjective variable, but 61% <strong>of</strong> the data is consistent within 2 points.<br />

But there still remains the question <strong>of</strong> comparability with other measures. <strong>The</strong>re is error<br />

in every measurement. <strong>The</strong> amount <strong>of</strong> error in a measure is indicated by various<br />

estimates <strong>of</strong> the reliability <strong>of</strong> the measure. A test with a low reliability <strong>of</strong>, for example,<br />

60, has more error than a test with a high reliability <strong>of</strong>, say, .80.<br />

224


Variable<br />

name<br />

Red<br />

Yellow<br />

Orange<br />

Purple<br />

Green<br />

Blue<br />

Brown<br />

White<br />

Black<br />

Intensity<br />

Paint-Line<br />

Draw-Line<br />

Space<br />

Em-Tone<br />

Form<br />

Authorities differ on recommended methods <strong>of</strong> inter-rater reliability statistics,<br />

and there are few previous good examples in this field 277 . A supporting equivalence test,<br />

such as Cronbach's alpha, used to demonstrate internal consistency between items<br />

measuring the same attribute is recommended 278 .<br />

Table 17. to show means and standard deviations* <strong>of</strong> rater scores for each variable over<br />

the sample <strong>of</strong> 7 pictures. <strong>The</strong>re is a maximum <strong>of</strong> 20 point scores for each variable.<br />

Simple counts are used.<br />

Rater 1<br />

5.29(4.31)<br />

7.43(5.47)<br />

3.57(7.16)<br />

5.29(7.57)<br />

6.86(4.88)<br />

9.29(6.63)<br />

5.28(5.50)<br />

0.86(2.27)<br />

12.14(6.31<br />

)<br />

15.38(4.46<br />

)<br />

5.62(4.41)<br />

0.81(1.86)<br />

8.46(5.36)<br />

13.05(4.01<br />

)<br />

5.57(5.56)<br />

*S.D. in parenthesis<br />

Rater 2<br />

6.86(5.52)<br />

7.57(5.00)<br />

3.57(6.88)<br />

4.86(5.90)<br />

6.86(4.95)<br />

9.71(6.50)<br />

3.43(4.58)<br />

1.00(2.65)<br />

11.86(6.31<br />

)<br />

14.48(2.57<br />

)<br />

7.19(4.64)<br />

0.62(1.50)<br />

8.11(4.79)<br />

13.86(2.62<br />

)<br />

4.86(5.96)<br />

Rater 3<br />

6.14(4.88)<br />

7.14(4.49)<br />

3.71(6.97)<br />

4.00(5.45)<br />

6.29(4.96)<br />

8.86(6.23)<br />

3.00(5.20)<br />

1.00(2.65)<br />

11.43(6.21<br />

)<br />

16.19(4.03<br />

)<br />

6.19(3.80)<br />

0.62(1.64)<br />

8.49(4.97)<br />

15.62(3.75<br />

)<br />

4.57(5.88)<br />

Rater 4<br />

6.86(4.26)<br />

7.86(5.61)<br />

2.71(7.18)<br />

4.71(6.78)<br />

5.57(4.04)<br />

8.71(5.94)<br />

4.86(5.43)<br />

1.00(2.65)<br />

12.14(6.54<br />

)<br />

16.33(3.41<br />

)<br />

7.19(4.84)<br />

1.38(3.65)<br />

7.00(3.96)<br />

13.52(3.90<br />

)<br />

4.71(5.88)<br />

225<br />

Rater 5<br />

5.00(4.00)<br />

7.57(5.38)<br />

3.14(7.08)<br />

5.00(6.88)<br />

6.29(4.54)<br />

10.43(6.35<br />

)<br />

3.29(4.72)<br />

0.57(1.51)<br />

11.29(6.52<br />

)<br />

16.86(3.57<br />

)<br />

6.81(4.98)<br />

0.81(2.14)<br />

7.97(4.61)<br />

12.52(4.94<br />

)<br />

4.71(5.88)<br />

Rater 6<br />

5.57(4.20)<br />

7.57(5.88)<br />

2.57(6.80)<br />

3.57(4.79)<br />

6.43(4.47)<br />

10.00(6.35<br />

)<br />

5.43(6.40)<br />

1.00(2.65)<br />

11.71(6.45<br />

)<br />

14.67(4.23<br />

)<br />

6.29(3.82)<br />

0.71(1.89)<br />

7.94(4.66)<br />

13.00(3.21<br />

)<br />

4.86(5.52)<br />

Rater 7<br />

6.43(5.16)<br />

8.14(5.64)<br />

3.14(7.08)<br />

4.14(5.70)<br />

5.86(4.45)<br />

8.14(5.34)<br />

3.14(5.40)<br />

0.00(0.00)<br />

11.86(6.59)<br />

16.67(3.35)<br />

7.76(4.74)<br />

0.76(2.02)<br />

7.34(4.11)<br />

13.76(4.95)<br />

4.57(5.88)<br />

Table 19. Inter-rater reliability showing Cronbach alpha score for internal consistency<br />

<strong>of</strong> scores between raters; and correlation coefficient for linear association between scores<br />

<strong>of</strong> raters.<br />

Variables Cronbach<br />

Alpha<br />

Red<br />

Yellow<br />

Orange<br />

Purple<br />

.987<br />

.994<br />

.995<br />

.993<br />

Intra-Class<br />

.975<br />

.988<br />

.991<br />

.986<br />

277<br />

Graham Dunn (1989), Design and Analysis <strong>of</strong> Reliability Studies: the statistical evaluation <strong>of</strong><br />

measurement errors , London: Arnold, recommends the correlation coefficient; Carol Taylor-Fitzgibbon &<br />

Lynn Lyons-Morris (1987), How to analyse Data , Beverley Hills, CA: SAge, echo the concerns <strong>of</strong> Altman<br />

that linear association does not imply agreement and suggest additional notes <strong>of</strong> discrepancies and<br />

difference statistics.<br />

278 Fitzgibbon & Morris (1987) op.cit.


Green<br />

Blue<br />

Brown<br />

White<br />

Black<br />

Intensity<br />

Painted line<br />

Drawn line<br />

Space<br />

Em Tone<br />

Form<br />

.994<br />

.993<br />

.966<br />

.994<br />

.998<br />

.962<br />

.950<br />

.983<br />

.993<br />

.961<br />

.992<br />

.988<br />

.984<br />

.934<br />

.988<br />

.995<br />

.926<br />

.905<br />

.966<br />

.987<br />

.925<br />

.984<br />

Table 19 presents the results <strong>of</strong> the inter-rater correlations analysis. <strong>The</strong> two<br />

statistics support each other and give scores for each <strong>of</strong> the 15 elements <strong>of</strong> the test and<br />

are comparable with every other study which has been covered in the literature analysis.<br />

As the significance <strong>of</strong> a reliability co-efficient is its absolute size, the closer to 1.00 the<br />

better, the overall reliability for raters was excellent for each element across the range <strong>of</strong><br />

paintings (alpha >.98): Bausell's recommended reliability floor <strong>of</strong> .60 is well exceeded 279 .<br />

However, Altman's point on the inadequacy <strong>of</strong> the correlation coefficient is well taken,<br />

since the variation in the subject matter (pictures 1-7) is quite large (see raw data tables<br />

1-15, which show the data count for each rater for each variable, appendix 2), and this has<br />

possibly falsely increased the value <strong>of</strong> r, because <strong>of</strong> large fluctuations. It is<br />

inappropriate to use a measure which is sensitive to variation in the subjects and Altman<br />

extends this criticism to the use <strong>of</strong> regression analysis for the same purpose. It is<br />

however wise to use a correlation index, cautiously for support, to check for negative<br />

correlations; even though the level <strong>of</strong> scoring may be close, its rhythms may vary. It<br />

remains to say that the most informative statistics we can get for this study are those<br />

279<br />

R. Barker Bausell (1986), A Practical Guide to Conducting Empirical Research , New York: Harper<br />

Row, p.204-6.<br />

226


shown in table 16, the mean difference and limits <strong>of</strong> agreement, which provide a good<br />

summary <strong>of</strong> the data.<br />

Conclusion<br />

It may seem that I have gone into too many tests in this section, but the importance <strong>of</strong><br />

method comparison is paramount here for the individual subject and it is important to<br />

show why this method rather than another is used; there is little point in taking<br />

measurements between groups if the measurement method has poor repeatability, or if<br />

judgements may be made at different levels since systematic variation will occur.<br />

<strong>The</strong> limitations <strong>of</strong> this type <strong>of</strong> test and the resources available must be<br />

remembered here. For unreplicated studies it is difficult to compare the repeatability <strong>of</strong><br />

the measurement and thus also the resulting statistics because there are no other studies<br />

with a similar systematic approach which publish enough information to allow it. In<br />

some ways the test defines itself: if 6 raters are used, they have either learnt the method<br />

or not, and the variation in their scores will indicate the limits <strong>of</strong> agreement. <strong>The</strong> general<br />

trend <strong>of</strong> the measure indicates that the method is repeatable, but the raters are<br />

inexperienced, they were less motivated than I should have liked and the sample size was<br />

smaller. Good agreement can only be reached when all raters are accurate. <strong>The</strong><br />

correlations were high and the level <strong>of</strong> agreement only varied within 1 points for 76% <strong>of</strong><br />

the scale, and within 2 points for 86%. I therefore claim good reliability was achieved.<br />

Weaknesses <strong>of</strong> the instrument and problems with rating<br />

227


All <strong>of</strong> the scales were understood by the raters at the time except for FORM, where the<br />

basic rating criteria were not followed, rendering analysis irrelevant, so it was not<br />

reported at that time, pending further research. Further research amongst pr<strong>of</strong>essional<br />

<strong>art</strong>ists found that the terminology referred to an <strong>art</strong>istic application which visual<br />

description or definition varied considerably even according to their own criteria 280 .<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was theoretical agreement that significant or dominant form referred to dominating<br />

structural regular shapes. Wadlington and McWhinnie 281 also found that the term<br />

Dominant Form caused much confusion amongst raters, their advice was to discard the<br />

term 'dominant', but this merely fogs the form definition and removes the essential<br />

element, in my opinion. A further test, done with 7 similar University students (2 male,<br />

5 female age 19-24), without an <strong>art</strong> background and with a more detailed explanatory<br />

sheet 282 , produced a reliability score <strong>of</strong> 0.99. <strong>The</strong> students needed to look at the picture<br />

as a whole first, and the position <strong>of</strong> the term dominant form at the end <strong>of</strong> the rating sheet,<br />

and the previous sectioning into grids may have influenced the previous raters'<br />

understanding <strong>of</strong> the process, so repositioning <strong>of</strong> the form element was indicated.<br />

280 see appendix 5: Artist's form questionnaires, sample copy. I used these among 15 trained and training<br />

<strong>art</strong>ists to determine how the parameters <strong>of</strong> my definition should change.<br />

281 Wadlington and McWhinnie (1973) op.cit.<br />

282 see Appendix 5 again, more detailed explanation, category slightly changed.<br />

228


3. <strong>Study</strong> 1<br />

Permission and ethics: This project was submitted and passed by the North<br />

Staffordshire Medical Ethics Committee 1994. Permission to access patients notes and<br />

to use the paintings for research was sought primarily directly from patients themselves<br />

through written or oral consent and also from the consultant psychiatrists in charge <strong>of</strong><br />

their case. Consent forms explained the project in simple terms, that patients would not<br />

be identified and that the project had no connection with the treatment <strong>of</strong> patients.<br />

Sample consent forms and explanatory notes for patients can be found in Appendix 5.<br />

Oral consent was taken by hospital staff from the ward with whom the patient was<br />

already acquainted. One patient withdrew her pictures from the study pool although she<br />

attended the groups and made pictures with the rest until the end.<br />

Patient Selection: 39 p<strong>art</strong>icipants were drawn from mixed sex wards <strong>of</strong> a psychiatric<br />

hospital serving an industrial community in North Staffordshire, England. <strong>The</strong> hospital<br />

accepts the full range <strong>of</strong> adult psychiatric disorder requiring admission. Four self-<br />

contained specialised wards were selected to provide subjects for the study. <strong>The</strong>se wards<br />

treated substance abusers, the psychiatric consequences <strong>of</strong> brain injury, and psychotic<br />

disorders. <strong>The</strong> substance abusers were out-patients who attended a general hospital ward<br />

weekly; the other three wards were in-patient acute admission. <strong>The</strong> brain injury and one<br />

rehabilitation ward were sited in a large country mental hospital, the other psychotic<br />

ward in a community based mental health centre.<br />

229


Table 1: Demographics for experimental groups. <strong>Study</strong> 1<br />

Total Sex M Sex F Age<br />

Mean<br />

230<br />

Age S.D.<br />

Entire Population 50 33 17 34.8 11.0<br />

Affective Disorder 9 6 3 38.0 11.0<br />

Non-Affective Psychosis 10 7 3 36.8 11.4<br />

Brain Injury 11 10 1 38.4 14.6<br />

Drug Abuse 9 7 2 32.2 6.4<br />

Controls (staff) 11 3 8 29.0 8.2<br />

Table 1 shows demographic statistics: 39 psychiatric patients and 11 controls made<br />

paintings in the same conditions according to the same set criteria with standardised<br />

materials.<br />

Choice <strong>of</strong> subjects<br />

Patients: <strong>The</strong> groups <strong>of</strong> patients were chosen for availability and ease <strong>of</strong> access. Only<br />

patients who were judged able to give their consent and who volunteered p<strong>art</strong>icipated.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were no larger groups <strong>of</strong> patients with a single diagnosis available, but primarily<br />

this investigation was to test the sensitivity <strong>of</strong> the measure - and also to give an idea<br />

whether general effects (patients against controls) against specific effects shown in the<br />

literature could be investigated (diagnostic differences). Most <strong>of</strong> the patients were in<br />

acute stages <strong>of</strong> illness.<br />

Controls: 11 staff members from the Substance Abuse ward and from the Community<br />

Mental Health Centre, these included student nurses with no experience <strong>of</strong> therapy<br />

groups, agreed to act as controls and completed their paintings together with the patient


groups, so that control conditions were identical to those <strong>of</strong> the patients. <strong>The</strong> advantage<br />

<strong>of</strong> the same conditions and environment outweighed unequal numbers.<br />

Exclusions: Most patients identified were included in study 1 and there were no<br />

exclusions for missing data. <strong>The</strong> study was voluntary and therefore did not encompass<br />

the whole population <strong>of</strong> the identified wards. One patient who was found to suffer a<br />

psychiatric disorder other than those mentioned was dropped from the study. 2<br />

unidentified pictures which were probably by student nurses were also dropped from the<br />

study.<br />

Measures<br />

Age and sex were recorded and their means are shown in Table 1.<br />

IQ was not recorded as IQ was not routinely tested and the researcher was not clinically<br />

involved with patients and had no training for such measurement. In addition, such<br />

measures are known to be unreliable with psychiatric populations, chronicity and drugs.<br />

Drugs: patients were mostly under assessment and were on low or no drugs although this<br />

was not always true, many were suffering from the effects <strong>of</strong> previous use <strong>of</strong> unidentified<br />

substances.<br />

Chronicity <strong>of</strong> patients: It was initially decided to examine work by new patients in<br />

acute stages <strong>of</strong> illness and wards were selected with this aim to control for traditionally<br />

identified indicators which have been described previously as common to the institutional<br />

environment or the long term effects <strong>of</strong> drugs. However, patients were difficult to<br />

231


classify, even as in and out patients. <strong>The</strong> wards selected dealt with acute illnesses, but<br />

casenotes revealed that although most patients were in acute stages, the majority had<br />

previous admissions and few were new to hospital admission.<br />

Diagnosis<br />

<strong>The</strong> ICD10 clinical diagnostic classifications were taken from patients hospital records.<br />

Description <strong>of</strong> the use <strong>of</strong> ICD10 for the purposes <strong>of</strong> this research appears in the<br />

instruments section <strong>of</strong> the Main <strong>Study</strong>.<br />

Inter - rater reliability - DF. made all the clinical decisions, but decisions which differed<br />

from what appeared in the patient record were discussed with the psychiatrist in charge<br />

<strong>of</strong> the case.<br />

<strong>The</strong> DAPA<br />

<strong>The</strong> DAPA rating guide version 2 was used which appears in Appendix 4. This was<br />

essentially similar to that presented with the Main study, however there were<br />

differences:<br />

1. One statistic appears for Line as a summary <strong>of</strong> painted and drawn lines. Although<br />

drawn line was distinguished from painted line when estimating variability <strong>of</strong><br />

thickness, the two types <strong>of</strong> line posed relational problems in judgment. <strong>The</strong>y<br />

were separated in the rating guide when interrater reliability was measured. <strong>The</strong><br />

method <strong>of</strong> summarising was; squares would be scored as varied if either a drawn<br />

or painted line was <strong>of</strong> variable thickness; or there was a thick drawn line together<br />

232


with a thin painted line; or where a thick painted line appeared together with a<br />

thin drawn line;<br />

2. <strong>The</strong>re was no form measurement; and<br />

3. Space was rated as an estimate <strong>of</strong> how full the picture was in tenths, whereas<br />

further development changed to the more successful 'bubble method' presently<br />

used in Version 3 (main study) and interrater study. Version 2 <strong>of</strong> the DAPA<br />

rating guide can be found in Appendix 4. Scoring procedures were the same.<br />

Treatment <strong>of</strong> missing data<br />

Painting Combination: Not all the subjects completed all the paintings. This could<br />

produce bias, as content in the early paintings is therefore emphasised. A Kruskal-Wallis<br />

ANOVA performed on the groups and the number <strong>of</strong> paintings each patient completed<br />

suggested a trend (_ 2 9.22, df=4, p=0.056). <strong>The</strong> number <strong>of</strong> paintings from each patient<br />

was collapsed into three ordered categories (1; 2, or 3; 4 or 5), and two-way ANOVAs<br />

(No. <strong>of</strong> paintings by diagnostic type) were performed on four significant variables. <strong>The</strong><br />

variable scores did not vary with the number <strong>of</strong> paintings (F=0.02-0.52, df=2, N.S.), and<br />

there were no significant interaction effects (F=0.43-0.59, df=6, N.S.).<br />

Outliers: no extreme values were excluded in study 1 as this was an exploratory study<br />

and numbers were too small to determine whether these were important.<br />

Distribution <strong>of</strong> data: <strong>The</strong>re was no need for transformation <strong>of</strong> data for the initial study<br />

as the Kolmogorov-Smirnov goodness <strong>of</strong> fit test revealed a normal distribution over the<br />

variable spread. Most <strong>of</strong> the raw data was rank-ordered or classificatory, but the final<br />

233


score for each subject was a ratio <strong>of</strong> the distribution <strong>of</strong> each variable over their output <strong>of</strong><br />

paintings, fulfilling the conditions for parametric analysis.<br />

Comparison <strong>of</strong> groups: Statistical procedures are the same as those covered in the main<br />

study. Analysis <strong>of</strong> variance tests compared the variances for individuals within and<br />

between groups and tested the differences between the means <strong>of</strong> five diagnostic types<br />

over each <strong>of</strong> the 13 variables. However, Anova assumes the same variance between all<br />

groups and there may have been variance differences between groups not apparent<br />

because <strong>of</strong> the small samples, which would mask differences. <strong>The</strong> Multiple Range Test<br />

(Duncan procedure), which applies t-tests between each independent sample for each<br />

variable and which used the Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, highlighted<br />

the most different groups as before. This use <strong>of</strong> the Bonferroni is most suitable for<br />

groups with uncorrected variances. If differences <strong>of</strong> variances exist, it is likely that<br />

variances between groups are different as whole.<br />

Procedure<br />

Procedure for diagnoses: ICD-10 diagnoses were made in the same way as the main<br />

study, from case records by Dr. D. Foreman D.F. was blind to the originating ward and<br />

paintings while making the diagnoses.<br />

Table 2: Diagnosis: Assignment to type.<br />

ICD-10 DIAGNOSTIC CATEGORIES (1993) N Assigned<br />

to type<br />

Moderate Depressive Episodes: (1 X F31.6, F32.1, F32.10; 3 X F33.1). 6 1<br />

234


ICD-10 DIAGNOSTIC CATEGORIES (1993) N Assigned<br />

to type<br />

Schizophrenia/Psychosis: (1 X F10.73, F16.7, F19.5, F19.7, F20.0, F20.1,<br />

F25.0, F25.2; 2 X F20.5).<br />

10 2<br />

Organic Brain Injury: (1 X F06.3, F06.8, F10.6; 5 X F07.0). 8 3<br />

Substance Abuse: (1 X F10.4, F11.2, F12.2, F18.2, F19.3; 4 X F10.2) 9 4<br />

Moderate Recurrent Depression + Alcohol Dependency: (F33.1 + F10.2). 1 1<br />

Specific Personality Disorder: (F33.1 + F60.8). 2 1<br />

Huntingdons + Dementia: (G10 + F02.2). 2 3<br />

Organic Psycho Syndrome + Epilepsy: (F07.9 + G40). 1 3<br />

Diagnostic types: (1) Affective Disorder, (2) Non-Affective Psychosis, (3) Brain Injury, (4) Drug Abuse<br />

Table 2 shows category definition <strong>of</strong> case assignment to 4 types; Affective<br />

Disorder (n=9); Non-Affective Psychosis (n=10); Brain Injury (n=11); Drug Abuse<br />

(n=9). This includes six cases with multiple diagnoses whose assigned categories are also<br />

shown. Type assignment was made on the basis <strong>of</strong> the clinically presenting<br />

<strong>psychopathology</strong> on admission; e.g. a case presenting drug dependency and psychosis,<br />

not resulting from immediate drug toxicity was categorised as Non-Affective Psychosis.<br />

All the patients available for <strong>art</strong> in the identified wards were used. This meant that some<br />

patients had secondary diagnoses and may have overlapped groups.<br />

Painting Generation: <strong>The</strong> paintings were produced in <strong>art</strong> familiarisation groups, run<br />

as p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the recreational program <strong>of</strong> the hospital. All painting procedures were<br />

discussed with the region's <strong>art</strong> therapist to maximise the experiential and reducing<br />

similarity to psychotherapeutic sessions.<br />

<strong>The</strong> <strong>art</strong> groups took place at the same place and time each week in daylight hours,<br />

took one hour a week for 5-6 weeks, and contained up to 8 p<strong>art</strong>icipants who were either<br />

patients or staff from the ward. I knew the patients from the non-patients but was<br />

unaware <strong>of</strong> diagnosis and therapeutically unacquainted with the group. I ensured that<br />

235


each p<strong>art</strong>icipant received the same materials and instructions and maintained a neutral<br />

supportive atmosphere in collaboration with a member <strong>of</strong> the hospital's therapeutic team<br />

in each ward. <strong>The</strong> work was collected and rated by me at the end <strong>of</strong> the series.<br />

P<strong>art</strong>icipants painted in rooms where there were no pictures visible. <strong>The</strong>y worked in<br />

groups <strong>of</strong> two and three, placed so that they could not see each others work without<br />

effort. Table tops rather than easels were used so that <strong>art</strong> could take place in the usual<br />

environment <strong>of</strong> the ward rather than in a special place which may have proved difficult<br />

for some p<strong>art</strong>icipants. Two <strong>of</strong> the wards had not previously been <strong>of</strong>fered <strong>art</strong>, but this<br />

approach limited disruption to ward routine. <strong>The</strong> other two groups had used this format<br />

before.<br />

<strong>The</strong>mes were set to hold the content constant and provoke personal involvement<br />

from the patients, because it is recognised that sometimes methods such as free painting<br />

can be demanding for people with functional impairments and they <strong>of</strong>ten produce no<br />

picture at all 283 . <strong>The</strong> thematic focus was self portraiture: previous research on draw-a-<br />

person studies has demonstrated that figure drawing works across a wide variety <strong>of</strong><br />

abilities, and suggests some personal involvement with the work. Staff were instructed<br />

to be encouraging but not to make comments or suggestions. <strong>The</strong> same subject matter<br />

was specified in all groups for 5 sessions based around self portraiture: (1) Draw yourself<br />

(2) Draw yourself as you would look if you were an animal (3) A picture which shows<br />

you doing something you enjoy (4) A picture <strong>of</strong> your life with future and past (5) A<br />

picture which shows how you are feeling now. One person with cerebral injury used 5<br />

283<br />

J.B. Couch (1994), DDS Research with older people diagnosed with organic mental syndrome and<br />

disorders, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.11(2):111-115.<br />

236


sessions for the self portrait because <strong>of</strong> his extreme slowness in manipulation, producing<br />

only 2 paintings in all. All the other p<strong>art</strong>icipants were asked to finish their paintings<br />

within the hour. Most p<strong>art</strong>icipants made one painting per session, although one or two<br />

individuals drew two or more for the first session. All groups had a mixture <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong><br />

materials; wax crayon, tempera paint, pencil and thick and thin brushes so that the range<br />

<strong>of</strong> colours and line quality was available. Most <strong>of</strong> the paper provided was white 17" x<br />

12", but some 24" x 17" was also available. Materials were set on the table from the<br />

st<strong>art</strong>.<br />

237


Results<br />

Interaction effects: <strong>The</strong> mean age <strong>of</strong> the staff was only slightly lower than that <strong>of</strong> the<br />

other groups but there was a clear predominance <strong>of</strong> males in the sample (see table 1).<br />

Only one variable, Space, was found to be influenced by Sex. Space was initially<br />

identified as a variable which distinguished between groups, but there was an interaction<br />

effect with Sex. When co-varied (2-way ANOVA F=0.89, df=4, P=0.47) neither variable<br />

turned out to be significant by itself. No other variable was influenced by sex.<br />

Paintings: Table 3 identifies differentiating variables in bold type, using the Duncan<br />

procedure; significant ANOVA results are indicated.<br />

Table 3: Multiple ranges: group means significantly different at 0.05% level. Dncan<br />

Procedure.<br />

Groups Affective<br />

Disorder<br />

n = 9<br />

Non-Affective<br />

Psychosis<br />

n = 10<br />

238<br />

Brain Injury<br />

n = 11<br />

Drug Abuse<br />

n = 9<br />

Controls<br />

n = 11<br />

VARIABLE mean SD mean SD mean SD mean SD mean SD<br />

Red<br />

Purple<br />

Green<br />

Blue<br />

Brown<br />

White<br />

Black<br />

Space<br />

Em tone<br />

Yellow +<br />

Orange +<br />

Intensity +<br />

Line +<br />

.24 .17<br />

.08 .11<br />

.34 .22<br />

.37 .25<br />

.20 .19<br />

.08 .11<br />

.60 .27<br />

7.71 1.79<br />

1.83 .22<br />

.14 .11<br />

.13* .14<br />

1.92 .53<br />

1.38 .50<br />

.42 .19<br />

.05 .11<br />

.28 .20<br />

.28 .19<br />

.16 .14<br />

.05 .09<br />

.45 .27<br />

5.81 2.65<br />

1.95 .26<br />

.18 .15<br />

.04* .07<br />

1.99 .40<br />

1.73 .28<br />

.22 .17<br />

.13 .23<br />

.23 .19<br />

.34 .26<br />

.13 .23<br />

.10 .13<br />

.40 .28<br />

5.84 2.85<br />

2.04 .13<br />

.12* .15<br />

.04* .06<br />

1.96 .46<br />

1.13* .70<br />

.25 .16<br />

.14 .18<br />

.27 .24<br />

.31 .17<br />

.33 .18<br />

.12 .09<br />

.44 .23<br />

7.42 1.92<br />

1.83 .20<br />

.29 .16<br />

.37 .13<br />

2.00 .48<br />

1.56 .37<br />

.42 .33<br />

.05 .11<br />

.32 .27<br />

.44 .16<br />

.14 .17<br />

.11 .21<br />

.50 .32<br />

7.90 1.74<br />

1.90 .39<br />

38* .33<br />

.07* .09<br />

2.50 .36<br />

1.99* .72<br />

df = 4.<br />

+ Variables identified by ANOVA as Significantly differentiating groups at the 5% level: yellow (p


orange. Brain Injury were the next most distinguishable type, and there was least<br />

difference between non-affective psychosis and affective disorders.<br />

Discussion<br />

Much <strong>of</strong> the first study was done on an exploratory basis and some procedures could<br />

have been better controlled. <strong>The</strong> group containing mostly patients with brain injury and<br />

also the affective disorder group made a small number <strong>of</strong> paintings on coloured paper,<br />

which may have influenced the choice <strong>of</strong> colours. However, this effect was minimised<br />

through averaging between paintings for each patient. <strong>The</strong> paintings were rated from the<br />

obvious 'right way up' and from the identification procedure which marks the back right<br />

hand corner <strong>of</strong> the work, but orientation <strong>of</strong> the paper to landscape/portrait might have<br />

had distortion effects on composition, thus influencing the space score, especially with<br />

the brain injury group. Better control would have been achieved by specifying p<strong>art</strong>icular<br />

orientation <strong>of</strong> the paper.<br />

Few dimensions from previous studies actually compared with the variables <strong>of</strong><br />

this study. However, the meta-analysis <strong>of</strong> the literature, supported by the qualitative<br />

analysis, showed significant differences in ratings <strong>of</strong> form variables relating to patient<br />

status and between patient groups. <strong>The</strong> results <strong>of</strong> this study were consistent with these<br />

findings, although not with the conclusions and 'common knowledge' implied in the<br />

literature, especially for depressed patients: their use <strong>of</strong> black was similar to that <strong>of</strong> other<br />

patients, but they did use more orange than psychotics, normal controls and brain injured<br />

patients; their paintings did not seem more negative than those <strong>of</strong> other patients. <strong>The</strong>re<br />

239


was no evidence <strong>of</strong> impoverishment for Depressed or Schizophrenic patients. Unlike<br />

previous literature 284 , space filled did not discriminate groups. However, the size <strong>of</strong><br />

paper used was not standardised and varied between 17" x 12" and 24" x 17". This had<br />

the effect that the media became more widely distributed over the picture plane, thus<br />

variably reducing the score for space. Emotive tone did not produce a discriminable<br />

dimension, possibly because it was measured as a distribution over the picture plane.<br />

This would tend to even out positive and negative scores to neutral.<br />

<strong>The</strong> diagnoses were consistent with the ICD-10 (1993), but patients with<br />

multiple diagnoses were included. This may have blurred the distinction between groups<br />

and so produce conservative errors. Some non-significant results could easily be due to<br />

lack <strong>of</strong> power, owing to small group size. However, this does not explain the positive<br />

results.<br />

Conclusion and limitations <strong>of</strong> the study<br />

This study presented a reliable method <strong>of</strong> describing paintings for research in an<br />

appropriate systematic way suitable for analysis. <strong>The</strong> method is general enough to be<br />

used by investigators with different theoretical backgrounds and shows promise for<br />

future development. <strong>The</strong> sample size <strong>of</strong> this study was only sufficient to categorise<br />

differences in patients and controls as the clinically differentiated groups were very small<br />

and included mixed diagnoses. <strong>The</strong>re were suggestions, however, that this measure may<br />

284<br />

A. Kirk & A. Kertesz (1989), Hemispheric contributions to drawing, Neuropsychologia,<br />

V.27(6):881-6;<br />

J.H. Wright & M.P. Macintyre (1982), <strong>The</strong> Family Drawing Depression Scale, J. Clin. Psychol. ,<br />

V.38(4):853-61; M. Milkjkovitch & G.M. Irvine (1982), Comparison <strong>of</strong> drawing preferences <strong>of</strong><br />

schizophrenics, other psychiatric patients and non-schizophrenic children on a draw-a-village task, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , V.9:203-16; and N.M. Knapp (1994), Research with diagnostic drawings for normal and<br />

Alzheimer's subjects, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.11(2):131-8.<br />

240


detect formal differences in the painting <strong>of</strong> patients with different psychiatric diagnoses.<br />

<strong>The</strong> study also showed selection bias, which, although impossible to quantify, merits<br />

some consideration due to the nature <strong>of</strong> volunteer groups.<br />

241


Chapter 5. Results<br />

This chapter presents the results <strong>of</strong> the main analyses for study 2. <strong>The</strong>re are 3 sections:<br />

1. Treatment <strong>of</strong> the data, which details the preliminary procedures to prepare the<br />

raw data for analysis; control <strong>of</strong> potential confounding variables, and<br />

transformation to normal distributions where appropriate;<br />

2. Results <strong>of</strong> the main analyses.<br />

3. Results <strong>of</strong> the subordinate analyses.<br />

Preparation <strong>of</strong> the data<br />

Transformation <strong>of</strong> the data: Tables and figures 1a-l, 2a-f and 3a-f can be found in<br />

Appendix 2. This set <strong>of</strong> statistics presents the raw data before and after transformation.<br />

Each variable is shown as: (i) Boxplots, for easy comparison between groups; (ii) Tables<br />

<strong>of</strong> means, for each group and whole population; (iii) Spreadlevel plot <strong>of</strong> the logarithmic<br />

co-ordinates for each group from which the Levene statistics are calculated. Other tables<br />

which summarise the information are presented in text.<br />

Variables which did not show a normal distribution<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was marked heterogeneity <strong>of</strong> variance within groups for 9 untransformed variables;<br />

red, yellow, orange, purple, brown, white, painted line, drawn line and form. Tables and<br />

figures 1a-i (appendix 2) show the raw data for variables which were transformed or<br />

deleted. Levene test results and projected transformations <strong>of</strong> the data to achieve normal<br />

distributions are presented in table 4 below.<br />

242


Table 4, showing projected transformations for data based on computations designed to<br />

verify the assumptions <strong>of</strong> the ANOVA test.<br />

Tests <strong>of</strong> homogeneity <strong>of</strong> variance [df1=4; df2=104]<br />

Dependent<br />

variable<br />

Red<br />

Yellow<br />

Orange<br />

Purple<br />

Green<br />

Blue<br />

Brown<br />

White<br />

Black<br />

Intensity<br />

Paint-Line<br />

Drawn-Line<br />

Space<br />

Em-Tone<br />

Form<br />

Levene Statistic and<br />

significance level<br />

3.0023 (p=0.02)<br />

4.2948 (p=0.00)<br />

5.4983 (p=0.00)<br />

*<br />

1.9975 (p=0.10)<br />

1.2702 (p=0.29)<br />

3.1268 (p=0.02)<br />

*<br />

2.7400 (p=0.03)<br />

2.5948 (p=0.04)<br />

*<br />

2.9744 (p=0.02)<br />

1.6085 (p=0.18)<br />

2.4954 (p=0.05)<br />

6.0704 (p=0.00)<br />

Power for<br />

transformation<br />

.329<br />

.709<br />

.529<br />

*<br />

.789<br />

-.198<br />

.544<br />

*<br />

1.391<br />

0.502<br />

*<br />

1.241<br />

4.756<br />

1.622<br />

0.657<br />

243<br />

Action<br />

SQRT<br />

SQRT<br />

SQRT<br />

*<br />

LEAVE<br />

LEAVE<br />

SQRT<br />

*<br />

SQUARE<br />

SQROOT<br />

*<br />

SQUARE<br />

LEAVE<br />

SQUARE<br />

SQROOT<br />

* Median not positive so LN <strong>of</strong> Spread vs LN <strong>of</strong> Level is not plotted.<br />

Homogeneity <strong>of</strong> variance not tested. Insufficient unique spread/level pairs to compute the regression slope.<br />

Table 5, showing transformed data based on computations designed to verify the<br />

assumptions <strong>of</strong> the ANOVA test.<br />

Tests <strong>of</strong> homogeneity <strong>of</strong> variance [df1=4; df2=104]<br />

Dependent<br />

variable<br />

Red<br />

Yellow<br />

Orange<br />

Brown<br />

Black<br />

Intensity<br />

Drawn line<br />

Em tone<br />

Form<br />

Levene Statistic and<br />

significance level<br />

0.6942 (p=0.5976)<br />

1.8480 (p=0.1253)<br />

4.7938 (p=0.0014)<br />

1.2466 (p=0.2959)<br />

5.0957 (p=0.0009)<br />

2.8094 (p=0.0292)<br />

1.8426 (p=0.1263)<br />

2.4488 (p=0.0508)<br />

1.0938 (p=0.3636)<br />

Action<br />

RETAIN<br />

RETAIN<br />

+<br />

RETAIN<br />

*<br />

+<br />

RETAIN<br />

+<br />

RETAIN<br />

* Squaring the data did not produce appreciable difference and the removal <strong>of</strong> outliers did not affect the<br />

distribution. <strong>The</strong>refore no clear appropriate transformation <strong>of</strong> this figure emerged.<br />

+ Although there was some improvement on Orange, values remained highly significant so this variable<br />

was dropped. <strong>The</strong> other 2 variables, Emotional Tone only gained slightly and was only just significant<br />

in its original state so was judged better left within tolerance (0.05); Intensity became significant on<br />

transformation (p=0.04) so it was also left.<br />

Variables left untransformed<br />

Transformation achieved no better results for homogeneity in the population than the<br />

original statistics for 3 variables: black, intensity, emotional tone (tables/figs 2a-f,


appendix 2) and these were left in their original state along with those that showed<br />

homogeneity in the Levene tests: green, blue, space (table 4 below).<br />

Transformed variables<br />

6 variables were successfully transformed: red, yellow, brown, painted 285 and drawn line,<br />

form, (tables and figures 3a-f, appendix 2). Levene calculations for homogeneity<br />

appear in table 5 above.<br />

To summarise, twelve variables which remained in the final analysis, transformed<br />

or not (tables 2a-f and 3a-f). Two variables, Purple (see table/fig. 1d, appendix 2) and<br />

White (1h, appendix 2), were dropped at this stage for 2 reasons; (1) their values were<br />

not plottable because <strong>of</strong> insufficient use, and (2) homogeneity tests within the analysis<br />

<strong>of</strong> variance (B<strong>art</strong>letts-Box, detailed later in this chapter, table 8) showed their range was<br />

unacceptable. Plots <strong>of</strong> their standardised differences (plots 7b and c,) showed<br />

concentration <strong>of</strong> cases below the mean. White was obviously irredeemable; the plot (7a,<br />

appendix 2) showed concentrations at 0 for all groups. Purple however (7b), looked more<br />

promising: although there were concentrations below the mean (at very low levels) for<br />

most groups, group 4 showed a wider scatter <strong>of</strong> differences. Cutting <strong>of</strong>f all values <strong>of</strong><br />

Purple above 0.28 (9 cases) did not redeem the median from negative but B<strong>art</strong>letts-Box<br />

showed acceptable homogeneity (F = 1.751; p=0.136). However, all previous<br />

significance disappeared when the ANOVA was calculated (F = 1.81; p=0.132). <strong>The</strong>se<br />

285 Painted line was retained because, although the square root transformation <strong>of</strong> Painted Line had a<br />

negative median, the calculation for the B<strong>art</strong>letts Box test (table 8) placed the variance within acceptable<br />

levels.<br />

244


findings are unreliable because cases were mostly removed from one group: 6 cases from<br />

the control group, 2 from group 5 and one from group 3, so purple was dropped.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were 2 further variables, Orange and Black ((table/fig 2a and 2b, appendix<br />

2), which showed significant heterogeneity in the groups. Transformation <strong>of</strong> the data did<br />

not produce appreciable difference and the removal <strong>of</strong> outliers did not affect the<br />

distribution. <strong>The</strong>refore no clear appropriate transformation <strong>of</strong> these figures emerged.<br />

<strong>The</strong> assumption that the groups came from populations with the same variance is<br />

important: the standard differences from the mean <strong>of</strong> each variable by group was plotted<br />

(Plots 6a-m and 7a-c, appendix 2). <strong>The</strong> plot for Orange (7a) clearly shows that diagnostic<br />

groups 1 and 4 have a much wider scatter <strong>of</strong> differences than the others and there are<br />

cases concentrated below the mean. <strong>The</strong> plot for Black (6f) shows only one group<br />

(controls) with wide variance and no concentrations <strong>of</strong> cases. <strong>The</strong> results <strong>of</strong> the<br />

B<strong>art</strong>letts-Box Homogeneity <strong>of</strong> Variance Test 286 (table 8 below) showed the range was<br />

unacceptable for Orange and it was dropped from the analysis, but was within tolerance<br />

for the ANOVA for Black which was retained.<br />

Normality <strong>of</strong> the final distribution<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were 12 remaining variables (tables/figures 2a-f and 3a-f, appendix 2 show mean<br />

values, standard deviations and standard error <strong>of</strong> the mean (how much the sample means<br />

vary in repeated samples from the same population)). Most <strong>of</strong> the standard errors were<br />

286<br />

B<strong>art</strong>letts test is an extension <strong>of</strong> the F test for assessing the null hypothesis that more than two samples<br />

come from populations with the same variance, recommended with ANOVA: P. Armitage and G. Berry<br />

(1987), Statistical Methods in Medical Research , Oxford: Blackwell, p.209.<br />

245


fairly small, less than 0.1, indicating homogeneity within groups. <strong>The</strong>re were no<br />

consistent outliers which could be easily eliminated and at this early stage it was not clear<br />

exactly what was important, so it would be unwise to change the data, and possibly the<br />

assumptions, merely to decrease the noise in the data.<br />

5 plots <strong>of</strong> group variances (plots 6a-m shown as the standard differences from<br />

each group's mean, Appendix 2) showed a wider scatter <strong>of</strong> differences for diagnostic<br />

group 4 (controls) generally, and five show a smaller scatter for group 2 (depressed). Six<br />

plots showed concentrations <strong>of</strong> group 1 (schizophrenics) cases below the mean, but<br />

generally the plots show a similar overall distribution for each <strong>of</strong> the five groups on every<br />

variable. A certain amount <strong>of</strong> deviation does not compromise the ANOVA test, even in<br />

unbalanced designs, providing group sizes result from chance fluctuation as fits the case<br />

here.<br />

<strong>The</strong> Main Analyses: comparison <strong>of</strong> Groups<br />

Table 9 identifies 10 variables as significantly different, using the results <strong>of</strong> an ANOVA<br />

performed among the 4 patient groups and the control group, pairwise comparisons are<br />

indicated using the modified LSD procedure, which compensates for multiple tests. F-<br />

values are shown in table 8.<br />

246


Table 8: showing results <strong>of</strong> analysis <strong>of</strong> variance for the purpose <strong>of</strong> identifying diagnostic<br />

grouping variables between 4 groups <strong>of</strong> psychiatric patients (total n=86) and 1 control<br />

group (n=23) on 13 formal measures <strong>of</strong> their paintings.<br />

DAPA<br />

Variable<br />

Blue<br />

Brown<br />

Orange<br />

Red<br />

Yellow<br />

Green<br />

Black<br />

Intensity<br />

Paint Line<br />

Drawn Line<br />

Space<br />

Em'Tone<br />

Form<br />

mean (S.D.) Confidence<br />

Interval<br />

.26 .20<br />

.30 .24<br />

.09 .12<br />

.45 .26<br />

.38 .25<br />

.25 .23<br />

.46 .28<br />

1.96 .48<br />

.31 .49<br />

1.03 1.05<br />

2.00 .48<br />

2.14 .36<br />

.39 .21<br />

0.22-0.30<br />

0.26-0.35<br />

0.07-0.11<br />

0.40-0.50<br />

0.34-0.43<br />

0.20-0.29<br />

0.41-0.51<br />

1.87-2.05<br />

0.42-0.60<br />

0.83-1.23<br />

1.91-2.09<br />

2.07-2.21<br />

0.35-0.43<br />

B<strong>art</strong>lett-Box<br />

Homogeneity<br />

<strong>of</strong> variance<br />

(sig. level)<br />

1.62 (0.17)<br />

0.75 (0.56)<br />

6.42 (0.00)<br />

1.00 (0.41)<br />

0.95 (0.44)<br />

1.79 (0.13)<br />

2.24 (0.06)<br />

2.45 (0.05)<br />

2.37 (0.05)<br />

2.05 (0.09)<br />

0.96 (0.43)<br />

2.48 (0.04)<br />

1.25 (0.29)<br />

Table 9: Multiple Comparisons: Duncan Procedure<br />

GROUPS Affective<br />

Disorder<br />

n = 18<br />

Schizophrenia<br />

n = 35<br />

247<br />

Personality<br />

Disorder<br />

n = 9<br />

F.<br />

Ratio<br />

1.00<br />

1.64<br />

1.63<br />

4.33<br />

2.62<br />

4.25<br />

8.12<br />

3.41<br />

3.21<br />

3.86<br />

3.05<br />

5.34<br />

6.31<br />

F.<br />

Prob.<br />

.409<br />

.170<br />

.172<br />

.003<br />

.039<br />

.003<br />

.000<br />

.011<br />

.015<br />

.006<br />

.020<br />

.001<br />

.000<br />

Drug Abuse<br />

n = 24<br />

Controls<br />

n = 23<br />

VARIABLE mean SD mean SD mean SD mean SD mean SD<br />

blue<br />

brown<br />

Red☺<br />

Yellow☺<br />

Green☺<br />

Black☺<br />

Intensity☺<br />

Linepaint☺<br />

Linedraw☺<br />

Space☺<br />

Em tone☺<br />

Form☺<br />

.21 .16<br />

.29 .24<br />

.47 .25<br />

.45 .26<br />

.26 .25<br />

.50* .27<br />

2.04 .51<br />

.59* .47<br />

.84* .69<br />

2.07* .54<br />

2.14* .20<br />

.23_ .18<br />

.25 .18<br />

.24 .24<br />

.43 .26<br />

.30* .24<br />

.21* .22<br />

.30_ .21<br />

1.75_ .52<br />

.55 .52<br />

.93 1.16<br />

2.00 .47<br />

2.11* .29<br />

.37* .16<br />

.26 .15<br />

.34 .16<br />

.47 .26<br />

.40 .20<br />

.21* .16<br />

.43 .27<br />

2.09* .36<br />

.50 .55<br />

1.39 1.27<br />

2.22* .29<br />

2.16* .38<br />

.39* .19<br />

.24 .20<br />

.30 .21<br />

.30* .18<br />

.35 .20<br />

.16* .16<br />

.66* .18<br />

1.96 .29<br />

.22_ .29<br />

1.62_ 1.00<br />

2.13* .47<br />

1.94* .42<br />

.41* .19<br />

Analysis <strong>of</strong> Variance<br />

df = 4; ranges 2.81-3.12; harmonic mean cell size = 17.8327<br />

☺ Variables identified by ANOVA as significantly differentiating groups (p


Red, Green, Black, Drawn Line, Emotional Tone and Form all distinguished<br />

diagnostic categories at high levels <strong>of</strong> significance (p


Colour: Red, Yellow and Brown are distinguished by *T* (meaning transformed). <strong>The</strong><br />

normal distribution was achieved by taking the square root transformation; the lower<br />

values were pushed from zero (no red) towards 1 (colour in every p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the picture).<br />

<strong>The</strong> greatest effect was to compress the latter half <strong>of</strong> the scale (half covered - fully<br />

covered), so that a score <strong>of</strong> 0.75 reveals that half the picture has the colour in it. <strong>The</strong><br />

dotted line indicates a half way point on each plot. <strong>The</strong> plots are divided in two. <strong>The</strong><br />

upper section shows those groups showing significant differences, with the differential<br />

single group in bold, and the whole group is marked '*'. Where there are two differences,<br />

there are two bold differentials; group* first, the second group is marked ''.<br />

10a. Confidence intervals and means by diagnostic type<br />

for variable RED *T*<br />

______________________________________<br />

_ : _<br />

*2_ •_|__• : _2 Substance abuse<br />

_ : _<br />

T *4_ •___|___•: _4 Controls<br />

Y _ : _<br />

P ______________________________________<br />

E 1_ •__|__• : _1 Schizophrenia<br />

_ : _<br />

3_ •____|___• : _3 Depression<br />

_ : _<br />

5_ •______|_____• : _5 Personality disorder<br />

_ : _<br />

______________________________________<br />

0 .25 .5 .75 1 *(p


1. Neither schizophrenics nor depressives were distinguishable from other patient groups<br />

or controls.<br />

2. Substance abusers used hardly any red, were the lowest scoring patient group, and<br />

scored significantly lower than controls.<br />

3. Diagnostic differences were not distinguishable between patient groups; personality<br />

disorder spanned all groups.<br />

4. As expected, the mean <strong>of</strong> the controls was higher than all patient groups (although not<br />

significantly different from any except substance abusers).<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was no evidence for red as a predictor <strong>of</strong> schizophrenic or depressive diagnosis.<br />

10b. Confidence intervals and means by diagnostic type<br />

for variable YELLOW *T*<br />

______________________________________<br />

_ : _<br />

*1_ •__|__• : _1 Schizophrenia<br />

_ : _<br />

T *4_ •___|___• : _4 Controls<br />

Y ______________________________________<br />

P 2_ •__|_• : _2 Substance abuse<br />

E _ : _<br />

3_ •___|___• : _3 Depression<br />

_ : _<br />

5_ •____|____• : _5 Personality disorder<br />

_ : _<br />

______________________________________<br />

0 .25 .5 .75 1 *(p


1. Schizophrenics but not depressives used very little yellow and significantly less than<br />

controls.<br />

2. Diagnostic differences were not distinguishable between patient groups.<br />

3. Against expectations, depressives covered the same range as the controls, so therefore<br />

there was no indication <strong>of</strong> low quantities <strong>of</strong> yellow as a predictor <strong>of</strong> depressive diagnosis.<br />

10c. Confidence intervals and means by diagnostic type<br />

for variable GREEN<br />

______________________________________<br />

*4_ •__|___: _4 Controls<br />

_ : _<br />

*1_ •_|__• : _1 Schizophrenia<br />

T _ : _<br />

Y *2_ •_|_• : _2 Substance abuse<br />

P _ : _<br />

E *5_ •___|___• : _5 Personality disorder<br />

_ : _<br />

______________________________________<br />

_ : _<br />

3_ •___|___• : _3 Depression<br />

______________________________________<br />

0 .25 .5 .75 1 *(p


schizophrenia was indistinguishable from other patient results. <strong>The</strong> lowest mean was for<br />

substance abusers. Patient status could be inferred from less green but not depression.<br />

10d. Confidence intervals and means by diagnostic type<br />

for variable BLUE<br />

______________________________________<br />

1_ •_|_• : _1 Schizophrenia<br />

_ : _<br />

T 2_ •__|__• : _2 Substance abuse<br />

Y _ : _<br />

P 3_ •_|__• : _3 Depression<br />

E _ : _<br />

4_ •___|___• : _4 Controls<br />

_ : _<br />

5_ •___|___• : _5 Personality disorder<br />

_ : _<br />

______________________________________<br />

0 .25 .5 .75 1 *(p


*T* Brown: is a dark, sombre and neutral colour, commonly understood as a dead colour,<br />

associated with waste and p<strong>art</strong>icularly excretion. More use <strong>of</strong> brown in pictures by<br />

depressives and schizophrenics than other patient groups could be expected, and more<br />

from patients than controls.<br />

1. <strong>The</strong>re were no significant differences within patient groups or between patients and<br />

controls.<br />

2. Against expectations, the control group mean was the highest (although not<br />

significantly higher) than patient groups. <strong>The</strong>re was no evidence to support indications<br />

<strong>of</strong> schizophrenia, depression or patient status from use <strong>of</strong> brown. All groups used<br />

equally low quantities.<br />

10f. Confidence intervals and means by diagnostic type<br />

for variable BLACK<br />

______________________________________<br />

*1_ •_|_• : _1 Schizophrenia<br />

_ : _<br />

*2_ : •__|__• _2 Substance abuse<br />

T _ : _<br />

Y *3_ •____|___• _3 Depression<br />

P _ : _<br />

E *4_ •____|:___• _4 Controls<br />

_ : _<br />

5_ •______|______• _5 Personality disorder<br />

_ : _<br />

______________________________________<br />

0 .25 .5 .75 1 *(p


Black: Black is the darkest colour and has a common association with death and<br />

emptiness. Depressives were expected to use most, schizophrenics next, other patients<br />

could be expected to use less black and controls least.<br />

1. Depressives were not differentiated from controls on use <strong>of</strong> black.<br />

2. Schizophrenics used least black, significantly less than depressives and controls.<br />

3. Depressives, schizophrenics and controls used significantly less black than substance<br />

abusers. <strong>The</strong>se results indicate support indicators <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia for small quantities<br />

<strong>of</strong> black, which is against expectations. <strong>The</strong> highest score was from substance abusers,<br />

who spread black consistently throughout more than half <strong>of</strong> the picture surface and were<br />

differentiated from all other groups. Controls and depressives were not differentiable<br />

from non-schizophrenic other patients on high or low use <strong>of</strong> black.<br />

254


10g. Confidence intervals and means by diagnostic type<br />

for variable INTENSITY OF COLOUR<br />

________________________________________<br />

*1_ •__| • _1 Schizophrenia<br />

_ : _<br />

*4_ : •__| • _4 Controls<br />

_ : _<br />

*5_ : •___|__• _5 Personality disorder<br />

T _ : _<br />

Y ________________________________________<br />

P 2_ : •|• _2 Substance abuse<br />

E _ : _<br />

3_ : •__|__• _3 Depression<br />

_ : _<br />

________________________________________<br />

0 1 2 3 *(p


the other groups since no confidence interval takes in 1(low) or 3(high).<br />

Structural variables<br />

Line: was understood as a defining border: outlines, lines used in pattern, dots, writing,<br />

broken line, straight ruled lines, and not blocks <strong>of</strong> colour intended for shading or close<br />

patterned pencil lines for shading which were treated as blocks. It was rated light/thin,<br />

varied (where thin lines appeared with thick), thick/heavy.<br />

Line quality was split into painted and drawn lines for ease <strong>of</strong> rating, since where<br />

pencil line coincided with painted line, the relative difference in thickness always<br />

polarised the pencil to thin and the painted to thick, thus creating ambiguities in the<br />

information. <strong>The</strong> scoring task asked for relative thickness <strong>of</strong> the lines within the picture.<br />

<strong>The</strong> literature tends towards expectations <strong>of</strong> thick or heavy lines for patients.<br />

Controls were expected to have more range, and substance abusers, speculatively, might<br />

suffer difficulties in control similar to people with brain injury, which are said to produce<br />

thin or shaky lines.<br />

10h. Confidence intervals and means by diagnostic type<br />

for variable PAINTED LINES<br />

_______________________________________<br />

_ : : _<br />

*2_ •__|_• :t :v t_2 Substance abuse<br />

_ :h :a h_<br />

T *3_ •____|___• :i :r i_3 Depression<br />

Y _ :n :i c_<br />

P *4_ •____|___• : :e k_4 Control<br />

E _ : :d _<br />

_______________________________________<br />

1_ •___|___• : : _1 Schizophrenic<br />

_ : : _<br />

5_ •________|________•: : _5 Personality Disorder<br />

_______________________________________<br />

.25 .75 1.25 1.75 *(p


Painted lines: <strong>The</strong> normal distribution was achieved by taking the square root<br />

transformation, the effect <strong>of</strong> this is to push the lower values, below one, up from zero<br />

towards 1, and compress 1-3, so the maximum score becomes 1.73 (thick lines covering<br />

the paper).<br />

1. No group's paintings were characterised by thick painted lines.<br />

2. Controls were not separable from other patient groups other than substance abusers.<br />

3. In line with expectations, substance abusers scored less than all other groups, but<br />

scores indicate lack <strong>of</strong> use <strong>of</strong> line rather than thin lines; they were not separable from<br />

schizophrenics. <strong>The</strong>re was no evidence to support the diagnosis <strong>of</strong> depression,<br />

schizophrenia, patient status or control status from thick or thin painted lines. Scores<br />

indicate lack <strong>of</strong> use rather than proliferation <strong>of</strong> thin lines.<br />

10i. Confidence intervals and means by diagnostic type<br />

for variable DRAWN LINES<br />

_______________________________________<br />

_ : : _<br />

*2_ •|• : _2 Substance abuse<br />

_ :t :v t _<br />

*4_ •| • :h :a h _4 Control<br />

T _ :i :r i _<br />

Y *3_ •| • :n :i c _3 Depression<br />

P _ : :e k _<br />

E 5_ •___|__• :d _5 Personality Disorder<br />

_______________________________________<br />

1_ • |• : : _1 Schizophrenic<br />

_ : : _<br />

_______________________________________<br />

0 2 4 6 8 *(p


Drawn line: <strong>The</strong> normal distribution was achieved by squaring the results, so as to push<br />

lower values which bunch just below one (thin lines), down the scale towards 0, and to<br />

expand the range from varied lines (4) to thick, the maximum score (9: thick lines covering<br />

the picture).<br />

1. No group's paintings were characterised by thick drawn lines.<br />

2. Substance abusers used significantly more lines than controls and depressives.<br />

3. Controls used less drawn lines than any group.<br />

4. Schizophrenics were not differentiable from controls, or from substance abusers, they<br />

either used thin or no lines, but their range, in line with expectations was similar to that<br />

<strong>of</strong> depressives.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was no evidence to support the diagnosis <strong>of</strong> depression, schizophrenia or patient<br />

status from thick drawn lines, which were rarely used even by controls. Patients with<br />

personality disorder showed the greatest variability in use <strong>of</strong> drawn line but were still<br />

predominantly in the thin range, however this result is probably compromised by small<br />

numbers. <strong>The</strong> division <strong>of</strong> drawn/painted line is supported because the groups changed<br />

relative positions, p<strong>art</strong>icularly controls and schizophrenics which reversed their<br />

orientation.<br />

10j. Confidence intervals and means by diagnostic type<br />

for variable SPACE<br />

______________________________________<br />

*4_ •|• _4 Controls<br />

_ _<br />

T *2_ •| • _2 Substance abuse<br />

Y _ _<br />

P *3_ • | • _3 Depression<br />

E _ _<br />

*5_ •|• _5 Personality disorder<br />

258


______________________________________<br />

1_ •|• _1 Schizophrenia<br />

_ _<br />

______________________________________<br />

0 1 2 3 4 5 *(p


proportions <strong>of</strong> empty space in the picture. Patient status may be indicated from<br />

consistent empty space in the picture, but not depression.<br />

10k. Confidence intervals and means by diagnostic type<br />

for variable SUBJECTIVE EMOTIONAL TONE<br />

_______________________________________<br />

*4_ • | • _4 Controls<br />

_ _<br />

*1_ •|• _1 Schizophrenia<br />

_ _<br />

*2_ • | • _2 Substance abuse<br />

T _ _<br />

Y *3_ •|• _3 Depression<br />

P _ _<br />

E *5_ •__|___• _5 Personality disorder<br />

_ _<br />

_______________________________________<br />

0 1 2 3 *(p


2. Schizophrenics and depressives were not differentiable from other patients.<br />

3. <strong>The</strong> lowest scores by patients (although not significantly lower) were from the<br />

pictures <strong>of</strong> substance abusers.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was no inclination towards negative for patient status but controls did score<br />

positively relative to patients. <strong>The</strong>re was no support for the diagnosis <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia<br />

or depression from high levels <strong>of</strong> negative emotional tone.<br />

10l. Confidence intervals and means by diagnostic type<br />

for variable DOMINANT FORM<br />

______________________________________<br />

*3_ •__|__• : _3 Depression<br />

_ : _<br />

T *4_ •___|__• : _4 Controls<br />

Y _ : _<br />

P *1_ •| • : _1 Schizophrenia<br />

E _ : _<br />

*2_ •__| • : _2 Substance abuse<br />

_ : _<br />

*5_ •____|____• : _5 Personality disorder<br />

_ : _<br />

______________________________________<br />

0 .25 .5 .75 1 *(p


1. Depressives showed significantly less dominant form in their pictures than any other<br />

group.<br />

2. As expected, the pictures <strong>of</strong> controls showed the most dominant form (significantly<br />

more than depressives, schizophrenics and personality disorder).<br />

3. Substance abusers' pictures were not significantly lower than controls (but they were<br />

significantly higher than depressed).<br />

4. Against expectations, schizophrenics showed significantly more dominant form in<br />

their pictures than depressives.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was no evidence to support the diagnosis <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia rather than any other<br />

patient group from lack <strong>of</strong> form, but depression may be indicated by such. Controls did<br />

score more than any patient group, but this may not be an indicator <strong>of</strong> health as such,<br />

because substance abusers overlap these scores.<br />

Summary <strong>of</strong> results by characteristics <strong>of</strong> typical pictures from all other groups:<br />

Schizophrenia: pictures showing very little black, <strong>of</strong> low intensity with some<br />

dominant form.<br />

Substance abuse: high levels <strong>of</strong> black, low green, drawn lines not painted, some<br />

dominant form.<br />

Depression: moderate levels <strong>of</strong> black (less than half the picture, but more than a<br />

qu<strong>art</strong>er), no dominant form.<br />

Personality disorder: less than half the picture black, low green, higher intensity,<br />

some dominant form.<br />

262


Controls: more positive emotional tone, and half or more <strong>of</strong> the picture contains<br />

the dominant form.<br />

Additional characteristics <strong>of</strong> controls: more red, yellow and green than other<br />

paintings, more intense colours, painted rather than drawn line and less empty<br />

space.<br />

Predictive value <strong>of</strong> the DAPA for patient status<br />

In order to find a combination <strong>of</strong> variables that classified a large proportion <strong>of</strong> subjects<br />

into the correct group so as to have good chance <strong>of</strong> allocating (diagnosing) new subjects<br />

correctly, the Discriminant Analysis technique was used to determine predictive value<br />

<strong>of</strong> the DAPA. A preliminary attempt between patient groups used the whole sample <strong>of</strong><br />

109 and yielded the percentage <strong>of</strong> grouped cases correctly classified as 81%. However,<br />

some caution was warranted in interpreting this figure because the classification functions<br />

were derived from the group <strong>of</strong> study; most authorities recommend reserving an<br />

independent data set on which to assess the fitness <strong>of</strong> the model 288 .<br />

A much sounder approach, at this stage was to divide the whole sample into two<br />

samples patients and non-patients and derive the classification functions using one <strong>of</strong> the<br />

samples and then, using these functions, to attempt to classify those subjects in the<br />

second sample as either controls or patients. Table 11 presents the results <strong>of</strong> the<br />

classification for the section which derives the functions and for the reserved section<br />

(which acts as the independent data set) repeated with five different p<strong>art</strong>itions. <strong>The</strong><br />

288<br />

D.G. Altman (1994), Practical Statistics for Medical Research , London, 5th Ed. original 1991:<br />

Chapman Hall, p.359.<br />

263


function contributed significantly to classifying the cases because the groups differed<br />

significantly. <strong>The</strong> Wilks lambda was typically around 0.4 which indicates around 60%<br />

discriminative power, which is not p<strong>art</strong>icularly high, but moderate. <strong>The</strong> derived Chi<br />

Square statistics were typically 34, which was highly significant (p=0.000). This model<br />

therefore had moderate discriminative power.<br />

No variables were correlated with the discriminant function higher than 0.6 and<br />

more typically 0.3-0.5, so there is no simple interpretation from one or two influencing<br />

factors, more a complex interaction <strong>of</strong> the contribution <strong>of</strong> different variables.<br />

<strong>The</strong> final p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the analysis was the classification <strong>of</strong> cases according to the<br />

derived function. SPSS predicts which group a subject belongs to by calculating which<br />

<strong>of</strong> the mean values <strong>of</strong> the discriminant function it is closest to. This classification<br />

appeared to be highly successful on the data set from which it derived the function, and<br />

overall was 86-97% correct. Using the derived function on an independent data set (half<br />

the sample) it proved 75-95% correct classification, which is reasonable considering the<br />

numbers.<br />

<strong>The</strong> next chapter will discuss the implications <strong>of</strong> these results, their relation to<br />

what is already known and augment them with exploratory and confirmatory subordinate<br />

analyses.<br />

264


TABLE 11: Discriminant analysis to classify controls or patients. Classification<br />

functions from the first sample are used to classify the second sample. Repeated 5 times<br />

with different p<strong>art</strong>itions <strong>of</strong> the data set. 86 PATIENTS 23 CONTROLS.<br />

Actual<br />

Group<br />

ill<br />

well<br />

ill<br />

well<br />

ill<br />

well<br />

ill<br />

well<br />

ill<br />

well<br />

Cases selected for use in the analysis Cases not selected for use in the analysis<br />

No. <strong>of</strong><br />

Cases<br />

42<br />

10<br />

40<br />

18<br />

44<br />

13<br />

38<br />

9<br />

42<br />

10<br />

Predicted Group Membership<br />

ill well<br />

40 2<br />

95% 5%<br />

2 8<br />

20% 80%<br />

correctly classified: 92%<br />

48 0<br />

100% 0%<br />

7 11<br />

39% 61%<br />

correctly classified: 89%<br />

43 1<br />

98% 2%<br />

5 8<br />

38% 62%<br />

correctly classified: 90%<br />

38 0<br />

100.0% 0%<br />

1 8<br />

11% 89%<br />

correctly classified: 98%<br />

40 2<br />

95% 5%<br />

5 5<br />

50% 50%<br />

correctly classified: 87%<br />

Comparison <strong>of</strong> effect with the previous literature<br />

265<br />

No. <strong>of</strong><br />

Cases<br />

44<br />

13<br />

38<br />

5<br />

42<br />

10<br />

48<br />

14<br />

44<br />

13<br />

Predicted Group Membership<br />

ill well<br />

43 1<br />

98% 2%<br />

3 10<br />

23% 77%<br />

correctly classified: 93%<br />

37 1<br />

97% 3%<br />

1 4<br />

20 80%<br />

correctly classified: 95%<br />

39 3<br />

93% 7%<br />

5 5<br />

50% 50%<br />

correctly classified: 85%<br />

39 9<br />

81% 19%<br />

6 8<br />

43% 57%<br />

correctly classified: 75%<br />

41 3<br />

93% 7%<br />

5 8<br />

38% 62%<br />

correctly classified: 86%<br />

<strong>The</strong> results <strong>of</strong> the t-test indicate that 6 variables: 4 structural and 2 colour, discriminate<br />

patients from controls at a highly significant level (p


the discriminant analysis. It will be noted that 2 variables have a negative t-value, which<br />

indicates the direction <strong>of</strong> the results (using a 1-tailed test, the differences lie on the other<br />

tail, therefore the tabled p-value was halved).<br />

Table 12: t-test results for significant differences between means <strong>of</strong> variables measured from<br />

paintings/pictures by patients against those <strong>of</strong> non patients as though they were independent.<br />

variable from<br />

DAPA<br />

Red<br />

Yellow<br />

Green<br />

Blue<br />

Brown<br />

Black<br />

Intensity<br />

Painted line<br />

Drawn line<br />

Space<br />

Em Tone<br />

Form<br />

Patients<br />

n=86<br />

mean SD<br />

0.4064 0.243<br />

0.3567 0.233<br />

0.2063 0.204<br />

0.2413 0.178<br />

0.2761 0.223<br />

0.4542 0.264<br />

1.9036 0.462<br />

0.4618 0.476<br />

1.1494 1.077<br />

2.0721 0.466<br />

2.0735 0.332<br />

0.3567 0.185<br />

non-patients<br />

n=23<br />

mean SD<br />

0.5945 0.262<br />

0.4895 0.286<br />

0.4018 0.267<br />

0.3307 0.259<br />

0.3944 0.270<br />

0.4767 0.320<br />

2.1725 0.508<br />

0.6929 0.528<br />

0.5667 0.787<br />

1.7304 0.430<br />

2.3846 0.363<br />

0.5332 0.246<br />

266<br />

F 2<br />

Value tail<br />

df=107 prob<br />

1.16 0.606<br />

1.51 0.185<br />

1.72 0.083<br />

2.13* 0.015<br />

1.46 0.219<br />

1.47 0.217<br />

1.21 0.526<br />

1.23 0.496<br />

1.87 0.096<br />

1.17 0.695<br />

1.20 0.543<br />

1.78 0.065<br />

Tvalue<br />

3.25<br />

2.31<br />

3.81<br />

1.56<br />

2.16<br />

0.35<br />

2.42<br />

2.02<br />

-2.42<br />

-3.17<br />

3.92<br />

3.77<br />

Sig.ce<br />

level<br />

0.002<br />

0.023<br />

0.000<br />

0.131*<br />

0.033<br />

0.729<br />

0.017<br />

0.046<br />

0.008+<br />

0.001+<br />

0.000<br />

0.000<br />

*Significant level <strong>of</strong> p from F-test, indicating non-homogeneous groups, therefore this variable used the separate variance T-test<br />

(df=27.8) rather than the pooled variance.<br />

+direction <strong>of</strong> sign indicates direction <strong>of</strong> results so the significance level is 2-tailed.<br />

Table 13a: the separate effect size for each variable from the DAPA test, based on the calculation <strong>of</strong> D =<br />

SQRT * 1/n1 + 1/n2 * t-value (patients/nonpatients).<br />

Variable D (Effect<br />

size)<br />

Red<br />

Yellow<br />

Green<br />

Blue<br />

Brown<br />

Black<br />

Intensity<br />

Paint<br />

Em-tone<br />

Form<br />

0.7629<br />

0.5423<br />

0.8944<br />

0.3662<br />

0.5071<br />

0.0822<br />

0.5681<br />

0.4742<br />

0.9202<br />

0.8850<br />

V Lower/Upper<br />

confidence limits<br />

.0578<br />

.0565<br />

.0588<br />

.0557<br />

.0563<br />

.0551<br />

.0566<br />

.0561<br />

.0590<br />

.0587<br />

0.5226 - 1.0033<br />

0.3047 - 0.7799<br />

0.6519 - 1.1368<br />

0.1302 - 0.6023<br />

0.2698 - 0.7443<br />

-0.1527 - 0.3170<br />

0.3302 - 0.8060<br />

0.2373 - 0.7111<br />

0.6773 - 1.1631<br />

0.6427 - 1.1273<br />

weight<br />

W=1/V<br />

17.3082<br />

17.7132<br />

17.0139<br />

17.9464<br />

17.7666<br />

18.1366<br />

17.6720<br />

17.8134<br />

16.9519<br />

17.0361<br />

W*ES = WD<br />

13.2059<br />

9.6053<br />

15.2170<br />

6.5721<br />

9.0086<br />

1.4901<br />

10.0383<br />

8.4469<br />

15.5993<br />

15.0769<br />

TOTAL 0.595 175.36 104.26<br />

sigma =0.08 Conf. interval (0.4465 - 0.7426) homogeneity stat 11.4075 (p>0.5 Chi-sq 8.34 df=9)<br />

Positive T-Values<br />

best estimation <strong>of</strong> sum(wd) = 104.26 = 0.595<br />

effect size sum(w) 175.36


Table 13b: the separate effect size for each variable from the DAPA test, based on the<br />

calculation <strong>of</strong> D = SQRT * 1/n1 + 1/n2 * t-value (patients/nonpatients).<br />

Variable Effect size<br />

D V<br />

Draw<br />

Space<br />

-0.5681<br />

-0.7441<br />

.0566<br />

.0576<br />

Lower/Upper<br />

confidence limits<br />

-0.8060 - -0.3302<br />

-0.9842 - -0.5041<br />

267<br />

weight<br />

W=1/V<br />

17.6720<br />

17.3472<br />

W*ES<br />

= WD<br />

-10.039<br />

-12.909<br />

TOTAL -0.656 35.02 -22.95<br />

sigma = 0.17; Conf. interval (-0.9865 - -0.3241); Homogeneity stat. 0.2714 (p


Subordinate Analyses<br />

Interaction effects<br />

Sex: <strong>The</strong> demographics showed more females than males, especially in the control group.<br />

Two variables, Emotional Tone and Yellow were found to be influenced by Sex. When<br />

co-varied (2 way ANOVA; yellow - F=0.10, df=4, P=0.98; emotional tone - F=1.14,<br />

df=4, P=0.34), this was found to be a function <strong>of</strong> the combination <strong>of</strong> the main elements,<br />

and neither variable was significant by itself.<br />

Painting Combination: Not all the subjects completed all the paintings. This could<br />

produce bias, as content in the early paintings is therefore emphasised. A preliminary<br />

Kruskal-Wallis ANOVA performed on the groups and the number <strong>of</strong> paintings each<br />

patient completed seemed to indicate a covariate influence (_ 2 17.30, df=4, p=0.0017)<br />

from the raw (i.e. untransformed scores). <strong>The</strong> number <strong>of</strong> paintings from each patient was<br />

collapsed into three ordered categories (1 or 2 paintings; 3-5 paintings or 6+), and two-<br />

way ANOVAs (No. <strong>of</strong> paintings by diagnostic type) were performed on the final scores<br />

for each variable to determine interaction effects for the number <strong>of</strong> paintings on the score<br />

for any variable remaining in the analysis.<br />

Table 14 shows that interaction effects were not significant and variance was<br />

mostly explained by differences in diagnostic grouping. Additionally, table 15 shows that<br />

only one variable from 13 was correlated with the number <strong>of</strong> paintings done (p=0.01, for<br />

the Substance abuse group). We can safely ignore one significant score from 65 on the<br />

268


asis that multiple tests produce the odd significant figure. <strong>The</strong>refore the number <strong>of</strong><br />

paintings done did not affect the average score.<br />

Table 14: Results <strong>of</strong> 2 way ANOVA - diagnostic groups by No. <strong>of</strong> paintings from each<br />

patient to show the influence <strong>of</strong> number <strong>of</strong> pictures on differences between groups was<br />

insignificant.<br />

Variable Source <strong>of</strong> Variation<br />

(Diagnostic Type)<br />

df=4<br />

F value and sig.(p)<br />

Red<br />

Yellow<br />

Orange<br />

Green<br />

Blue<br />

Brown<br />

Black<br />

Intensity<br />

Line-Draw<br />

Area<br />

Em-Tone<br />

Form<br />

4.913 (p=.001)<br />

2.521 (p=.046)<br />

.986 (p=.419)<br />

3.897 (p=.005)<br />

1.897 (p=.469)<br />

2.604 (p=.040)<br />

9.469 (p=.000)<br />

4.765 (p=.001)<br />

3.757 (p=.007)<br />

2.731 (p=.033)<br />

5.052 (p=.001)<br />

7.128 (p=.000)<br />

Covariate<br />

(No. <strong>of</strong> Pictures)<br />

df=1<br />

F value and sig. (p)<br />

0.131 (p=.718)<br />

3.473 (p=.493)<br />

0.369 (p=.545)<br />

0.272 (p=.262)<br />

0.404 (p=.527)<br />

1.547 (p=.216)<br />

2.507 (p=.116)<br />

1.364 (p=.246)<br />

0.291 (p=.591)<br />

1.275 (p=.262)<br />

1.339 (p=.250)<br />

0.352 (p=.554)<br />

269<br />

Explained<br />

df=5<br />

F. Value and sig.(p)<br />

0.957 (p=.003)<br />

2.112 (p=.070)<br />

0.862 (p=.509)<br />

3.372 (p=.007)<br />

0.798 (p=.554)<br />

2.392 (p=.043)<br />

8.077 (p=.000)<br />

4.085 (p=.002)<br />

3.064 (p=.013)<br />

2.439 (p=.039)<br />

4.309 (p=.001)<br />

5.773 (p=.000)


Table 15: showing correlations between No. <strong>of</strong> pictures within each group with each<br />

variable.<br />

Variables Schizophrenia<br />

Red<br />

Yellow<br />

Orange<br />

Green<br />

Blue<br />

Brown<br />

Black<br />

Intensity<br />

Line-Draw<br />

Area<br />

Em-Tone<br />

Form<br />

n=35<br />

.0535<br />

.1611<br />

.0789<br />

.0282<br />

.1560<br />

.1376<br />

.3339<br />

.3276<br />

.1493<br />

.2396<br />

.1938<br />

.2216<br />

Substance<br />

Abuse<br />

n=24<br />

.3558<br />

-.1658<br />

.1916<br />

.1450<br />

-.1978<br />

.2329<br />

.1735<br />

.0852<br />

.0451<br />

-.3166<br />

-.2097<br />

.3340<br />

270<br />

Affective<br />

Disorder<br />

n=18<br />

.4304<br />

-.1925<br />

-.0063<br />

.1010<br />

.5046<br />

.5935*<br />

-.0318<br />

.0870<br />

-.1239<br />

.0188<br />

.2522<br />

.4596<br />

Descriptive analysis: associations between variables<br />

Controls<br />

n=23<br />

.2143<br />

.2584<br />

.3820<br />

.0561<br />

-.1138<br />

.3874<br />

.3458<br />

.2964<br />

.0302<br />

-.0313<br />

.1694<br />

-.1506<br />

Personality<br />

Disorder<br />

n=9<br />

-.3182<br />

-.2029<br />

-.3976<br />

-.6751<br />

-.5477<br />

-.6028<br />

.3055<br />

.2777<br />

-.5823<br />

.2632<br />

-.4022<br />

.1234<br />

Correlations within groups: Bi-variate scatter plots for the significant variables showed<br />

a reasonably elliptical pattern indicating a normal distribution <strong>of</strong> the data so the validity<br />

<strong>of</strong> the associated hypotheses tests were not violated. <strong>The</strong> non-significant scatter plots<br />

were checked for unusual patterns, which the Pearson's r is likely to underestimate. <strong>The</strong><br />

purpose <strong>of</strong> this analysis was to describe the association <strong>of</strong> the variables from the DAPA<br />

test to provide information for the discussion <strong>of</strong> differences in the results <strong>of</strong> the DAPA<br />

test to those <strong>of</strong> the literature and general expectations. Correlation tables for each<br />

diagnostic group 16-20 measuring (a) colour and (b) structure can be found at the end <strong>of</strong><br />

appendix 2. Summary information is presented here.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were most associations between variables for schizophrenics' paintings, especially<br />

for colour:<br />

1. All colours were correlated with intensity except black and brown.<br />

2. Green was correlated with all colours except black.


3. Painted line correlated with red and green.<br />

4. Yellow with brown and blue.<br />

Intensity showed a relationship to bright colour and green appeared most consistently,<br />

whereas only black was independent. <strong>The</strong> structural variables were mostly independent.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were least associations between variables for paintings by substance abusers:<br />

1. Only green and brown were correlated from colours.<br />

2. Blue showed a strong negative relationship with form.<br />

3. <strong>The</strong>re were no associations <strong>of</strong> colour with intensity.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were associations between variables for paintings by depressed patients:<br />

1. Green showed a moderate correlation with black.<br />

2. Green and yellow were moderately correlated with intensity.<br />

3. Painted line negatively correlated with drawn line.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were associations between variables for the paintings <strong>of</strong> controls:<br />

1. Red and yellow were moderately correlated.<br />

2. Intensity showed a correlation with red.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were associations between variables for the paintings by patients with personality<br />

disorder:<br />

1. <strong>The</strong>re were no correlations between colours.<br />

271


2. Intensity showed a correlation only with red.<br />

3. Drawn line was negatively correlated with painted line.<br />

272


Chapter 6: Discussion<br />

<strong>The</strong> question Is there sensitive psychopathological evidence presented in the <strong>art</strong>work <strong>of</strong><br />

mental illness? has been answered. <strong>The</strong> bare results tell us that pictures by people with<br />

psychiatric disorder contain some characteristic structural and content differences from<br />

those <strong>of</strong> normal controls and furthermore, some <strong>of</strong> these characteristic differences are<br />

specific to certain psychiatric disorders. <strong>The</strong> DAPA test has proved effective in that it<br />

has been sensitive enough to detect differences relating to diagnostic grouping. But this<br />

question has been answered before, and also proved positive if weak 291 .<br />

Generally the DAPA test results agree with the other major contemporary<br />

studies in the field, which is to say that they agree with the general direction <strong>of</strong><br />

assumptions <strong>of</strong> the literature. However, a close look at the areas <strong>of</strong> differentiation, each<br />

<strong>of</strong> which are discussed below, gives very different discrimination factors. <strong>The</strong>refore<br />

another question presents itself How closely do the answers from the DAPA test resemble<br />

what is generally 'known' from the rest <strong>of</strong> the field and if they do not what reasons have<br />

we to think that the interpretation <strong>of</strong> the DAPA test answers are more valid than this<br />

accumulated experience?. <strong>The</strong> answer to some <strong>of</strong> this question must be sought in what<br />

is reported in Chapter 2. <strong>The</strong> DAPA uses formal measurements <strong>of</strong> structure whereas the<br />

other tests were predominantly biased to content, in line with the clinical use <strong>of</strong> such<br />

measures in the assessment <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy. <strong>The</strong> other tests are remarkably variable in the<br />

291<br />

See B. Cohen, J.S. Hammer and S. Singer (1988), <strong>The</strong> Diagnostic Drawing Series: a systematic<br />

approach to <strong>art</strong> therapy evaluation and research, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.15(1):11-21, and my discussion<br />

<strong>of</strong> their results, Chapter 2.<br />

273


quality and reliability <strong>of</strong> their measurement, in the definition <strong>of</strong> their criteria, and their<br />

results are <strong>of</strong>ten uninterpretable because <strong>of</strong> flawed methodology.<br />

<strong>The</strong> DAPA, however, cannot simply claim a higher status if the test establishes<br />

only the same general results but leaves questions relating to differences with what is<br />

generally accepted from the accumulated experience <strong>of</strong> clinical pr<strong>of</strong>essionals and other<br />

major statistical studies open. <strong>The</strong> answer must be accompanied by the answer to these<br />

subsidiary questions, one <strong>of</strong> which must be Why should we use the DAPA rather than<br />

other tests?<br />

If the DAPA results show a great divergence from established opinion, that<br />

difference needs to be explained. <strong>The</strong> discussion explains the results from the DAPA and<br />

their relation to the literature in 4 p<strong>art</strong>s:<br />

1. <strong>The</strong> discussion <strong>of</strong> the results tries to relate the findings <strong>of</strong> group differences<br />

identified by the ANOVA and the complementary corrected t-tests (Duncan<br />

procedure) to what are generally accepted as psychiatric characteristics from the<br />

literature. It explains differences and correspondences with that literature and<br />

speculates on how these elements are interpreted. Interrelations between the<br />

DAPA variables, identified by the subordinate analyses from the Results section,<br />

are discussed to determine associations which might affect the results and<br />

possibly explain some <strong>of</strong> the deviations from established knowledge;<br />

2. <strong>The</strong> findings <strong>of</strong> <strong>Study</strong> 1 (Chapter 4) are related to those <strong>of</strong> <strong>Study</strong> 2, exploring and<br />

explaining the differences to find common correspondences;<br />

274


3. <strong>The</strong> comparison <strong>of</strong> effect sizes with those <strong>of</strong> the literature review, from the<br />

Results section, is discussed; and<br />

4. <strong>The</strong> predictive value and derivations <strong>of</strong> the functions from the discriminant<br />

analysis.<br />

1. Comparison <strong>of</strong> the results with the predictions<br />

Note on comparison with the literature: Generally, the style and format <strong>of</strong> the<br />

literature reviewed in Chapter 2 <strong>of</strong> this thesis used content appraisal with selective<br />

illustration <strong>of</strong> typical examples. Few studies measured any formal qualities, and these<br />

were usually interpreted through a theoretical framework. Embedded in content appraisal<br />

were assumptions that stylistic or formal features <strong>of</strong> the paintings <strong>of</strong> the patients were<br />

reflective <strong>of</strong> mental disorder. <strong>The</strong>se associations were not overt, and <strong>of</strong>ten described as<br />

global or 'intuitive' assessment, and combining personal and objective dimensions. Often,<br />

pictures were reproduced instead <strong>of</strong> explanations as 'obvious' examples <strong>of</strong> thought<br />

disorder, or as illustration <strong>of</strong> the process <strong>of</strong> recovery, in which their descriptions were not<br />

explicit (Where explanations occurred, they depended on the interaction <strong>of</strong> two or more<br />

formal elements out <strong>of</strong> context 292 ). Colour associations were rarely mentioned unless<br />

important to the general message, were usually supported by the reported comments <strong>of</strong><br />

the patient about the work and differed considerably between studies. Furthermore most<br />

authors advise that colour associations with content are individual. <strong>The</strong>refore comparison<br />

<strong>of</strong> the DAPA results with characteristics <strong>of</strong> underlying assumptions <strong>of</strong> the field, as well<br />

292 Such as 'inappropriate colour', i.e. green faces, or structural anomalies, i.e. falling ap<strong>art</strong> trees.<br />

275


as being unreliable, would be methodologically difficult to justify. References to<br />

interpretations <strong>of</strong> elements in single studies as typical examples would take comments<br />

out <strong>of</strong> context and intention. Most 'tests' relate to protocols for <strong>art</strong> therapy practice, for<br />

which the DAPA is not designed. <strong>The</strong> DDS approach to assessment cannot be compared<br />

because they have produced no interpretable results so far, although their protocol is<br />

widely used for <strong>art</strong> therapy assessment, the assessment process remains with the<br />

experience and interpretations <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong> therapist, not the 'test'. <strong>The</strong>refore I have<br />

concentrated on the theoretical influences <strong>of</strong> assumptions and 'intuitions' <strong>of</strong> all these<br />

approaches in my comments on comparison <strong>of</strong> the DAPA scores with the literature or<br />

to studies which use explicit descriptions or statistical evidence, using two <strong>of</strong> the most<br />

influential and well known authors to disseminate research results on distinguishing<br />

features <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> to contemporary general and research literature, Wadeson and Amos 293 .<br />

Both present easily digestible tabulations <strong>of</strong> common structural and content<br />

characterisations and are regularly cited in recent literature.<br />

Organisation <strong>of</strong> this discussion<br />

<strong>The</strong> purpose <strong>of</strong> this discussion is to explore and discuss possible explanations for<br />

differences in the results <strong>of</strong> the DAPA test to what has previously been found. <strong>The</strong><br />

secondary purpose, in further development <strong>of</strong> the test, was to determine any reduction<br />

<strong>of</strong> the scales to increase efficiency if some <strong>of</strong> the scales measured the same quantities.<br />

293<br />

H. Wadeson (1980) Art Psychotherapy,<br />

New York: Wiley; (1987), <strong>The</strong> Dynamics <strong>of</strong> Art Psychotherapy ,<br />

New York: Wiley; (1992) (ed.) A Guide to Conducting Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Research , Mundelein, ILL: Am. Art<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy Assn; and Stephen Amos, <strong>The</strong> Diagnostic, Prognostic, and <strong>The</strong>rapeutic Implications <strong>of</strong><br />

Schizophrenic Art, Arts in Psychotherapy 1982, V.9:131-43.<br />

276


<strong>The</strong> scales are explained in two sections; (a) colour and (b) structure. Section (a) includes<br />

colour intensity because it adds to the conclusions for colour rather than for structure.<br />

Summary Table <strong>of</strong> Results: Table 1<br />

Red controls> substance abuse<br />

Yellow controls> schizophrenics<br />

Green controls> all patients except depressives<br />

Black substance abusers> depressed/controls /pers'y disorder> schizophrenics<br />

Intensity controls/ personality disorder> schizophrenics<br />

Painted Line depressives /control>substance abuse<br />

Drawn Line substance abusers> control or depressives; personality disorders> control<br />

Empty Space control schizophrenics /personality disorder> depressed<br />

Em. Tone control> all patients<br />

<strong>The</strong> information generated by the DAPA naturally divides between two obvious<br />

points: controls used more and brighter colours (except black), more line, form, space,<br />

were more positive than other groups; and schizophrenics or substance abusers scored<br />

least. Information from the correlation matrices between each group help to confirm,<br />

explain or elaborate on earlier points from other data. To limit 'data dredging', the most<br />

obvious hypotheses tests were the predictions from the discussion <strong>of</strong> the literature. <strong>The</strong><br />

expected associations were:<br />

Between bright colours and between dark colours reflecting their use together and<br />

a negative correlation between bright and dark colours generally reflecting the<br />

opposing emotional tone <strong>of</strong> the picture.<br />

Bright hues would generally correlate with intensity.<br />

277


<strong>The</strong>re would be a correlation between all or most <strong>of</strong>: emotional tone, hue,<br />

intensity and form reflecting increased aesthetic harmony with greater structure<br />

and positivity <strong>of</strong> the picture.<br />

Between blue and brown reflecting their neutral status.<br />

Painted line would negatively correlate with drawn line because patients used little<br />

painted and more drawn line.<br />

Differences between groups - Colour<br />

<strong>The</strong> focus is on the most important findings: Black differentiated substance abusers from<br />

all other groups and schizophrenia from 3 other groups; green differentiated controls from<br />

3 other diagnostic groups. Additionally, there were differences in colour associations<br />

between diagnostic groups; Figure 3 summarises these relations. Art therapy research<br />

has provided support for the assumption <strong>of</strong> a consistent relationship between colour and<br />

emotion 294 , so the work <strong>of</strong> people with psychiatric disorders, which are <strong>of</strong>ten<br />

characterised by mood disorder should differ from normal controls. It has been suggested<br />

that colour usage is related to the adequacy <strong>of</strong> individual resources for integrating affective<br />

experience 295 , a facility rarely developed in schizophrenics, so within group differences<br />

should be expected. Presented here is a summary (table 2) by Amos, <strong>of</strong> the work <strong>of</strong><br />

Tarmo Pasto, 1968 296 , empirically developed from experience with the use <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> in<br />

294<br />

Bernard I. Levy (1984), Research into the psychological meaning <strong>of</strong> colour, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.23,<br />

(reprinted from V.19, July 1980, pp.87-91).<br />

295<br />

D. Rapaport, M. Gill and R. Schafer (1946), Diagnostic Psychological Testing , Chicago: Year Book<br />

Publishers.<br />

296<br />

Tarmo Pasto (1968), <strong>The</strong> bio-mythology <strong>of</strong> colour: a theory, in I. Jakob, ed., Psychiatry and <strong>art</strong>: Art<br />

278


diagnosis and therapy with psychiatric groups, which probably represents the common<br />

knowledge <strong>of</strong> the field. Amos gives the qualification that hypotheses are <strong>of</strong> little<br />

consequence without corroboration from other factors so there is very little emphasis on<br />

this speculation where differences are not obvious.<br />

Table 2: Interpretations <strong>of</strong> the use <strong>of</strong> colour in <strong>art</strong>istic productions: from S.P. Amos.<br />

Colour Interpretation<br />

Red<br />

Yellow<br />

Orange<br />

Blue<br />

Green<br />

Violetred<br />

Purple<br />

Black<br />

White<br />

Self, ego development, active relation to reality, physicalemotional<br />

balance.<br />

Inner emotionalism, frustration, threat to ego, identity with violent<br />

inner forces.<br />

Aggressive resistance to dependency.<br />

Alo<strong>of</strong>ness, lack <strong>of</strong> emotionality, spirituality, overtly controlled,<br />

emotional blocking.<br />

Suffering, resisting emotion, positive, dogmatic, insecure.<br />

<strong>The</strong> psycho-sexual self, lack <strong>of</strong> proper sexual identification,<br />

troubled by sexual drives, emotionalism tied to sexual<br />

frustrations.<br />

Passivity, uninvolved, tendency to depression, may represent<br />

paranoid tendencies.<br />

Absence <strong>of</strong> extrinsic emotion, hostility, bound, denying,<br />

aggressive, fearful.<br />

Negation <strong>of</strong> both physical and emotional being, passivity,<br />

receptiveness, spiritualness.<br />

<strong>of</strong> interpretation and <strong>art</strong> therapy (Vol.2) New York: Karger, tabulated by in S.P. Amos, (1982) op.cit.<br />

279


Figures 3. Map <strong>of</strong> Associations between colour variables measured between diagnostic<br />

groups from the results <strong>of</strong> the DAPA test.<br />

_____________________________________________________________________<br />

a. Schizophrenics b. Substance abusers<br />

c. Depressives d. Personality Disorder<br />

red<br />

red<br />

brown gree black<br />

yello blue<br />

brown gree black<br />

yello blue<br />

e. Controls<br />

red<br />

brown gree black<br />

yello blue<br />

280<br />

red<br />

red<br />

brown gree black<br />

yello blue<br />

brown gree black<br />

yello blue


Figure 3 shows the results <strong>of</strong> the DAPA correlations as a map <strong>of</strong> associations between<br />

colour variables. It is immediately apparent that each group differs within colour<br />

associations, which informs the discussion <strong>of</strong> differences identified by the ANOVA<br />

procedure. Controls used more <strong>of</strong> all colours than the patients, except black, in line with<br />

expectations, they used lots <strong>of</strong> red and yellow, bright colours together (plot 3e), and<br />

moderate dark colours, so their pictures probably looked brighter and fuller than the<br />

patients. Depressives were nearest to controls, but there were associations between<br />

green and black, dark colours, although these were little used. Personality disorder were<br />

next; substance abusers and schizophrenics showed least use <strong>of</strong> colour. This finding<br />

supports the general consensus which reports 'impoverishment' <strong>of</strong> work by<br />

schizophrenics 297 but does not support more frequent reports for work by depressives 298<br />

or by patients generally 299 .<br />

297<br />

Examples <strong>of</strong> later studies, see M. Miljkovitch, M. Irvine, (1982) Comparisons <strong>of</strong> drawing performances<br />

<strong>of</strong> schizophrenics, other psychiatric patients and normal schoolchildren on a Draw-A -Village task, Arts<br />

in Psychotherapy , V.9:203-16 differentiated schizophrenics from other patients; M.B. Morris (1995), <strong>The</strong><br />

DDS and the Tree Rating Scale: an isomorphic representation <strong>of</strong> Multiple Personality Disorder, Manic<br />

Depressive and Schizophrenic populations, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.12(2):118-128; Wadeson (1976, cited 1980,<br />

op.cit.) found both complete and impoverished pictures in schizophrenia and noted impoverishment was<br />

traditionally associated with psychotic depression, p.193; Amos (1982) op.cit. states that many <strong>of</strong> the<br />

properties <strong>of</strong> Manic Depressives found by H. Wadeson and W.E. Bunney (1970), Manic Depressive Art:<br />

a systematic study <strong>of</strong> differences in a 48 hour cyclic patient, J. Nervous and Mental Disease , V.150:215-<br />

31 are also present in schizophrenia and "reflect dimensions important to diagnosis, prognosis and/or<br />

treatment", p.141.<br />

298<br />

See H. Wadeson (1975), Suicide: expression in images, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.14:75-82; (1980)<br />

op.cit., characteristics <strong>of</strong> unipolar depression by Wadeson, also tabulation <strong>of</strong> traditional literature, p.190<br />

reprinted from Wadeson and Carpenter (1976), A comparative study <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong> experience <strong>of</strong> schizophrenic,<br />

unipolar depressive and bipolar non-depressed patients, J. Nervous Mental Disease , V.162(2):334-44; Later<br />

example: H.Wright and M.P. McIntyre (1982), <strong>The</strong> Family Drawing Depression Scale, J. Clinical<br />

Psychology , V.38(4):853-61; and Amos (1982) op.cit., endorsed and reprinted Wadeson & Bunney's<br />

(1970) Mania/Depression table, low colour, closed forms, hopeless, empty.<br />

299<br />

S. Russell-Lacy et al. (1979) An experimental study <strong>of</strong> pictures produced by schizophrenic subjects, B.<br />

J. Psychiatry , V.134:195-200 found that schizophrenics and other patients performed equally poorly when<br />

compared with normals; C. Bergland & R.M. Gonzalez (1993), Art and madness: can the interface be<br />

quantified, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.31:81-90, compared patients with personality disorder with normals<br />

although they also found an effect for cross classified depression.<br />

281


Schizophrenic pictures showed correlations between consistent and associated<br />

moderate use <strong>of</strong> red and low yellow, green, blue and brown indicating multiple use <strong>of</strong><br />

colours, but from the results <strong>of</strong> the ANOVA, we can see that they used little <strong>of</strong> any<br />

colour but red, so their pictures probably looked more monochromatic and bare.<br />

Substance abusers tended to use only small amounts <strong>of</strong> muddy colours together (green<br />

and brown), with a little bright colour (red or yellow) and lots <strong>of</strong> black, so their pictures<br />

probably seemed dark and dramatic with sharp contrasts. Depressives used a little green<br />

together with moderate black and bright colours so their pictures probably looked fairly<br />

balanced. Colours used by patients with personality disorder showed balance in colour<br />

but no associations; they used bright and dark colours moderately but little green and<br />

mostly were not differentiable from the other groups. Blue and brown were used in<br />

equally low quantities by all groups, but proportionally they would tend to add more to<br />

the darker colours in the paintings <strong>of</strong> schizophrenics and substance abusers. <strong>The</strong>re were<br />

three obvious and important obvious discussion points in variations <strong>of</strong> use <strong>of</strong> colour<br />

within groups in this study: (1) the use <strong>of</strong> green and (2) red in controls and schizophrenia<br />

and the use <strong>of</strong> (3) black in all groups, but especially substance abusers and<br />

schizophrenics. (4) Minor differences are discussed after.<br />

Use <strong>of</strong> Green: Green was important in schizophrenic paintings. Green appeared with<br />

all colours but black, especially yellow and multiple colours 300 . <strong>The</strong>se findings are mildly<br />

supported by impressionistic reports <strong>of</strong> a preference for, or inappropriate use <strong>of</strong>, green<br />

300 Yellow was highly correlated with green and moderately with blue and brown, but not red. So given<br />

that green was used most with yellow, and yellow turned up with blue and brown, multiple rather than<br />

singular use <strong>of</strong> colours are indicated.<br />

282


from the literature 301 . However, green was more apparent only relatively in<br />

schizophrenics' pictures, because only a little more green was used than in other patient<br />

groups; both schizophrenics and substance abusers used significantly less than controls,<br />

who used twice as much. Depressives covered a wider range, but still used much less<br />

green than controls. Judging from the placement <strong>of</strong> the groups, the interpretation <strong>of</strong><br />

suffering for green seems unlikely.<br />

<strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> red: Previous associations and preferences in patients have been found for<br />

red and black 302 especially for anger. In this study, controls used much more red than<br />

patients (although they were differentiated only from drug abusers, who used least). <strong>The</strong><br />

confidence intervals for the use <strong>of</strong> red by depressives and patients with personality<br />

disorder were wide (Figure 11a, results) so use was quite varied in these groups although<br />

mean values show moderate amounts <strong>of</strong> red as in schizophrenia where the use was more<br />

consistent.<br />

<strong>The</strong> balanced use <strong>of</strong> bright and dark colours by depressives indicates that<br />

diagnostic impressions <strong>of</strong> dark, sombre colours 303 in clinical depression are unjustified.<br />

301<br />

Roberta H. Shoemaker (1978), <strong>The</strong> significance <strong>of</strong> the first picture in <strong>art</strong> therapy, Proceedings <strong>of</strong> the 8th<br />

Annual Conference <strong>of</strong> the Am. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Ass. , (p.156-62) provides a good example <strong>of</strong> green used in<br />

a bizarre way, especially for people. She describes the unnatural colouring <strong>of</strong> relatives faces in green as<br />

p<strong>art</strong>icularly significant.<br />

302<br />

Frances F. Kaplan, Previous reports <strong>of</strong> preferences for red and black among patient groups (R. Langevin,<br />

M. Raine, D. Day, K. Waxer (1975), Art, intelligence and formal features in psychotics' paintings, Arts<br />

in Psychotherapy , V.2(2):149-158) may be explained through recent studies <strong>of</strong> representations <strong>of</strong> anger<br />

(Frances F. Kaplan (1994), <strong>The</strong> imagery and expression <strong>of</strong> anger: an initial study, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.11:139-<br />

143). Kaplan found 52% <strong>of</strong> the 'angry' paintings <strong>of</strong> students presenting this combination and 33% more<br />

only black or hot colours; Further research in 1996 (Positive images <strong>of</strong> anger in an anger management<br />

workshop, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.23(1):69-75) reported that deliberately modifying these images<br />

showed marked decrease in these colours and increase in blues and greens and 30% eliminated black and<br />

red. Kaplan's qualification that colours were probably related to current mood state rather than propensity<br />

for anger may indicate a lack <strong>of</strong> affect in depressives.<br />

303 Wadeson (1980), op.cit. p.190 and Amos (1982) op.cit. p.140 summarises the majority <strong>of</strong> the literature<br />

on the subject; for examples <strong>of</strong> these principles see D. Arrington (1991), Thinking systems- seeing<br />

283


<strong>The</strong> DAPA results for depression are consistent with some studies showing brighter<br />

colours used by depressives in treatment 304 .<br />

Red was used by schizophrenics more than any other colour and thus could have<br />

been clinically noticeable, supporting preferences for red in this group, but not for black,<br />

which confidence interval was much shorter than the other groups and lower down the<br />

scale (Figure 10f, results).<br />

Use <strong>of</strong> red and black: Of the other groups, substance abusers alone consistently used<br />

large amounts <strong>of</strong> black, but depressives, controls and personality disorder all used<br />

moderate amounts with a much wider variability.<br />

Generally then, there was no evidence <strong>of</strong> general patient preference for red, or<br />

indeed black (except for substance abuse), but there were within-group differences. This<br />

finding confirms the value <strong>of</strong> within-group comparisons, since findings for the whole<br />

population would combine these quite different characteristics. Additionally, the<br />

interpretation for red in table 2 is consistent with a more positive control position and<br />

with the conclusions reached by those studies which found brighter colours in depressive<br />

presentations.<br />

systems: an integrative model for systemically oriented <strong>art</strong> therapy, <strong>The</strong> Arts in Psychotherapy , V.18:201-<br />

211; M.D. Cagnoletta (1983), Artwork as a representation <strong>of</strong> object relation in the practice, Pratt Instit ute<br />

Creative Arts Review , V.4:46-52; D. Heine and M. Steiner (1986), Standardised paintings as a proposed<br />

adjunct instrument for monitoring mood states: a preliminary note, Occupational <strong>The</strong>rapy in Mental<br />

Health , V.6(3):21-7, using modified Wadeson's table <strong>of</strong> characteristics.<br />

304<br />

Shoemaker (1978) op.cit. describes bright warm colours as the depressive patient touches her emotional<br />

intensity in the process <strong>of</strong> treatment; S. Buchalter Katz (1985), Observations concerning the <strong>art</strong><br />

productions <strong>of</strong> depressed patients in short term psychiatric facilities, Arts in Psychotherapy,<br />

V.12(1):35-38<br />

describes bright colours and happy scenes reflecting hope.<br />

284


Use <strong>of</strong> Black: All groups except schizophrenics used more black than other colours.<br />

Amongst the range <strong>of</strong> colours used by schizophrenics, black was not unusually low; the<br />

variation is because schizophrenics used low levels <strong>of</strong> all colours but red.<br />

Black wasn't correlated with any other dark colour, except in depressives, which<br />

group showed a significant positive association between green and black. This linear<br />

relationship occurs in no other group (plot 3c); other results show weak negative<br />

correlations, which confirm that black was used rather more separately than any other<br />

colour in other groups. <strong>The</strong> position <strong>of</strong> the depressives, as indistinguishable from<br />

controls and below the much greater use by substance abusers, does not support the<br />

commonly held belief that large values <strong>of</strong> black indicate depression 305 , since both<br />

schizophrenics 306 and depressives should then exhibit more <strong>of</strong> the characteristic than<br />

other groups, and controls should exhibit less.<br />

<strong>The</strong> DAPA results for black support the empirical conclusions <strong>of</strong> Kaplan 307 , that<br />

black denotes strong affect in concurrent expression <strong>of</strong> anger; together with the<br />

interpretation, from table 2 above, <strong>of</strong> aggression, fear or hostility, which includes the<br />

absence <strong>of</strong> extrinsic emotion (which subjectively, may well characterise the drug<br />

305<br />

<strong>The</strong>re is no doubt that angry, depressed and emotionally traumatised people do use large amounts <strong>of</strong><br />

black paint in their expressive work, a recent description <strong>of</strong> paintings by Soviet post-e<strong>art</strong>hquake<br />

traumatised children clearly contrasts their obsessive use <strong>of</strong> white, red and especially black with their<br />

previous normal work and their gradual return to full spectrum (V.S. Gregorian, A. Azarian, M.B.<br />

DeMaria and L.D. McDonald (1996), Colors <strong>of</strong> disaster: the psychology <strong>of</strong> the "black sun", Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , V.23(1): 1-14). However, Kaplan, and others providing hard data (including the above<br />

study) used normal subjects.<br />

306 Wadeson (1980), op.cit. reports her 1976 study <strong>of</strong> schizophrenics (Wadeson & Carpenter, op.cit.) 52<br />

out <strong>of</strong> 56 patients drew designations <strong>of</strong> depressed feelings, describing typical black self-images.<br />

307 Kaplan, 1994, 1996 op.cit. characteristics <strong>of</strong> anger.<br />

285


abusers 308 , but not the schizophrenics). Thus the DAPA results agree with other<br />

empirical studies.<br />

<strong>The</strong> measurement procedures <strong>of</strong> the DAPA may have contributed to the<br />

polarisation <strong>of</strong> scores between schizophrenia and substance abuse. <strong>The</strong>re could be large<br />

variations in quantity applied to an area and quality <strong>of</strong> tone, for example, a range from<br />

pure black, rated as high intensity, and pencil lines, rated at low intensity. Large amounts<br />

<strong>of</strong> pencil line would be rated at a very consistent intensity <strong>of</strong> black, which would make<br />

it inconsistent with the fluctuations <strong>of</strong> the other colours. This explanation does fit with<br />

the low negative correlations for black throughout. Schizophrenics are said to produce<br />

fragmented and dissolute compositions 309 , indicating lack <strong>of</strong> or less certain outlines with<br />

a corresponding low score and drug abusers could similarly have produced more pencil<br />

lines. If this was the case then black scores should correlate with form or with drawn line<br />

for schizophrenics and substance abusers; this hypothesis is examined in the discussion<br />

<strong>of</strong> structure.<br />

Minor differences:<br />

yellow: All groups used moderate yellow except schizophrenics, but as before,<br />

schizophrenics used little colour. This finding contradicts previous findings <strong>of</strong> increased<br />

yellow in paintings by schizophrenics 310 , but this could easily be explained by<br />

308 Communicated to the author in discussion with group and hospital staff post experimental period.<br />

309<br />

Wadeson (1980), op.cit. table <strong>of</strong> characteristics <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia, p.190; Amos (1982), op.cit. provides<br />

typical examples.<br />

310 Russell-Lacy et al. (1979) op.cit. found schizophrenics and normal controls used more yellow and<br />

blending than other patients. Russell-Lacy mentioned that a 'template' was used to determine<br />

286


differences in measurement systems. <strong>The</strong> finding that paintings <strong>of</strong> schizophrenics were<br />

both low in yellow and black, according to table 2 seems to be contradictory.<br />

'Neutral' colours: Blue and brown were indistinguishable for all groups and the range <strong>of</strong><br />

scoring was not wide. Blue was used moderately and brown was little used, controls used<br />

less than any other colour, although they used more than patients (but not significantly<br />

more). <strong>The</strong> groups most expected to use brown, seen as a dark dismal colour, were<br />

schizophrenics and depressives, but they used less than any other colour. <strong>The</strong>re was<br />

thus no support for the use <strong>of</strong> blue or brown as an indicator <strong>of</strong> depression, psychosis or<br />

patient status.<br />

Indications for removal <strong>of</strong> colours/alterations: <strong>The</strong> removal <strong>of</strong> green, which was<br />

associated in three patient groups; and blue and brown, which were non-discriminatory,<br />

would effectively eliminate related colours in patient groups. Removal <strong>of</strong> associated<br />

colours in controls would eliminate discriminatory factors. It would be useful to<br />

discriminate pencil lines from black to eliminate ambiguity <strong>of</strong> media.<br />

Summary for colour<br />

It was expected that there would be correlations between bright and warm colours: red,<br />

yellow and green and between any or all <strong>of</strong> green, blue, brown and black. Patients<br />

generally, but especially schizophrenics and depressives, were expected to produce less<br />

on the paper, use less colours, show preference for red and black and controls for<br />

multiple colours, especially the brighter warmer colours.<br />

quantification <strong>of</strong> colour and space, but generally this method and how many colours were measured was<br />

undescribed. One result out <strong>of</strong> a possible 10 would not exceed chance expectations.<br />

287


Control pictures showed the only association between the brighter colours, red<br />

and yellow, but they were not the only group to use multiple colours: schizophrenics<br />

showed association between all colours but black. Three <strong>of</strong> the other four groups showed<br />

associations with green. <strong>The</strong>re was no polarisation to darker colours/brighter colours for<br />

any or all <strong>of</strong> the patient/control groups. <strong>The</strong> separation between controls and the<br />

patients on quantity <strong>of</strong> colour is clear, especially in red and green. No colours, including<br />

black, isolated depression or personality disorder. <strong>The</strong>re were some positive indicators:<br />

Large quantities <strong>of</strong> black indicated substance abuse<br />

Small quantities <strong>of</strong> black indicated schizophrenia.<br />

Large quantities <strong>of</strong> red and green (and possibly yellow) indicated control.<br />

Most <strong>of</strong> the literature seemed to suggest more chromatic variation 311 , but comparison <strong>of</strong><br />

the DAPA procedure with other major studies is difficult since they group colours or use<br />

a system which includes opinion, such as 'inappropriate colour' 312 . Studies claim to be<br />

supported by reference to the patient, but the assumption that the patient is an informed<br />

source must surely be questionable, especially as most <strong>of</strong> the practical literature insist<br />

that the <strong>art</strong> process is an unconscious one, that the associations are made afterwards, and<br />

cannot be made by the patient alone 313 . This research generally refutes the diagnostic<br />

311<br />

W. L. Wadlington and H.J. McWhinnie (1973) <strong>The</strong> development <strong>of</strong> a rating scale for the study <strong>of</strong><br />

aesthetic qualities in the paintings <strong>of</strong> mental patients, Art Psychotherapy,<br />

V1(3-4):201-20, tested chromatic<br />

variation between 5 patient groups (in value and tonality) but schizophrenics were separated from paranoid<br />

schizophrenics. <strong>The</strong>y found most depressives and neurotics scored higher than schizophrenics and patients<br />

with adjustment reaction.<br />

312 <strong>The</strong> DDS, Cohen, Hammer and Singer (1988), op.cit.; Linda Gantt (1990)A validity study <strong>of</strong> the<br />

Formal Elements in Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Scale (FEATS) for diagnostic information in patients' drawings,<br />

Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, University Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania - 'colour fit'; <strong>The</strong> SPAR scale,<br />

Bergland & Gonzalez (1993), op.cit. - expressive 'developmental' level.<br />

313<br />

T. Dalley and C. Case (1992), Handbook <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , London: Routledge. This subject is addressed<br />

288


interpretation <strong>of</strong> the meaning, quality <strong>of</strong> colour and emotional associations 314 which have<br />

appeared in the literature and are summarised in table 2. Taking into account the<br />

diagnostic expectations, few <strong>of</strong> the common interpretations <strong>of</strong> the meaning or associations<br />

<strong>of</strong> colours made sense within the placement <strong>of</strong> diagnostic groups in this study;<br />

observations and experience reported in the literature probably related the colour more<br />

to the content and associations from the subject. <strong>The</strong> explanation that the affect value<br />

<strong>of</strong> a colour varies with some other characteristic <strong>of</strong> the painting 315 would probably<br />

demand a greater variation in the confidence intervals, so cautions from authors presenting<br />

these theories which recommend support from other case related aspects, as with the<br />

interpretations from Tasco are well founded. Generally the controls used more <strong>of</strong> all<br />

colours except black. This tends to cast suspicion generally on the table <strong>of</strong><br />

interpretations since normality should not represent an extreme. Where agreement with<br />

the interpretation was indicated, it was between patient groups but some indications were<br />

contradictory. <strong>The</strong> lower extremes were split between substance abuse and<br />

schizophrenia.<br />

in the introduction.<br />

314<br />

R.R. Crane and B.I. Levy (1962) Color scales in responses to emotionally laden situations, J.<br />

Consulting Psychology , V.26(6):515-9, different emotional situations lead to different colour scaling; I.G.<br />

M<strong>art</strong>in, Universal vs learned emotional responses to colors: afterthoughts to thesis research, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , V.9:245-7 discusses theoretical confusion <strong>of</strong> the literature on the relationship between color<br />

and affect, whilst broadly supportive <strong>of</strong> universal meanings <strong>of</strong> colour.<br />

315 Levy (1984/1980), op.cit. reported that colour and emotion were related, advised <strong>art</strong> therapists to<br />

consider colour "yet another source <strong>of</strong> information about our clients, a valuable guide in forming helpful<br />

reactions based on our clients' <strong>art</strong> work".<br />

289


yellow<br />

Structural variables<br />

Figures 4a-e. Map <strong>of</strong> association <strong>of</strong> structural variables measured between diagnostic<br />

groups using the DAPA test. denotes negative correlation.<br />

_____________________________________________________________________<br />

yellow<br />

intensity<br />

Controls<br />

Painted<br />

Line<br />

Schizophrenia Substance abuse<br />

red<br />

blue green<br />

intensity<br />

intensity<br />

red<br />

green<br />

Drawn<br />

Line<br />

Depression Personality Disorder<br />

Painted<br />

Line<br />

Painted<br />

Line<br />

Drawn<br />

Line<br />

Drawn<br />

Line<br />

290<br />

blue form<br />

Painted<br />

Line<br />

Drawn<br />

Line


<strong>The</strong>re were two obvious important associations between structural variables: associations<br />

between intensity and colour for three groups, together with the association <strong>of</strong> painted<br />

line and red/green for schizophrenia; and negative associations between painted and drawn<br />

line.<br />

Differences between groups - Intensity<br />

<strong>The</strong> order <strong>of</strong> brightness was much the same as that <strong>of</strong> amount <strong>of</strong> colour: controls,<br />

depressives, personality disorder, substance abusers and schizophrenics. We could<br />

expect relations between hue and intensity because the first qualities are contained by the<br />

latter, although not necessarily explicitly, but previous studies have found high<br />

correlations for intensity with colour 316 . Figures 4a-e show associations in three groups<br />

between intensity and different colours for each diagnostic group, confirming that<br />

intensity was actually measuring brightness <strong>of</strong> colour. So colour analysis alone cannot<br />

fully answer the question whether patients paint darker, or gloomier, pictures than<br />

controls 317 .<br />

Different colours in the paintings <strong>of</strong> patients and controls did vary systematically<br />

and supported the combination <strong>of</strong> measurements <strong>of</strong> hue and chroma, rather than tone<br />

316 Wadlington and McWhinnie (1973) op.cit., found correlation in hue and chroma and intensity <strong>of</strong> colour.<br />

317<br />

R. D'Andrade and M. Egan (1974) found that emotional associations with colour existed but were not<br />

confined to hue but to the degree <strong>of</strong> saturation and brightness in normal populations, <strong>The</strong> colours <strong>of</strong><br />

emotion, American Ethnologist , Feb. 1(1):49-63. <strong>The</strong>re are also indications, from preference studies, that<br />

different psychiatric groups may see different colours as dark, such as depressives but not controls grouped<br />

blue with dark colours in a study by M.J. Garvey and M. Luxenberg (1987), Comparison <strong>of</strong> color<br />

preference in depressives and controls, Psychopathology , V.20:268-271.<br />

291


directly, to answer this question. In combination with reference to the results for colour,<br />

it can be deduced that:<br />

Schizophrenics scored mostly neutral, towards low;<br />

Substance abusers used extremes <strong>of</strong> high and low intensity;<br />

Controls and depressives used more variety in colour and intensity.<br />

Personality disordered patients used a range <strong>of</strong> tone, but these scores were not<br />

interpretable due to small numbers in the group.<br />

<strong>The</strong> fact that there were no associations with black and intensity probably<br />

reflected the use <strong>of</strong> pencil. More pencil (scored as black) would not score higher in<br />

intensity, neither would large quantities <strong>of</strong> pink (scored as mixed red).<br />

Scores for schizophrenia indicated that most colours used were bright, especially<br />

green and yellow, since as more colour was used, the score for intensity increased.<br />

None <strong>of</strong> the substance abusers' colour scores related to intensity. <strong>The</strong> confidence<br />

interval for substance abusers was very short which indicated a high degree <strong>of</strong> conformity<br />

in the group. <strong>The</strong>y scored only slightly higher than schizophrenics on intensity (who<br />

scored the least), therefore colours were mainly used as neutral (mixed). <strong>The</strong> explanation<br />

<strong>of</strong> equal coverage <strong>of</strong> high (3 saturated) and low intensity colour (1 mixed muddy or<br />

watery colours) is possible but it is unlikely they would consistently use every colour<br />

in this way.<br />

Depressives used bright green and yellow but not red (Figure 4c), indicating some<br />

bright colours but muted reds and greys, not a prevalence <strong>of</strong> dark, gloomy, low intensity<br />

292


colours as predicted, which would show up as a correlation between dark colours and<br />

intensity.<br />

Controls used bright red consistently, but not bright yellow or green (the reverse<br />

<strong>of</strong> depressives), because intensity was correlated only with red. <strong>The</strong>re is a correlation<br />

between high red and high yellow (figure 3e; but increases were more variable,<br />

scatterplots 17: results) which implies they were used together but high scores on<br />

intensity had more to do with the presence <strong>of</strong> red. <strong>The</strong>refore there was minimal influence<br />

on judged intensity from yellow as for green, blue, brown and black nor negative<br />

influence. This indicates more muted colours for controls and not bright colours as<br />

previously thought (except red), but it is likely that controls used more mixed colours,<br />

thus creating a more positive 'feeling' <strong>of</strong> light and harmony, which probably positively<br />

influenced emotional tone and intensity ratings.<br />

Evidence supports the suggestion that the domination <strong>of</strong> the picture by gloomy<br />

and low intensity colours (not black) indicates disturbance <strong>of</strong> thought, but not depression<br />

or substance abuse. Controls did prefer bright colours; they were not, however,<br />

characterised by lack <strong>of</strong> use <strong>of</strong> the 'gloomy' colours and they used a varied range <strong>of</strong><br />

intensity. <strong>The</strong>se results suggest varied hues would be appropriate rating criteria for<br />

controls.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was evidence to support the sectioning <strong>of</strong> patients into high and low<br />

intensity by diagnostic type, even though the high category included black, since it<br />

seemed to isolate certain patient groups, but it would probably increase diagnostic rigour<br />

to eliminate neutral from the intensity category. Although intensity correlated with red,<br />

293


yellow and green, its removal would remove a discriminating variable for schizophrenics,<br />

and in combination, intensity may contribute to the qualitative interpretation <strong>of</strong> the<br />

structural variables. It would probably be better to incorporate it into a reduced colour<br />

list.<br />

Differences between groups - Line quality<br />

Older studies mention thick heavy line for patients, especially schizophrenics 318 . In the<br />

DAPA study, thick line was notably absent: if thick lines were scored, they were<br />

swamped by the lower scores in the production <strong>of</strong> averages, which seems likely because<br />

there were inconsistencies in homogeneity <strong>of</strong> scores for painted lines within groups 319 .<br />

Contemporary studies tend to show no differences between diagnostic groups on line<br />

quality 320 , and the results from the DAPA agree ins<strong>of</strong>ar as they show there was a limited<br />

range <strong>of</strong> line for all groups for both types <strong>of</strong> line, but the DAPA did pick up differences<br />

in this limited range. <strong>The</strong> distribution statistics suggested little use <strong>of</strong> painted or drawn<br />

line but the information is ambiguous because there is little difference between DAPA<br />

measures <strong>of</strong> few lines and thin lines. <strong>The</strong> confidence intervals for all groups in painted<br />

318<br />

Amos (1982), op.cit. schizophrenics with prominent paranoid tendencies, produce drawings with heavy<br />

overall line emphasis, p.135.<br />

319 <strong>The</strong> Levene statistic was not computable because painted line was infrequently used in some groups.<br />

B<strong>art</strong>letts box (table 8, results) put scores for painted line just within the range for discrimination<br />

purposes.<br />

320 Gantt (1990), op.cit. found no differences in 4 groups <strong>of</strong> patients and a control group in line quality;<br />

the DDS, Cohen et al. (1988), op.cit. found no differences in 3 types <strong>of</strong> patients to controls; Langevin et<br />

al. (1975) op.cit. found no differences in schizophrenics and normal controls and Miljkovitch et al. (1982),<br />

op.cit. found no differences in line with schizophrenics and other patients.<br />

294


lines ranged from 0-thin and a little higher in drawn lines, indicating a greater proportion<br />

<strong>of</strong> zero scores (lack <strong>of</strong> use).<br />

Painted line: Only schizophrenic pictures showed association <strong>of</strong> line with colour: either<br />

painted lines were red and green, or they were inconsistent in colour and bright green or<br />

red was consistently present for some other reason. This provides an opportunity for<br />

further investigation for clinical utility.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re may have been an element <strong>of</strong> lack <strong>of</strong> control <strong>of</strong> paint by patients and<br />

especially substance abusers, who scored least, which was concealed by the non-use <strong>of</strong><br />

painted line by patients, although controls' use was still low. <strong>The</strong> order <strong>of</strong> scores for<br />

drawn line was the opposite <strong>of</strong> that for painted and there was a strong negative<br />

correlation between painted and drawn line in every group except substance abusers (who<br />

used the least painted lines and the most drawn) thus subjects used either drawn or<br />

painted line, not both at once.<br />

Drawn Line: Controls should have been mid-range in drawn line, but instead, scored less<br />

than other groups. In fact, the confidence interval for controls remained consistent<br />

through painted or drawn lines, it was the patients' strategies that changed (this further<br />

supports the division <strong>of</strong> drawn and painted line for patients). It is possible that thick and<br />

thin lines were always scored together (varied = 2) so reducing the mean score to neutral,<br />

but most groups scored in the 0-1 range indicating a greater proportion <strong>of</strong> 0 scores (no<br />

line present). Drawn lines were more prevalent in substance abusers and patients with<br />

personality disorder.<br />

295


Schizophrenics used little colour except red, they used low intensity colours, and<br />

little line. This implies some other activity than colour was filling the space (since from<br />

the results, space was filled). <strong>The</strong> correlation <strong>of</strong> drawn line with space may be<br />

underestimated since the plot showed a non linear relationship, unusual in that there was<br />

a loose correlation up to about half filled with drawn line, so drawn line was most used<br />

at that point and then as space increased, drawn line decreased. This pattern is consistent<br />

with the negative correlation between painted and drawn lines shown by most groups.<br />

<strong>The</strong> space was filled with either colour or line (and none consistently as there were no<br />

correlations with space). Line was likely to be light pencil (possibly writing 321 ), which<br />

fits with the score for low intensity black and predominantly thin line.<br />

321 Many studies report 'writing in' as a frequent indicator <strong>of</strong> schizophrenic drawings, Amos 1982 gives<br />

examples, op.cit., or patient status, Cohen et al. 1988, op.cit., but others have found no differences<br />

Russell-Lacy et al. 1979, op.cit., or infrequency Miljkovitch et al., 1982, op.cit.<br />

296


Differences between groups - Space<br />

<strong>The</strong>re wasn't as much variation as expected between the controls and the patients and no<br />

other variable had an effect on space left uncovered in the picture. Small but significant<br />

differences existed, but all groups' paintings lay within the 25-55% space bandwidth and<br />

no group consistently left large amounts <strong>of</strong> empty space. <strong>The</strong>se findings are curious in<br />

view <strong>of</strong> the reports from the literature which diverge widely so some explanation is<br />

needed.<br />

I have already indicated the variety <strong>of</strong> definition <strong>of</strong> space in the literature, the<br />

most common is perspectival space. Psychiatric patients have been distinguished from<br />

controls on dimensionality <strong>of</strong> elements or deformation <strong>of</strong> perspective 322 , but traditional<br />

views <strong>of</strong> disintegration <strong>of</strong> perspective, especially in schizophrenic <strong>art</strong> have latterly been<br />

challenged 323 , although some latter-day studies have also found differences in odd placing<br />

<strong>of</strong> elements and objects or figures 'floating in space' 324 . <strong>The</strong> general understanding <strong>of</strong> this<br />

'space' is difficult to define because it assumes that paper left uncovered is orientated<br />

322 Patient groups are differentiated from controls: for example, the SPAR scale found that patients with<br />

personality disorder scored lower than normal controls on perspectival space (Bergland and Gonzalez<br />

(1993, op.cit.); Miljkovitch and Irvine (1982 op.cit.) found no differences between schizophrenics and<br />

other patients but poor performance generally on space from all patients, and schizophrenics were more<br />

likely to draw planned views than others.<br />

323 Amos (1982) op.cit., reviews most <strong>of</strong> the essential 1960s and 1970s literature which characterise<br />

schizophrenic disintegration <strong>of</strong> hierarchical perspective, proportion and logical spatial organisation under<br />

the subheading <strong>of</strong> 'composition'; Russell-Lacy et al. (1979 op.cit.) found schizophrenics used more two<br />

dimensional space than normal controls although they weren't differentiable from patient controls.<br />

Disagreement in later views: Wadeson's table <strong>of</strong> characteristics <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia, depression and mania<br />

report disorganisation, disturbed spatial organisation, disintegration for schizophrenia agree with Amos,<br />

but her own experiments (1976) with schizophrenics showed wide variations (cited in Wadeson 1980,<br />

op.cit. p.190-192); Gantt's experiments with the FEATS study show no differences in use <strong>of</strong> perspectival<br />

space between 5 groups including controls (1990 op.cit.).<br />

324<br />

Cohen, Hammer and Singer (1988) op.cit.; Bergland and Gonzalez (1993), op.cit.; J.B. Couch (1994),<br />

<strong>The</strong> Diagnostic Drawing Series: research with older people diagnosed with organic mental syndromes and<br />

disorders, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.20(3):231-241.<br />

297


directly to the environment; assessment includes the objects within it so that it appears<br />

widespread, whereas a simple groundline would have 'located' the objects 325 . <strong>The</strong> DAPA<br />

uses a definition <strong>of</strong> covered area <strong>of</strong> the picture, as a less common but recognised objective<br />

assessment 326 and does not distinguish between these kinds <strong>of</strong> visual representation. <strong>The</strong><br />

patient scores indicate they produced less on the paper, but also indicate sketchy work,<br />

which is <strong>of</strong>ten seen as empty because it lacks richness, solidity and detail. A thin yellow<br />

wash could be described as empty space to someone looking for a depiction <strong>of</strong> a visual<br />

plane, and filled, by someone looking for floating objects or a ground, but the DAPA<br />

would record that 'space' as filled without exception. No group in this study consistently<br />

produced much uncovered space. Any mark within the field was included and the 'space-<br />

bubble' was estimated around it 327 - if there were two spaces in the square, only one was<br />

included. Thus sometimes the picture would look as if it had more space than the score<br />

for space indicated.<br />

Alternative explanations <strong>of</strong>fered for results from the discussion <strong>of</strong> intensity and<br />

line that the type <strong>of</strong> measurement may have obscured the results when they were<br />

extreme, cannot apply here. <strong>The</strong> weighting <strong>of</strong> 5, given to empty squares would have<br />

given a higher average score if applied throughout the picture. <strong>The</strong>refore there must have<br />

been a considerable portion <strong>of</strong> the population with 55-75% filled space. It could be<br />

325 See explanations <strong>of</strong> this definition in Cohen, Hammer and Singer (1988), op.cit.; Gantt (1990), op.cit.<br />

326 Wright and McIntyre (1982, op.cit.) found significant differences in depressive and normal controls<br />

using a similar method; Russell-Lacy et al. (1979, op.cit.) used 'a template' (the only description) to<br />

quantify space and found differences in patients and controls on covered paper; the DDS - Cohen, Hammer<br />

and Singer (1988, op.cit.) found that patients mainly used '33-99%' <strong>of</strong> paper, but the actual range covered<br />

was indeterminable as it allowed an inch around the perimeter <strong>of</strong> the paper for the extra 1%!<br />

327 see definition for space DAPA rating guide 1994, methods section.<br />

298


argued that the procedure <strong>of</strong> taking the mean <strong>of</strong> several paintings might have simply<br />

produced a neutral score from wildly different paintings. Three points refute this: (i) the<br />

scale point in the middle and with the widest interval was 3(25-55%), whereas scores for<br />

space centred on 2 (10-25%); (ii) the confidence intervals are very small indicating very<br />

little variation within groups; and the ANOVA between paintings showed no significant<br />

differences between paintings (Results section).<br />

Differences between groups - Emotional Tone<br />

Comparison with other studies is difficult here because it is assumed that the content is<br />

decodable and assumptions about the meanings <strong>of</strong> what is portrayed form the basis <strong>of</strong><br />

most studies. Generally the patient picture is expected to score much lower: to distort<br />

proportion, perspective and to show more negative content than the control 328 ; there are<br />

said to be reflections <strong>of</strong> thought disorder in schizophrenia, hopelessness and despair in<br />

depression 329 . Although the DAPA results agree with the general tone <strong>of</strong> the literature,<br />

that controls score higher than patients, there was a narrower band <strong>of</strong> variation than this<br />

literature suggests. All patient scores hovered around neutral and there were no<br />

correlations with other variables, suggesting that structure and colour had no separable<br />

328 Distinguishing features <strong>of</strong> psychotic <strong>art</strong> reported in the literature, collected by Wadeson (1980), op.cit.<br />

p.190 although she does not endorse all <strong>of</strong> these characteristics, especially that disorganised behaviour<br />

reflects in the drawings. She does note that hopelessness and emptiness, enclosed trapped feelings<br />

predominate in the themes <strong>of</strong> depressed patients' paintings; Amos (1982), op.cit., in a synthesis <strong>of</strong> the<br />

1960s and 1970s literature suggests content reflects the schizophrenic's world view <strong>of</strong> unusual or<br />

maladaptive experiences, impaired reality sense, no baseline reference, disconnected and bizarre images,<br />

especially human, heavy line emphasis, and words, disintegrated composition without regard for<br />

perspective, primitive style and inappropriate or uncontrollable use <strong>of</strong> colour.<br />

329 Shoemaker (1978 op.cit.) provides a sensitive 'guidelist' <strong>of</strong> how to examine a painting by a patient for<br />

visually available dimensions 'within which the specific definitions <strong>of</strong> <strong>psychopathology</strong> may fall, as a step<br />

towards measurement'; she suggests: synthessence, space, substance, time, energy, relativity, reflection<br />

<strong>of</strong> perception and expression.<br />

299


influence on apparent negativity. Where precedents for the DAPA results exist, they<br />

used systematic methods 330 and it is not certain that previous studies have taken the<br />

effect <strong>of</strong> quality judgements into consideration 331 , even though the justification for<br />

selection <strong>of</strong> features may be theoretically remote.<br />

A probable explanation for these findings lies in the test procedure. Other studies<br />

may have placed increased emphasis on negative indicators thus influencing the view <strong>of</strong><br />

the whole painting as negative, whereas corresponding or small positive p<strong>art</strong>s are ignored.<br />

<strong>The</strong> DAPA scores tell us that no group consistently produced negative indicators over<br />

the whole picture surface. Where negative scores were given, they must have been<br />

balanced by positive scores, because <strong>of</strong> the centring on neutral. <strong>The</strong> confidence intervals<br />

were very short in the groups most expected to produce negative paintings: schizophrenia<br />

and depression, indicating little variability within group and we know that the sequence<br />

<strong>of</strong> paintings did not show fluctuations from negative to positive 332 .<br />

330<br />

Russell-Lacy et al. (1979 op.cit.) found differences isolating schizophrenics from both controls and other<br />

patients in repetitions <strong>of</strong> abstract forms (mannerism) only, between patients and controls only in these<br />

form variables: imbalance, detail, two dimensions and space covered between patients and controls. <strong>The</strong>re<br />

were no differences in relationship <strong>of</strong> imagery, focal points, monochromism, perspective, words or<br />

portrayed houses in the picture; Wadeson (1980, op.cit.) warns that casual observation alone revealed no<br />

p<strong>art</strong>icular patterns <strong>of</strong> content in diagnostic groups; E.L. Phillips, S.K. Geller and M. Ireland (1983),<br />

Research on the use <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy in a university setting, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.23(1):26-9 were unable<br />

to assess whether themes in <strong>art</strong>work were sequential.<br />

331<br />

examples <strong>of</strong> previous studies finding no relationship in thematic content after quality judgements were<br />

accounted for have been; R. Langevin and L.M. Hutchins (1973), An experimental investigation <strong>of</strong> judges'<br />

ratings <strong>of</strong> schizophrenics and non-schizophrenics paintings, J. Personality Assessment , V.37(6):537-543,<br />

results from 13 judges ratings <strong>of</strong> 200 paintings as schizophrenic/normal were only 10% greater than chance<br />

using their own criteria which was subsequently found to be <strong>art</strong> quality; E. Feher, L. Vandicreek, H.<br />

Taglasi (1983), <strong>The</strong> problem <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> quality in the use <strong>of</strong> human figure drawings, J. Clinical Psychology ,<br />

V.39(2):268-275, although 8 from 16 judges were warned on quality, all returned a 58% hit rate on<br />

quality.<br />

332 Different paintings by individuals were compared by analysis <strong>of</strong> variance (see results section) in order<br />

to determine the propriety <strong>of</strong> taking the mean value for each subject, results were non-significant.<br />

300


<strong>The</strong> DAPA considers the area <strong>of</strong> negative tone within the context <strong>of</strong> the whole<br />

painting. This may not be clinically relevant for interpretations, since although a negative<br />

area may be small, it may dominate and give focus to the painting subjectively. However,<br />

there seems little indication <strong>of</strong> clinical depression from a large proportion <strong>of</strong> negative<br />

areas as measured by the DAPA, since the groups predicted as most likely to score<br />

negative: schizophrenics and depressives, who actually occupied the same range according<br />

to the DAPA, scored less negative than the substance abusers. Recent research on<br />

emotional associations with image intensity shows some correspondence with these<br />

results and suggests that pictorial intensity reflects current feelings rather than general<br />

tendencies 333 . <strong>The</strong>se results support Wadeson's 334 contention that schizophrenics<br />

present with typical pictorial signs <strong>of</strong> depression. <strong>The</strong> implication is then that substance<br />

abusers might subjectively feel more depressed than all groups. <strong>The</strong> confidence intervals<br />

for personality disorder are much wider than the other groups, indicating greater<br />

variability, and more inclination towards positivity (but this probably reflects<br />

inadequacy <strong>of</strong> the smaller sample size).<br />

Comparison <strong>of</strong> form<br />

<strong>The</strong> lower scores for dominant form in depressives and the higher use in controls<br />

gives support to the general view <strong>of</strong> dissolution or lack <strong>of</strong> form in depressive illness<br />

333 Kaplan, Imagery and expression <strong>of</strong> anger (1994 op.cit.), found that black and red associate with feelings<br />

<strong>of</strong> anger and image intensity reflects current angry feelings rather than propensity.<br />

334 Wadeson (1980, op.cit.) p.193, 324.<br />

301


ather than for schizophrenia 335 . This might have been more obvious in content and<br />

influenced the judgement <strong>of</strong> emotional tone, so a negative correlation between emotional<br />

tone and form might have been a reasonable expectation. <strong>The</strong>re is, however, no such<br />

evidence; small non-significant positive correlations indicate that the judgement <strong>of</strong> the<br />

positivity <strong>of</strong> the painting was not influenced by form.<br />

<strong>The</strong> position <strong>of</strong> substance abuse, which scored significantly more form than<br />

depressed, indicates that form should be a true predictor <strong>of</strong> depression, unlike most <strong>of</strong><br />

the other elements which put substance abuse further down the scale than depression.<br />

Patient groups exhibited some dominance <strong>of</strong> form, but their focus covered less <strong>of</strong> the<br />

surface than that <strong>of</strong> controls, who tended to use about half the page; this probably<br />

accounts for the confusing results when untrained raters are apparently able to distinguish<br />

patients from controls slightly better than chance using their own criteria, which usually<br />

turn out to be quality. Higher levels <strong>of</strong> form, though, are not necessarily an indication <strong>of</strong><br />

health as the pictures <strong>of</strong> substance abusers were not distinguishable from controls.<br />

Substance abusers did not use more blue or form than other groups, but there was<br />

a high negative association between blue and form. <strong>The</strong> plot for blue and form is unusual<br />

in that very little blue, when half filled with form decreases to no blue as form increases;<br />

it is likely that blue was deposed by the form, being a background colour (sky?) rather<br />

335 Amos (1982, op.cit.) reports Billig's 1969 observations <strong>of</strong> a schizophrenic recovery "beginning with<br />

random scribblings" and generally dissolution <strong>of</strong> form in schizophrenia; Wadeson's (1980, op.cit.)<br />

categorisation <strong>of</strong> <strong>psychopathology</strong> from the literature shows schizophrenics were thought to produce<br />

disorganised and deteriorated compositions, but she reports experiments from 1976, which disagree. She<br />

found the majority <strong>of</strong> schizophrenic pictures full formed and developed, depressive-like qualities such as<br />

colourless, emptiness, were noted in a few paintings from manic/depressive and schizophrenics also.<br />

Wadlington and McWhinnie (1973), op.cit. found no differences between patient groups (including<br />

schizophrenia and depressives) on dominant form and size relations.<br />

302


than a fill colour, but no colour is p<strong>art</strong>icularly associated with form so there was no<br />

consistent fill or outline colour.<br />

Conclusion for the discussion <strong>of</strong> results<br />

<strong>The</strong> results from the DAPA scales follow the general direction <strong>of</strong> the empirical literature<br />

in that psychiatric pictures were shown to use less colour, were <strong>of</strong> lower intensity, fewer<br />

lines were drawn and painted, they left more space, were less positive in content and<br />

contained less dominant form than pictures by controls. However, the results do not<br />

agree with most <strong>of</strong> the diagnostic differentiations between patient groups suggested by<br />

the early impressionistic literature. DAPA results also disagree in the magnitude <strong>of</strong> the<br />

patient/control differences.<br />

Summary: <strong>The</strong>re are two related likely explanations which have probably polarised the<br />

patient/control differences and suggested other differences between patients because they<br />

are antithetical strategies:<br />

1. Reports from the literature could have emphasised the importance <strong>of</strong> p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> a<br />

picture because <strong>of</strong> the overwhelming influence <strong>of</strong> content on subjective<br />

judgement;<br />

2. <strong>The</strong> way the DAPA scores pictures reduces the importance <strong>of</strong> p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> a picture<br />

because it measures the distribution <strong>of</strong> an element over the picture plane, rather<br />

than concentrating on how a message is conveyed over p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> it. If small, the<br />

focus point <strong>of</strong> a picture could have quite intense scores but the picture would<br />

score mainly from the material surrounding it, in the production <strong>of</strong> averages.<br />

303


Supporting this point, the focus area is usually encompassed by the dominant<br />

form, which is considerably smaller for patients than for controls.<br />

This last is an important point because there is no standard form for the way we<br />

look at and describe pictures, so it seems quite acceptable for us to attend to details in<br />

one group and whole paintings in another. Judgement may occur on different indices,<br />

individually and subjectively according to what the judge finds important, or worth<br />

considering in that group especially in impressionistic reports 336 . <strong>The</strong>re may indeed be<br />

differences in the pictures, but if, for example, red is actually distributed equally in both<br />

groups but being attended differently in the focus group because it denotes unsatisfactory<br />

or violent content (maybe as blood/danger in patients, but cheerful flowers in control),<br />

what is being reported as amount <strong>of</strong> red may actually denote something else.<br />

Unsatisfactory aspects <strong>of</strong> the procedure for rating<br />

<strong>The</strong>re are several unsatisfactory issues in the DAPA procedure. <strong>The</strong> structuring process<br />

was not essentially statistical. <strong>The</strong> statistical process was applied afterwards and is<br />

constrained by its categorical organisation. This difficulty cannot be resolved from<br />

outside; more experience with statistics has changed my approach to these processes and<br />

a similar programme would have the statistical procedures organised integrally from the<br />

beginning.<br />

336<br />

J.K. Dent and H.Y. Kwiatkowska, (1970), Aesthetic preferences <strong>of</strong> young adults for pictures drawn by<br />

mental patients and by members <strong>of</strong> their immediate families, Sciences d'L'<strong>art</strong> , V.7(1-2):43-54. found that<br />

20 judges <strong>of</strong> 1500 paintings by mixed psychiatric patients trying to describe the paintings and indicate<br />

their preferences showed wide individual differences in what they attended to and very poor criteria and<br />

judgement, out <strong>of</strong> 20 common descriptive elements, 10 were related to the personality <strong>of</strong> the judges.<br />

304


Effect <strong>of</strong> the distribution <strong>of</strong> elements over the picture plane<br />

It is possible that a lot <strong>of</strong> empty space could swamp a small amount <strong>of</strong> information<br />

generated by the DAPA. Presently it is difficult to distinguish a consistent small amount<br />

<strong>of</strong> media from a concentrated area in one p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the picture from individual scales,<br />

although it can be deduced from a combination <strong>of</strong> related scales.<br />

Further development <strong>of</strong> the DAPA should consider the issue <strong>of</strong> distribution <strong>of</strong><br />

a small amount <strong>of</strong> material over the picture. <strong>The</strong> averaging process between all 20<br />

sections <strong>of</strong> the grid works for the colour and form measures <strong>of</strong> presence/absence, but<br />

intensity, line, space and emotional tone would have a nil rating for empty space,<br />

confusing measures <strong>of</strong> little with none as it has for line. I have tried to avoid this problem<br />

by scoring empty squares on the emotional tone and intensity scale as neutral, but this<br />

has probably biased the scoring range towards neutral and may even obscure strong<br />

contrasts. If the mean <strong>of</strong> line, emotional tone and intensity were taken between occupied<br />

squares by treating the empty squares as missing data, it would obscure how much <strong>of</strong> the<br />

picture was taken up. A better answer would be to reduce the subvariables (low, neutral,<br />

high) to 2 values (low and high) effectively eliminating neutral. Later development will<br />

enable different p<strong>art</strong>s <strong>of</strong> the picture to be considered separately. A better way would<br />

probably record missing values and average between as many grid sections filled. It<br />

would also be useful to correlate records <strong>of</strong> position <strong>of</strong> the Dominant Form so that<br />

persistent compositional devices could be measured as products <strong>of</strong> Space/Form.<br />

Rating problems.<br />

305


Several kinds <strong>of</strong> difficulties were encountered, especially in the attitude <strong>of</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>essionals<br />

towards a content-free measure <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>. <strong>The</strong> positive points were that the form and length<br />

<strong>of</strong> the instrument, although training was required, were satisfactory. Raters' agreement<br />

was high and they found the instrument understandable and reasonably easy to use in a<br />

systematic way, although tedious, so there were no obstacles to the judges rating. Close<br />

adherence to the rating guide improved reliability and in this respect <strong>art</strong> students may not<br />

have been the best raters, since they already had their own concepts <strong>of</strong> the terms used by<br />

the DAPA, especially for form, and were not well disposed towards <strong>quantitative</strong> ratings.<br />

None <strong>of</strong> the raters could suggest any further formal dimension upon which the picture<br />

could be rated.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were 3 areas <strong>of</strong> discussion <strong>of</strong> weaknesses <strong>of</strong> the procedure. <strong>The</strong>y are<br />

presented here with development possibilities:<br />

(i) Colours: <strong>The</strong> rating instrument probably gives too much emphasis to colour,<br />

however, there seems no obvious grouping system since the traditional dark/light<br />

separation was not found to relate well to diagnosis, and is subjective since colours look<br />

lighter and darker with changes in environment. <strong>The</strong> alternative is to measure only those<br />

colours which have high discriminatory properties and which do not strongly correlate<br />

with other variables, so that results are independent. Orange, purple and white were<br />

eliminated due to infrequent or inconsistent use; blue and brown were non-discriminatory.<br />

<strong>The</strong> elimination <strong>of</strong> green could further remove associations between colours in<br />

schizophrenia, but equally it may be that the effect <strong>of</strong> the combination with the other<br />

colours may be important in discrimination and other associations would occur. <strong>The</strong><br />

306


pattern <strong>of</strong> associations itself might be important. Colours measured would otherwise be<br />

reduced to red, yellow and black, which might be too few. Pencil line might have<br />

influenced scores for black, so it should be scored differently.<br />

(ii) <strong>The</strong> information value <strong>of</strong> Painted line was conveyed by the strong negative<br />

relationship to drawn line in the majority <strong>of</strong> groups. <strong>The</strong> normality <strong>of</strong> the distribution<br />

<strong>of</strong> painted line was questionable, and elimination would make intensity independent for<br />

schizophrenia.<br />

(iii) Form was a p<strong>art</strong>icularly difficult area for non-<strong>art</strong> trained people, and even <strong>art</strong><br />

trained people found it difficult to objectify. <strong>The</strong> initial explanation <strong>of</strong> form was<br />

misunderstood, so further development went on. <strong>The</strong> rating guide (appendix 4) shows<br />

the final explanation for form. Speculatively though, a simplified 'most intense focus area'<br />

explanation might point out the crucial focus <strong>of</strong> the picture in four adjacent squares which<br />

form a box shape would probably limit the training and provide a more accessible<br />

explanation, but this requires further development.<br />

Relation <strong>of</strong> the results from <strong>Study</strong> 2 to <strong>Study</strong> 1<br />

Differences between methods<br />

<strong>Study</strong> 2 used more clearly differentiated subjects and larger groups so differences should<br />

be more obvious, some differences are expected because <strong>of</strong> the overlap between subject<br />

types in <strong>Study</strong> 1. <strong>Study</strong> 2 was retrospective, conditions were not standardised and<br />

content was free, where study 1 held the content constant to some extent with self-<br />

307


portraiture, so it is difficult to say which <strong>of</strong> these elements probably contributes the most<br />

to differences between diagnostically similar groups on specific variables for study 2.<br />

Differences in scores from the first study: three variables were transformed, and this<br />

commentary describes the usage figures (transformed back) so as to make better sense<br />

between relations to figures from <strong>Study</strong> 1. Five findings from study 1 were generally<br />

supported; two <strong>of</strong> the non-significant findings (blue and brown) and three out <strong>of</strong> four<br />

significant variables identified in study one (yellow, intensity and line), which showed<br />

only slight changes in study 2 (the other significant variable, orange, was too infrequent<br />

to be used).<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was no signiicant difference between the findings <strong>of</strong> Studies 1 and 2 for<br />

brown or blue. Patients used more brown in study 1, this put them equal with controls<br />

who used roughly the same amount in study 1 as 2; depressed patients used more blue<br />

in study 1 and controls less, but these small fluctuations did not affect the outcome <strong>of</strong> no<br />

significant difference between groups.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was little difference in the findings <strong>of</strong> studies 1 and 2 for yellow. Both<br />

depressed patients and schizophrenics scored only slightly higher than brain injury in<br />

study 1 which group was not included in study 2. Controls used slightly less yellow in<br />

study 2, but were still differentiated from schizophrenics, the group who used least.<br />

Generally the scores for intensity hovered around the same point, neutral for both<br />

studies. Controls scored much higher than the patients in study 1; in study 2 the<br />

schizophrenic score was lower than the rest and this is probably attributable to better<br />

308


diagnostic grouping, but the controls' score was also reduced, so these two groups were<br />

identified as significantly different, by approximately the same proportion in both<br />

studies.<br />

Two study variables were subjected to a change <strong>of</strong> scoring procedure for study<br />

2, so they cannot be accurately compared, but general findings are similar although<br />

sharper distinctions are drawn.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were significant differences in line in both study 1 and 2 between controls<br />

and patients. <strong>Study</strong> 1 records more variation than study 2. This is probably due to the<br />

combined assessment <strong>of</strong> painted and drawn line in study 1 which tends to see painted line<br />

as thick and drawn as thin when adjacent, but then judges on relative thickness when<br />

separated. <strong>The</strong> position <strong>of</strong> controls was reversed when the painted line was separated<br />

from the drawn line, from most (painted) to least (drawn), which probably explains their<br />

thick line scores in study 1, when there were no thick line scores in study 2 in either<br />

painted or drawn line.<br />

<strong>The</strong> rating for space was made simpler for study 2, so scores cannot be so<br />

accurately compared. <strong>The</strong> scores indicated the same sort <strong>of</strong> media coverage <strong>of</strong> the surface<br />

area (the scores for study 1 represented the average percentage coverage <strong>of</strong> square, and<br />

in study 2, the average space per square); no group had predominantly empty pictures.<br />

So generally scores for study 2 were similar to those for study 1 except that<br />

schizophrenic pictures seemed to cover more surface area and thus the controls were not<br />

differentiated. This could be an effect <strong>of</strong> another group characteristic secondary to<br />

schizophrenia but it is likely that this was an effect <strong>of</strong> the scoring procedure for study<br />

309


1, as generally the scores for schizophrenia were stable in other areas, indicating a similar<br />

percentage <strong>of</strong> space used. <strong>The</strong> groups showed more 'bunched' positioning in study 2 at<br />

around 25-30% space, this had the effect <strong>of</strong> isolating the controls at around 10-20% space<br />

in study 2.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were slight differences in four variables which produced distinctions from the<br />

results <strong>of</strong> study 2: there were trends but no significant differences for study 1.<br />

Although controls and depressives used about the same amount <strong>of</strong> red in both<br />

studies, substance abusers and p<strong>art</strong>icularly schizophrenics used much more red in study<br />

1. Some schizophrenics and some substance abusers for study 1 also had personality<br />

disorder, which tends to scatter the results (from study 2). <strong>Study</strong> 2 isolated the controls<br />

from substance abusers and put the schizophrenics level with depressed, who had not<br />

fluctuated and were a more discrete group.<br />

Ap<strong>art</strong> from substance abusers whose score reduced considerably in study 2, there<br />

were only minimal differences in green between study 1 and 2, but it was enough to<br />

isolate the controls, who scored slightly more than most <strong>of</strong> the patients.<br />

Substance abusers scored much more black in study 2, which isolated this group<br />

as significantly higher scoring than all other groups. This could be an effect <strong>of</strong> better<br />

grouping. Depressives showed slightly higher scores for black in study 1 but some<br />

secondary diagnoses were alcohol related; the schizophrenic group in study 1 included<br />

psychosis due to drug and alcohol use. In study 2, the majority <strong>of</strong> the substance abuse<br />

was for alcohol and the schizophrenic group did not include psychosis: if drug use pushes<br />

up the black score, it would explain both higher scores. <strong>The</strong> drug abuse group scored<br />

310


much less in study 1, but the sample were mostly outpatients using varied substances in<br />

a recovery state, whereas for study 2, they were inpatients, more homogenously alcoholic<br />

and mostly in an acute state. This different group explanation does cover the<br />

possibilities, an alternative could have been that content influenced work, but this is<br />

unlikely because the controls who worked in the same surroundings at the same time<br />

remained constant. <strong>The</strong>se results support the explanation <strong>of</strong> differences between groups<br />

for study 2 (at the beginning <strong>of</strong> this chapter) suggesting that substance abusers may<br />

communicate more subjective unhappiness through their work than other groups.<br />

Emotive tone should certainly be affected by content and differences were<br />

expected between studies 1 (fixed content) and 2 (free content). All groups were slightly<br />

negative in study 1, with no significant differences; in study 2, all groups were slightly<br />

positive except substance abusers, whose pictures contained more large quantities <strong>of</strong><br />

black, but whose rating remained stable but relatively lower than other patients.<br />

Schizophrenics increased their score only slightly, but the pictures <strong>of</strong> depressives and<br />

controls were both more positive. <strong>The</strong> only thing that changed in free content was that<br />

controls were differentiated as more positive than all patients.<br />

Summary: Generally, trends from study 1 were followed in study 2, and mostly, groups<br />

remained surprisingly constant in their use <strong>of</strong> colour and structure in their pictures, with<br />

one or two exceptions from p<strong>art</strong>icular groups. However, fluctuations due to changes in<br />

the painting generation procedure, the sharper definition <strong>of</strong> the patient groups and greater<br />

311


numbers resulted in differentiation for study 2, where study 1 showed no differences.<br />

In study 1:<br />

Controls showed 8 small fluctuations, 6 were probably attributable to content:<br />

From 4 colour variables, less red, yellow and brown and more blue, only red made<br />

a significant difference. <strong>The</strong>ir paintings were viewed as less positive and more<br />

intense. <strong>The</strong> other two were attributable to changes in the measure: they left<br />

more space and they used more or thicker lines.<br />

Substance abusers used more green, less black and yellow. <strong>The</strong>se were all large<br />

fluctuations and probably attributable to better grouping in study 2.<br />

Depressives used more blue and brown, less yellow, more line and their paintings<br />

seemed more negative. <strong>The</strong>y were small fluctuations probably attributable to<br />

content and made no significant difference.<br />

Schizophrenics used more red and black and less yellow; their paintings showed<br />

more space and were less intense. <strong>The</strong>se fluctuations were probably attributable<br />

to better grouping in study 2.<br />

All these fluctuations resulted in 6 changes <strong>of</strong> significance level for study 2, which mostly<br />

followed the trends from study 1. Although there were differences in the methodology<br />

<strong>of</strong> the study, the way some <strong>of</strong> the categories were rated, the sampling and the results<br />

from the items rated the general conclusions remained the same:<br />

Controls were most different from substance abusers and schizophrenics.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was least difference between Schizophrenia and Depression.<br />

312


Patients were clearly differentiated from non-patients.<br />

In addition, the differences reported from the findings <strong>of</strong> the DAPA to what is regarded<br />

as common knowledge in the literature were maintained:<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was no greater use <strong>of</strong> black in depressives compared with other groups.<br />

Depressive paintings were not more negative than those <strong>of</strong> other groups.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was no consistent impoverishment in the pictures <strong>of</strong> depressives or<br />

schizophrenics compared with other groups (although pictures by patients<br />

generally covered slightly but significantly less picture area than those <strong>of</strong> controls<br />

in study 2, and schizophrenics used less colour).<br />

Generally then, the conclusions <strong>of</strong> <strong>Study</strong> 1 are supported by study 2. <strong>The</strong> effect<br />

<strong>of</strong> content seemed to be greater on the controls, which was against expectations, and<br />

better grouping seemed to affect the scores <strong>of</strong> substance abusers and schizophrenics<br />

most.<br />

This measure has the facility to clearly distinguish psychiatric patients from<br />

controls and also different diagnostic groups from combinations <strong>of</strong> variables (see results<br />

section; identification <strong>of</strong> groups by characteristic). Patients and controls were clearly<br />

differentiated by most colour ratings and all structural ratings. <strong>The</strong> findings from study<br />

2 are consistent with results from study 1. In addition, preliminary finding from study<br />

1 <strong>of</strong> inconsistencies in commonly believed characteristics <strong>of</strong> pictures by patient groups<br />

were supported.<br />

In <strong>Study</strong> 2, 6 from 12 variables, which appeared frequently enough to measure,<br />

were identified as discriminating groups at very high levels <strong>of</strong> significance and 4 variables<br />

313


at or above the usual level. Personality disorder was p<strong>art</strong>icularly hard to differentiate, but<br />

this may reflect wide symptom variability in diagnosis. In the early stages <strong>of</strong><br />

experimentation, it would be more appropriate to use groups which are more clearly<br />

differentiable diagnostically. Difference in content did affect formal measures <strong>of</strong><br />

paintings, but not as much as predicted by Amos 337 . <strong>The</strong>re were however, large<br />

differences for emotional tone, the only content variable. It seems that pictures were<br />

more discriminable using free subjects than with content held constant, so this study also<br />

predicts possible discriminable differences in content, the measures for which, as<br />

presented in the published literature, presently do not compare well with the DAPA in<br />

reliability and discrimination. This study points towards a common metric which could<br />

contribute to <strong>art</strong> assessments and complement the present analytic or dynamic methods.<br />

Discriminatory power between controls and patients<br />

This p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the chapter examining the comparison between results <strong>of</strong> the DAPA and<br />

what is generally accepted as <strong>art</strong> characteristics <strong>of</strong> patients by the psychotherapy<br />

literature discusses the validity <strong>of</strong> the discrimination between patients and controls using<br />

(i) comparison <strong>of</strong> effect sizes with the literature and (ii) the discriminant analysis.<br />

(i) Is the DAPA a better assessment than the other tests reviewed in Chapter 2?<br />

Effect sizes<br />

337 This study clearly refutes the statement by Amos (1982), op.cit. that "prognostic and therapeutic<br />

implications for the content <strong>of</strong> schizophrenic drawings remain largely a matter <strong>of</strong> interpretation and clinical<br />

judgment. This will be the case for composition, style and use <strong>of</strong> colour also".<br />

314


<strong>The</strong> magnitude <strong>of</strong> the relationship (effect size) for characteristics <strong>of</strong> pictures was<br />

calculated in Chapter 2 for the 11 studies which gave interpretable results as though they<br />

were independent. This strategy was perhaps controversial as an assessment <strong>of</strong> the<br />

papers judged because <strong>of</strong> interrelationships between the variables. It was justified<br />

because it gave an indication <strong>of</strong> the type <strong>of</strong> characteristics measured by the vast majority<br />

<strong>of</strong> studies which give either insufficient information or whose methodology obviously<br />

compromised the results. <strong>The</strong>re would be a vast degree <strong>of</strong> multicollinearity in these<br />

relations, but it is impossible to sort out because <strong>of</strong> many reasons, not least these major<br />

contributions to confusion:<br />

Most <strong>of</strong> the categories, which were put together by the author from similar<br />

indices <strong>of</strong> different studies, were probably heterogeneous within tables. This<br />

means that where 'line' was measured in one study, 'line' in another study was not<br />

the same element;<br />

All studies used different variables from each other, and different diagnostic<br />

groups, so it was impossible to compare even pairs <strong>of</strong> studies. Replications <strong>of</strong>ten<br />

brought up different results.<br />

Of 14 categories from the literature review (effect sizes calculated in Chapter 2),<br />

the categories <strong>of</strong> line, form and colour achieved negligible effect sizes (below _=0.02)<br />

where the DAPA effect sizes were medium and large for line and form and averaged above<br />

medium effect for colours, which were measured separately (large effects on red and<br />

green) and a medium effect for the DAPA category <strong>of</strong> intensity which was also measured<br />

separately; the space category from the literature review achieved a small-medium effect<br />

315


size (_=0.36) which contrasts wildly with the medium-large effect from the DAPA<br />

(_=0.74); 7 other separate categories were encompassed under the DAPA emotional tone<br />

category: three categories, those <strong>of</strong> themes, reality and complexity which seemed very<br />

close in meaning but were measured separately by most studies, showed negligible effect;<br />

quality showed a low effect; energy and composition, medium effect. Only content as<br />

defined by counts <strong>of</strong> specific psychiatric indicators showed a high effect comparable to<br />

the general category <strong>of</strong> emotional tone. <strong>The</strong> other high effect from the literature review,<br />

body details was not included in the DAPA, there was a medium-high effect on control<br />

which was not included in the DAPA test and neither was detail in other areas (which<br />

achieved a small effect).<br />

<strong>The</strong> deletion <strong>of</strong> negligible effects from the literature review set <strong>of</strong> categories gave<br />

a subset <strong>of</strong> 8 significant areas <strong>of</strong> measurement <strong>of</strong> drawing with a medium effect size<br />

(_=0.57), and is still lower than that <strong>of</strong> the DAPA, but it only covers two DAPA<br />

categories and was relatively overloaded on content. <strong>The</strong> DAPA test therefore seems<br />

more sensitive to different areas <strong>of</strong> measurement <strong>of</strong> a painting and is potentially a more<br />

effective discrimination tool. However, the t-test does not take account <strong>of</strong><br />

interrelationships between variables, nor the number <strong>of</strong> tests performed 338 so this<br />

analysis is limited to a theoretical comparison <strong>of</strong> independent effect <strong>of</strong> the variables with<br />

that produced in Chapter 2 and is not an appropriate measure to predict practical<br />

discriminations.<br />

338 We could use the Bonferroni correction here, but it merely confuses the data and the problem <strong>of</strong><br />

correlation <strong>of</strong> the variables remains and it is better to use the following analysis.<br />

316


(ii) <strong>The</strong> Discriminant analysis technique - Which variables discriminate between<br />

patients and controls?<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were 6 common variables which turned up 3 or more out <strong>of</strong> 5 times in the<br />

derivation <strong>of</strong> the function from 5 different p<strong>art</strong>itions <strong>of</strong> the data set: most <strong>of</strong> which<br />

variables showed the largest effect sizes identified above. Step 1 used Emotional tone,<br />

which had the largest effect (_=0.92) or green (_=0.89), which had the highest F to enter.<br />

<strong>The</strong> other variables in order <strong>of</strong> their appearance were: Form (_=0.88), Space (_= -0.74),<br />

Drawn line (_=-0.56), and Black (n.s.). Painted line, which did not appear, was highly<br />

negatively correlated with Drawn line and so the effect must have been accounted for<br />

with the entry <strong>of</strong> Drawn line as it is unlikely that the F value was not high enough on<br />

Painted line. It is a mystery to me, given the small effect size, why Black was chosen as<br />

a discriminating variable, unless it was because it took the large effect <strong>of</strong> Red because <strong>of</strong><br />

the high negative correlation. <strong>The</strong> results for the ANOVA show that controls were not<br />

differentiated on either high or low black from the patients. Red was highly correlated<br />

with intensity, which also wasn't included. On the two occasions Red did turn up, none<br />

<strong>of</strong> the 6 common variables were consistently absent, so it was probably excluded because<br />

its effect became negligible after accounting for other variables. <strong>The</strong> middle effects were<br />

not included; Yellow, Brown, Intensity, Painted line, although Drawn line (_=0.57) was<br />

included and Blue (_=0.36), so these low effects were probably due to correlation with<br />

Green, although they all showed F less than 1 when the analysis stopped. <strong>The</strong><br />

discrimination set therefore contained most <strong>of</strong> the variables with high effect sizes and was<br />

generally consistent with the independent t-tests for effect size. <strong>The</strong> best discrimination<br />

317


sets had all the identified high effect variables involved in the discrimination, and the least<br />

had only 3. <strong>The</strong>refore, the best discrimination probably includes most <strong>of</strong> the variables,<br />

which was consistent with their correlation with the discriminating function; so<br />

interactions were complex and there was no major influence from one or two variables.<br />

Using the information from the discriminant analysis and the t-tests we can say<br />

that content is a powerful discriminatory index, it comes first in the discrimination<br />

analysis and it has the highest effect size in both the DAPA and the reviewed literature<br />

analysis, but that the caution with which most authors regard molecular analysis is<br />

justified, because few <strong>of</strong> the molecular measures <strong>of</strong> content in the review analysis actually<br />

discriminated. <strong>The</strong> highest effect from the review, for counts <strong>of</strong> psychiatric<br />

characteristics, was highly subjective and probably difficult to operationalise, whereas<br />

the DAPA global judgement was equally effective and did not require rigorous<br />

procedures. Green was highly discriminative, but the tests in the review used a different<br />

index; i.e. 'quality <strong>of</strong> colour' tests, in which some subset or other p<strong>art</strong>ial preprocessed<br />

colour judgement was made, <strong>of</strong>ten mixed up with subjective content judgements, and this<br />

may explain the difference in effect for colour. Black was included in dark colours. <strong>The</strong><br />

next most valid discriminator was form, only one study in the review actually used it as<br />

a compound 339 ; different spectrums <strong>of</strong> shape and composition from other studies were<br />

found inconsistent. Space, which was mainly measured by perspective in the review,<br />

distinguished mental retardation but was not as effective as total cover, and <strong>of</strong> the rest,<br />

the difference in effect can be explained in that no other study produced a reliable strategy<br />

339 But raters found their measure difficult to understand and produced inconsistent responses.<br />

318


for estimating cover. Line was measured in many ways and inconsistently - many studies<br />

had to be excluded because <strong>of</strong> reliability issues and the main point <strong>of</strong> the line review was<br />

that line was difficult to measure objectively without relation within the picture.<br />

Because the other variables from the DAPA test are not included in the<br />

discriminant analysis does not necessarily render those non-significant, especially for<br />

inter-patient discrimination. <strong>The</strong> analysis worked slightly differently each time with each<br />

p<strong>art</strong>ition <strong>of</strong> the data removing certain variables for others. <strong>The</strong>re were medium effects on<br />

colours and a different system <strong>of</strong> combining these might remove confounding correlations<br />

and it would also be useful to use a bigger sample <strong>of</strong> controls. <strong>The</strong>se other variables may<br />

be important but the problems <strong>of</strong> multicollinearity must be addressed. This is a task for<br />

more extensive statistics which would not be appropriate on this sample.<br />

Weaknesses <strong>of</strong> the study<br />

<strong>The</strong> study was not able to control for medication, or IQ/ability; measures were recorded<br />

but casenotes were too inconsistent, so more independent measures could fine these<br />

results. <strong>The</strong> size <strong>of</strong> the control group was too small for proper comparison with each<br />

patient group and was not matched for age and sex, but these variables were statistically<br />

controlled and did not invalidate the main findings. Because the staff could not be<br />

assumed not to understand the purpose <strong>of</strong> the study, and as with any other such study,<br />

could have produced deliberately different paintings than the patients, two control<br />

measures were taken: most <strong>of</strong> the study was done on retrospective work - the staff and<br />

patients had already produced paintings as p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the recreational programme <strong>of</strong> the<br />

319


hospital; the mean <strong>of</strong> up to 10 paintings by each person was taken, and all comparisons<br />

were between groups, so a considerable number <strong>of</strong> the control group would have had to<br />

be deliberately and consistently altering their style to seriously affect the results.<br />

It must be restated here that diagnoses were necessarily quite distinct because<br />

ICD-10 classifications were made from casenotes, which included lists <strong>of</strong> observations<br />

<strong>of</strong> the patient by trained psychiatrists but nevertheless, some diagnoses may have been<br />

made from uncertain criteria. <strong>The</strong>se circumstances were controlled as much as possible;<br />

difficult to classify cases and dual diagnoses were excluded, the groups were as widely<br />

distinct as possible and patients were usually in severe states requiring hospitalisation.<br />

Controls were as far as could be determined, functioning members <strong>of</strong> the working<br />

population, so existing mental health problems would have been mild and quite distinct<br />

from the patients.<br />

Strengths <strong>of</strong> the study as a whole<br />

Through a careful review <strong>of</strong> the literature, this study showed that traditionally the<br />

study <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> has fallen into the domain <strong>of</strong> the subjective rather than the objective.<br />

Attempts to externalise subjective and probably non-verbal responses have usually relied<br />

on explanations which are longer and more complicated than the picture itself and<br />

influenced by expectation. <strong>The</strong> argument has been that significant indices <strong>of</strong> the quality<br />

or direction <strong>of</strong> a painting can be indicated by attending to intuitions and a combination <strong>of</strong><br />

knowledge <strong>of</strong> style, skill and history <strong>of</strong> the maker that usually serves an encounter with<br />

<strong>art</strong>. In this sense mental projection onto the object is intrinsic to the nature <strong>of</strong> how the<br />

320


painting is made and viewed. But it has been shown that subjective impressions are<br />

rather more questionable when they are taken to be an interpretation <strong>of</strong> fact.<br />

<strong>The</strong> DAPA study took a scientific approach to look at what is produced as <strong>art</strong>work,<br />

objectively and did not take account <strong>of</strong> mental projections onto the object. It attempted<br />

to measure on externally observable criteria what was publicly verifiable using quantified<br />

information, which made it easier to specify which aspect <strong>of</strong> the observation was being<br />

attended.<br />

Unlike other studies the DAPA took account <strong>of</strong> normal distribution <strong>of</strong> variables in<br />

order to adhere to the assumptions <strong>of</strong> the testing procedure and produce valid<br />

interpretation <strong>of</strong> the results.<br />

This study produced a reliable scale through the elimination <strong>of</strong> subjective decisions,<br />

which was consistent and unambiguous, and demonstrated a method <strong>of</strong> comparing<br />

agreement that was valid, unambiguous and more appropriate than previous studies.<br />

A careful and thorough testing procedure compared the essential elements with those <strong>of</strong><br />

the previous literature and found some consistencies and gave coherent explanations for<br />

the discontinuities.<br />

Strengths <strong>of</strong> the DAPA<br />

It was important to establish whether the removal <strong>of</strong> content from judgements <strong>of</strong><br />

paintings allowed diagnostic distinctions. <strong>The</strong> DAPA demonstrated an ability to make<br />

distinction between paintings from different diagnostic groups. This is consistent with<br />

previous findings using mixed form/content scales but the use <strong>of</strong> form increases the<br />

321


eliability <strong>of</strong> the test and shows the neglect <strong>of</strong> the literature in this area since formal<br />

measurements previously employed have been piecemeal and ineffective. <strong>The</strong> literature<br />

review showed that content measures can be reliable where such variables are carefully<br />

described and operationalised. <strong>The</strong> effect size for this study far exceeds that <strong>of</strong> previous<br />

content only measures and thus justifies further research.<br />

<strong>The</strong> DAPA can be used as a complementary assessment which does not interfere<br />

with normal <strong>art</strong> therapy programmes. It uses standard scientific terminology and<br />

produces <strong>quantitative</strong> continuous scores rather than ordinal or graded sections thus<br />

allowing more powerful statistical comparisons than previous tests. <strong>The</strong> DAPA does not<br />

require specific materials and does not need specialist <strong>art</strong>-trained staff. It does not<br />

require information from the patient about the painting, therefore inconsistencies are<br />

minimalised; the systematic approach eliminates attempts to interpret or judge intention<br />

and it is independent <strong>of</strong> the theoretical origins <strong>of</strong> the investigator. <strong>The</strong> DAPA is primarily<br />

a descriptive system which provides a standardised medium for a variety <strong>of</strong> uses and has<br />

potential for development.<br />

Although the assessment is quite difficult and training was required, interrater<br />

results were well ahead <strong>of</strong> the field. This study therefore supports the use <strong>of</strong> systematic<br />

<strong>art</strong> measures, especially in circumstances where additional verbal material is not available.<br />

Effectiveness <strong>of</strong> <strong>The</strong> DAPA: Error types, 1 and 2, can be related to mistakes in clinical<br />

testing; 1. To diagnose someone as psychiatrically unstable who is normal; and 2. to<br />

322


diagnose someone as normal who is psychiatrically unstable. <strong>The</strong> DAPA is more likely<br />

to err on type 1, which is on the side <strong>of</strong> caution.<br />

Variation was minimal in patient discrimination and ranged only from 95-100%<br />

on the selected cases and from 81-98% on the independent data; but the control sample<br />

ranged from 50-88% for identification on the selected sample and from 50-80% (although<br />

the average was 65%) on the independent data set using the function derived from the<br />

selected data. When the control sample size was bigger, i.e. when the function derived<br />

from the control group <strong>of</strong> 18 was applied to the independent set <strong>of</strong> 5, it produced a better<br />

discrimination. <strong>The</strong> sample size therefore looks like the most influential variation factor;<br />

greater numbers should improve discrimination. <strong>The</strong> correct classifications yield is<br />

comparable with other systems in use such as the G.H.Q. and the MSE, even though the<br />

patient sample included some with diagnoses which were widely scattered (personality<br />

disorder). It can be seen from the breakdown <strong>of</strong> specific differences earlier in this<br />

chapter, the widest confidence intervals were for patients with personality disorder, they<br />

most <strong>of</strong>ten spanned other groups and were hardest to identify so it is likely that the<br />

removal <strong>of</strong> these patients would increase the effectiveness <strong>of</strong> the discrimination.<br />

323


Chapter 7. Conclusion<br />

This thesis represents an attempt to devise an empirical measure for <strong>art</strong> products that<br />

was theoretically coherent.<br />

Literature Review<br />

Chapter One argued that the approach to psychiatric <strong>art</strong> has neglected rigorous empirical<br />

study <strong>of</strong> <strong>psychopathology</strong> in favour <strong>of</strong> phenomenology.<br />

<strong>The</strong> literature review reported that impressionistic papers provided some<br />

theoretical consensus that a change in the patient was reflected in the <strong>art</strong>work <strong>of</strong> the<br />

patient, but there was little sequential comparison <strong>of</strong>fered or relation to the method to<br />

direct the change, and no evidence <strong>of</strong> a coherent system to access the <strong>art</strong>work. Claimed<br />

benefits were independent <strong>of</strong> assessment <strong>of</strong> the patient or the work, aims or descriptions<br />

<strong>of</strong> the therapy and explanatory theory.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re were 4 routes <strong>of</strong> access to the image from Case studies:<br />

1. Most therapists who claimed the <strong>art</strong>work produced expressed feelings did not<br />

define this term, nor present their rationale for relation <strong>of</strong> feelings to the image.<br />

<strong>The</strong>y described their use <strong>of</strong> paintings to monitor progress and develop<br />

communication, but there was no common base for interpretation.<br />

2. Signposts Content characteristics identified were dependent upon the importance<br />

<strong>of</strong> associations assigned by the therapist. Formal characteristics were overladen<br />

with emotional terminology, <strong>of</strong>ten negative, and there were conflicting findings<br />

324


from different studies using slightly different definitions. Both <strong>of</strong> these categories<br />

lacked standard objective definitions <strong>of</strong> visual phenomena.<br />

3. Studies emphasising the therapeutic relationship were all based on a allegorical<br />

model <strong>of</strong> activity which did not relate to the picture at all.<br />

4. Studies which emphasised behaviour change simply described the environment<br />

<strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong> room as a relaxing or useful friendly site for implementation <strong>of</strong> new<br />

patterns <strong>of</strong> behaviour.<br />

Contrary to the contemporary recommendations towards accumulation <strong>of</strong> this<br />

kind <strong>of</strong> reportage as 'data' to be compiled later 340 , there was little transferable information<br />

relevant to the investigation <strong>of</strong> the decoding process; it was reported obscurely,<br />

subjectively, and inconsistently. Benefits reported were subjectively described by the<br />

reporter and there was no independent evidence. Benefits were found to be unrelated to<br />

the methods used and the treatment given <strong>of</strong>ten bore little relationship to the assessment.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was agreement that aspects <strong>of</strong> visual <strong>art</strong> did indicate psychiatric status and that<br />

therapy based on <strong>art</strong> was beneficial to the patient, but not on description <strong>of</strong><br />

characteristics <strong>of</strong> paintings, nor on how <strong>art</strong> related to the therapy.<br />

Controlled studies<br />

When the one rater studies, invalid instruments and unreliable variables had been removed<br />

for the meta-analysis, content measures showed a higher overall effect than form,<br />

340<br />

Among others: A. Gilroy (eds.) (1992), Research in Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, in D. Waller and A. Gilroy, Art <strong>The</strong>ra py: A<br />

Handbook , Buckingham: Open University, p.238 advocates the comparison <strong>of</strong> amassed case 'evidence', but has not<br />

attempted such herself. L. Gantt (1986), Systematic investigation <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>works: some research models drawn from<br />

neighbouring fields, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy ,V.24(4):111-8, recommends case studies as the optimum research mode<br />

325


probably because the form variables were fewer, widely scattered and defined more<br />

explicitly. Additionally, it was shown that the structures and conceptual fields in which<br />

drawings were assessed, which were adopted by most studies, may be irrelevant. Thus<br />

the conclusions <strong>of</strong> the case studies were repeated - <strong>art</strong> is sensitive, but we cannot say to<br />

what it is sensitive.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re was a significant effect for most <strong>of</strong> the elements which were assembled into<br />

the drawing categories defined by the reliablity tables; they differentiated patients from<br />

controls and patients from other patients. <strong>The</strong> instruments reviewed discriminated<br />

controls better than patients. Empirical studies which found no difference were fewer,<br />

were less controlled and used less valid instruments, with more content-based scales than<br />

those which found differences. Subjective categories which required interpretation, fine<br />

distinctions and global judgments were the most unreliable. <strong>The</strong>re were widespread<br />

conceptual mistakes in the use <strong>of</strong> mathematical language, flaws in the testing procedure,<br />

validation <strong>of</strong> the measure, the analysis and the interpretation <strong>of</strong> results.<br />

<strong>The</strong> DAPA: Development <strong>of</strong> a valid instrument<br />

From the literature analysis, it was reasonable to suppose that if the object presented<br />

some discriminable quality which was consistently encoded in the structure <strong>of</strong> the work,<br />

proper analysis <strong>of</strong> the object would reveal traces <strong>of</strong> it.<br />

Objective formal categories were more reliable and accessible than content<br />

categories. <strong>The</strong> DAPA presented a more systematic method than most tests currently<br />

for therapists, as others, see introduction.<br />

326


in use because it distributes the elements over the whole painting rather than<br />

concentrating on a p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the image to be subjectively decoded. This systematic and<br />

objective system is innovative since previous instruments have always placed the<br />

emphasis on meaning and therefore concentrated on p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the object. It has been<br />

recognised, although not very widely, that statistics for drawing tests are <strong>of</strong>ten unreliable<br />

because there is physically less, or less definable image, on the patient side 341 , as well as<br />

a limiting effect because <strong>of</strong> inadequate translation <strong>of</strong> intention. This was not a problem<br />

for the DAPA because the avoidance <strong>of</strong> subjective identification <strong>of</strong> elements <strong>of</strong> content<br />

throws the focus on the structure <strong>of</strong> the work, no translation is required.<br />

Effectiveness and validity <strong>of</strong> the DAPA<br />

<strong>The</strong> DAPA was developed as a psychiatric assessment to objectively define and describe<br />

psychopathological criteria <strong>of</strong> paintings. It comprised 6 rating scales <strong>of</strong> mostly formal<br />

elements derived from predicted psychiatric symptoms from clinical observations <strong>of</strong><br />

psychiatric pictures and from the literature: colour, intensity, line, space, emotional tone,<br />

form.<br />

In three studies, the DAPA has proved:<br />

reliable<br />

flexible enough to be used retrospectively<br />

discriminatory: between patients and nonpatients and to a lesser extent<br />

341<br />

Stanley R. Kay (1978), Qualitative differences in human figure drawings according to schizophrenic subtype,<br />

Perceptual and Motor S kills , V.47:923-932 - reporting floor/ceiling effects in retarded schizophrenics. This effect<br />

is <strong>of</strong>ten reported as 'statistically but not clinically significant'.<br />

327


etween different groups <strong>of</strong> patients.<br />

<strong>The</strong> Main <strong>Study</strong><br />

<strong>The</strong> main study reported a validation experiment with 109 subjects, in which there were<br />

no specific instructions to patients and where there would be no interference with the<br />

therapeutic milieu. It was hypothesised there would be significant difference between<br />

two or more diagnostic categories <strong>of</strong> patients or controls on each <strong>of</strong> the 12 remaining<br />

subscales after 3 infrequent colours were removed: colour (1-6) - red, yellow, green, blue,<br />

brown, black; intensity (7); line (8-9) - painted and drawn; space (10), emotional tone<br />

(11) and form (12).<br />

Paintings were collected from controls and hospitalised adult patients mid-<br />

treatment. Patients fit the ICD10 categories for: schizophrenia, major depression,<br />

substance abuse, personality disorder.<br />

Results: Analysis <strong>of</strong> variance showed significant difference between two or more groups<br />

on ten subscales from 12. <strong>The</strong> Duncan pairwise post-hoc comparison showed differences<br />

in patient/control pictures and between patient groups.<br />

Patients were significantly different from controls on:<br />

Schizophrenics on 6 variables: yellow, green, black, intensity, emotional tone and<br />

form.<br />

Depressives on 3 variables: space, emotional tone and form.<br />

328


Personality disorder on 5 variables: green, drawn line, space, emotional tone and<br />

form.<br />

Substance Abusers on 7 variables: red, green, black, painted line, drawn line,<br />

space, emotional tone.<br />

Within patient significant differences:<br />

Schizophrenics from depressives on 2 variables: black and form; from substance<br />

abuse on black.<br />

Personality disorder from depressives on form; from schizophrenics on intensity;<br />

from substance abuse on black.<br />

Substance abusers from depressives on 4 variables: black, painted line, drawn line,<br />

form; from schizophrenics and personality disorder on black.<br />

Only three variables showed differences that isolated diagnoses - black (2 diagnostic<br />

groups isolated); emotional tone, and dominant form. It seemed likely the other groups<br />

could be isolated through combinations <strong>of</strong> variables. <strong>The</strong> discriminant analysis technique<br />

was the most appropriate instrument for predictive analysis, but entry for significant<br />

variables was complicated by association between measures. <strong>The</strong> functions derived<br />

varied from 3-6 variables, and suggested that most <strong>of</strong> the variables contributed to the<br />

result. <strong>The</strong> analysis consistently differentiated differences between patient and controls<br />

80-90% accurately. Unlike the measures from the literature, the DAPA differentiated<br />

patients better than controls. This was encouraging for further development <strong>of</strong> the<br />

measure and suggested that formal analyses <strong>of</strong> paintings provide a extra dimension,<br />

329


previously unexplored, which complement traditional content interpretation and may<br />

contribute considerably to the research knowledge <strong>of</strong> the field.<br />

<strong>The</strong> findings do not support the traditional view <strong>of</strong> colour interpretation. <strong>The</strong>y<br />

do support the broader picture <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>istic impoverishment in psychiatric populations.<br />

It was important to establish whether the removal <strong>of</strong> subjective content from judgements<br />

<strong>of</strong> paintings allowed diagnostic distinctions. <strong>The</strong> DAPA demonstrated an ability to make<br />

distinction between paintings from different diagnostic groups. This is consistent with<br />

previous findings using mixed form/content scales but the use <strong>of</strong> form increased the<br />

reliability <strong>of</strong> the effect and showed the neglect <strong>of</strong> the literature in this area since previous<br />

attempts at systematic measurements have been piecemeal and ineffective. <strong>The</strong> effect<br />

size for this study far exceeds that <strong>of</strong> content only measures and thus justifies further<br />

research. Though the DAPA worked well, the view <strong>of</strong> the majority <strong>of</strong> the literature,<br />

<strong>of</strong> content based scales as more sensitive than formal elements, is nonetheless supported,<br />

because it was the content features <strong>of</strong> the DAPA: emotional tone and form which gave<br />

the best and most accurate discrimination.<br />

<strong>The</strong> main findings <strong>of</strong> this study are augmented by the literature review in that it<br />

was shown that there was no evidence <strong>of</strong> privileged knowledge available to <strong>art</strong><br />

therapists 342 . Thus the concept <strong>of</strong> privileged knowledge was unhelpful to the<br />

development <strong>of</strong> appropriate theory for this area <strong>of</strong> investigation, and therefore small<br />

samples with long texts on personal interpretation have not been helpful to identifying<br />

342 This has already been recognised for years in the experimental literature, but is not generally acknowledged.<br />

<strong>Study</strong> by B.I. Levy and E. Ulman (1967) Judging <strong>psychopathology</strong> from paintings, J. Abnormal Psychology,<br />

V.72(2):182-7.<br />

330


the features <strong>of</strong> <strong>psychopathology</strong>. This finding, and the fact that some traditional<br />

gradations <strong>of</strong> drawing features were ineffective as discriminators opens up a whole new<br />

area <strong>of</strong> primary exploration in both formal and content evaluation.<br />

Is the DAPA better than other <strong>art</strong> assessments?<br />

<strong>The</strong> question whether the DAPA is better than other <strong>art</strong> assessments is meaningless.<br />

Previous assessment methods have tried to do two jobs: to provide a standardised format<br />

for <strong>art</strong> therapy approaches, and also to provide immediate accessible lists <strong>of</strong><br />

characteristics from an individual picture for the <strong>art</strong> therapist. <strong>The</strong> DAPA assessment<br />

requires much processing and is not suitable for these purposes. No immediate<br />

impression can be gained from its indices. However, no other published <strong>art</strong> assessment,<br />

to my knowledge, provides a replicable, reliable and effective discrimination between<br />

patient groups. Effect size comparison between patients and controls places the DAPA<br />

far above the rest <strong>of</strong> the field. <strong>The</strong> majority <strong>of</strong> other tests assess content as a whole and<br />

then relative to p<strong>art</strong>s; the other apparent structural indexes are hardly mentioned,<br />

although they are commented on qualitatively. <strong>The</strong> DAPA results show that the raw<br />

data for a simplified objective measure <strong>of</strong> content (emotional tone) only just satisfied the<br />

assumptions for ANOVA and the energy-focus <strong>of</strong> the picture (form) did not satisfy the<br />

criteria. Simplified statistics therefore are not appropriate for these measures and as far<br />

as I know, none have taken account <strong>of</strong> distribution bias or <strong>of</strong> correlations between the<br />

variables. This problem <strong>of</strong> multicollinearity, in addition to the problem <strong>of</strong> misused<br />

331


statistical tests and missing data makes most studies uninterpretable and therefore<br />

incomparable with this study.<br />

Where do we go from here?<br />

Improvements and further development <strong>of</strong> the instrument:<br />

Replication studies should take into account the weaknesses already mentioned in the<br />

rating procedure, and use better criteria for patient diagnoses. It is obvious that content<br />

measures are inescapable even though their rating is difficult and can be inconsistent.<br />

Greater complexity in description and fine discriminations simply promote confusion and<br />

do not add significantly to the discrimination properties <strong>of</strong> the instrument. Furthermore<br />

human beings cannot simply attend to form and discard the image. Gibson explains that<br />

a person can:<br />

notice only the information for the perception <strong>of</strong> what is represented or he can pay<br />

attention to the picture as such, the medium, the technique, the style, the<br />

composition, the surface, and the way the surface has been treated, what is 'in' the<br />

picture or to the picture itself .... a person can never fully attain to the<br />

representational attitude and wholly exclude from attention such features <strong>of</strong> the<br />

picture itself as its surface texture or its arrangement <strong>of</strong> line and colour.<br />

What is needed for further development is a very simple level <strong>of</strong> measurement at which<br />

form and content become one. Form in the DAPA context is not a simple matter <strong>of</strong><br />

configuration. Any picture has properties referring to itself and referring outside it when<br />

relating to something else. <strong>The</strong> discardable concepts can be understood as those which<br />

332


elate the picture to external things. What is needed could be synthesised in a new<br />

concept <strong>of</strong> feature, where content is measured through the formal internal properties <strong>of</strong><br />

the picture rather than relating to the external, like the measures <strong>of</strong> dominant form and<br />

emotional tone already employed on the DAPA, so that they specifically and<br />

consistently relate to the observer and not to other associative mental projections. This<br />

is basically what has been attempted all along by previous empirical studies which have<br />

developed so-called 'formal' analyses. Many describe the 'measurement <strong>of</strong> pictures' as<br />

if it was self evident how and what was being measured, <strong>of</strong>ten without going through the<br />

rigorous process <strong>of</strong> testing the measure appropriately and use evasive and immaterial<br />

concepts; this is not the answer. <strong>The</strong> attempt to evolve a metric <strong>of</strong> visual form through<br />

the problem <strong>of</strong> measuring visual form or pattern is a subject ideally addressed by<br />

objective or scientific research. It may well be a mistake to suppose that expression<br />

always lies outside the reducible qualities <strong>of</strong> measureable dimensions. <strong>The</strong> incorporation<br />

<strong>of</strong> 'feature', using the objective internal relation to the picture, <strong>of</strong>fers a potential bridge<br />

between form and content and is already regularly and reliably used in projective testing,<br />

such as the appearance <strong>of</strong>, for example, figures in a painting, and their relative size,<br />

without going into the psychodynamic inferences.<br />

<strong>The</strong> future for measures <strong>of</strong> <strong>psychopathology</strong><br />

<strong>The</strong> validity <strong>of</strong> the sole use <strong>of</strong> assessments which rely on an interpretative<br />

construct by the therapist must be questionable, especially in cases where the patient is<br />

verbally compromised. If there is no relation or an indirect relation between visual and<br />

verbal symptomatology in the patient, then we cannot use verbal explanations <strong>of</strong> visual<br />

333


<strong>psychopathology</strong> in clinical diagnosis. <strong>The</strong>re are two main arguments to map its<br />

characteristics:<br />

a) All the advances in psychiatry have been based on a careful and complete descriptive<br />

<strong>psychopathology</strong> <strong>of</strong> phenomenology. A phenomenology that does not include<br />

visuospatial elements cannot be a complete description.<br />

b) <strong>The</strong> verbal phenomenology under Mental State Examination is hidden in many<br />

disturbed patients e.g. drug addicts; conduct or behaviourial disorders in children;<br />

personality disorders and some emotional disorders. If the verbal differential is lacking,<br />

then the case is diagnosed solely on the history. <strong>The</strong> descriptive <strong>psychopathology</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>art</strong>work by the patient may show other phenomenology which may be categorisable and<br />

assist differential diagnosis in difficult situations.<br />

Thus the future must be towards greater distinction and more precise<br />

measurements. <strong>The</strong> obvious development here would be to adapt this system for a<br />

computer programme, using more section divisions, to take advantage <strong>of</strong> the greater<br />

facility <strong>of</strong> scanning technology and repetitive consistent decisionmaking.<br />

Further development taking account <strong>of</strong> the weaknesses <strong>of</strong> the study, replication<br />

and refining <strong>of</strong> the instrument is indicated.<br />

334


Appendix 1<br />

List <strong>of</strong> Variables for Analysis <strong>of</strong> Commonalities<br />

1. Impressionistic/<strong>The</strong>oretical Studies<br />

2. Case Studies - therapeutic benefit - expression <strong>of</strong> feelings<br />

3. Controlled Studies<br />

4. Validity Studies<br />

335


6 scales to categorise commonalities in impressionistic, theoretical or experiential papers<br />

which report therapeutic effects or benefits <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> for psychiatric patients.<br />

One category for each scale, except benefit, where the most emphatic four benefits were<br />

selected.<br />

1. Origin <strong>of</strong> study material, descriptive Information:<br />

Opinion - Author's philosophy with no specific support, but might<br />

reference others.<br />

Discussion - Argues two or more sides <strong>of</strong> an issue.<br />

Experience - Report <strong>of</strong> a specific personal experience using <strong>art</strong> in<br />

therapy with psychiatric patients, sometimes illustrated<br />

and called a 'case report', but with no patient information.<br />

Programme - Description or update <strong>of</strong> an <strong>art</strong>s therapy hospital<br />

programme.<br />

2. Diagnosis: under these groups from information in the paper:<br />

1 schizophrenia; 2 traumatic stress/sex abuse; 3 substance abuse; 4 psychotic; 5<br />

Alzheimer's/dementia/brain damage; 6 emotional disorder; 7 depression; 8 conduct<br />

disorder; 9 normal; 10 retarded; 11 undifferentiated psychiatric patients; 12 sex<br />

abnormalities/abusers; 13 personality disorder.<br />

3. <strong>The</strong>oretical base:<br />

Psychotherapeutic - supportive, interpretive, humanistic, therapeutic.<br />

Psychoanalysis - using psychoanalytic language (may not have been<br />

traditional or valid).<br />

Environmental - using the <strong>art</strong> environment for educational or functional<br />

support or stimulation.<br />

Social/ using the social group for interaction, providing an<br />

developmental accessible activity, using behaviourial treatment in the group<br />

environment.<br />

Energy theories - suggestions that the patient can be affected by the power<br />

<strong>of</strong> the unconscious mind whilst engaged in <strong>art</strong>, and that the<br />

therapist can be a conductor <strong>of</strong> the power.<br />

Assessment - diagnostic drawings, using <strong>art</strong> as an initial interview with<br />

the patient.<br />

4. Author's personal explanation <strong>of</strong> how <strong>art</strong> promotes therapeutic change<br />

Signposts Patient illustrates their trauma or illness, objectifying it,<br />

allowing access for therapist.<br />

Self healing Art as healing in itself, or the therapist as a conductor for<br />

power from the unconscious.<br />

Group interaction - Observing behaviour <strong>of</strong> the group or <strong>of</strong> an individual whilst<br />

patient drawing.<br />

336


Insight New perception <strong>of</strong> the patient's problems through<br />

representation <strong>of</strong> self.<br />

Transfer <strong>of</strong> skill - Transfer <strong>of</strong> learning from one area to another. Thus patient<br />

may paint angry feelings and explore visually possibilities<br />

<strong>of</strong> containment which may be used practically later.<br />

Communication By visual presentation things patient finds difficult to say,<br />

increasing understanding through image.<br />

No explanation Author assumes agreement or no <strong>of</strong>fer <strong>of</strong> explanation.<br />

5. Any four benefits derived from <strong>art</strong>, for the patient, where the author mentioned<br />

these kind <strong>of</strong> words:<br />

Expression <strong>of</strong> feelings - cath<strong>art</strong>ic expression <strong>of</strong> feelings, representation <strong>of</strong><br />

the unconscious, psychic energy, expression <strong>of</strong><br />

trauma, self expression and projection.<br />

Development <strong>of</strong> a skill - social benefits, practical confidence through skill,<br />

problem solving strategies, self mastery, creative<br />

skill, record <strong>of</strong> progress (for the therapist).<br />

Communication Cuts through verbal defences, diagnostic,<br />

verbal/visual bridge, promotes understanding (<strong>of</strong><br />

the patient) and support.<br />

Self awareness self documenting, reality testing, promotes insight,<br />

promotes body awareness, male/female issues,<br />

cognitive gain.<br />

Symptom Relief healing, resolution <strong>of</strong> conflict, reduction in anxiety,<br />

promotes growth, remission <strong>of</strong> illness.<br />

Relationship - counter/transference, nonthreatening, friendly,<br />

alliance.<br />

6. Against - Observations <strong>of</strong> negative effect on the patient or the therapist.<br />

Resistance by patient, limitations <strong>of</strong> patient, withdrawal or<br />

confrontation.<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapist limitation - Method too powerful, limitations <strong>of</strong> the therapist,<br />

individual theory, projective guesses, respect for<br />

patient's interpretations.<br />

Standards Low status <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy.<br />

Patient approach Structure <strong>of</strong> the session wrong, aggression, frivolity<br />

and setting wrong.<br />

5 scales to categorise commonalities in 67 case studies with primary benefit <strong>of</strong><br />

expression <strong>of</strong> feelings; papers report illustrations <strong>of</strong> the images drawn by the client<br />

with some demographic and diagnostic information.<br />

Descriptive information: Age: adolescent (under 18); adult (18+ below 60);old (over 60)<br />

Sex: male, female, mixed (group). Diagnosis - as impressionistic studies.<br />

337


Orientation <strong>of</strong> the researcher, what were the intentions:<br />

Not known or stated<br />

Cognitive -<br />

Projective - projective methods, designed to elicit information about the<br />

patient's problems, interpreted pictures as representation <strong>of</strong> self.<br />

Occupational <strong>art</strong> as a stimulating or pleasurable activity for the patient.<br />

Analytical used psychoanalytic theory and language to interpret patient's<br />

painting in terms <strong>of</strong> archetypal imagery, universal symbols etc.<br />

(may not have been traditional or valid).<br />

Method <strong>of</strong> <strong>Study</strong>, description <strong>of</strong> the practical techniques <strong>of</strong> eliciting expressionistic work:<br />

Projective Used a projective test or protocol with some standard meanings<br />

for content.<br />

Psychoanalytic Interpreted using psychoanalytic language and concepts (may<br />

not<br />

have been valid or intelligable).<br />

Expressive Primarily eliciting emotional responses and individual expression.<br />

Occupational Used <strong>art</strong> as stimulation activity or educational/social activity.<br />

Comparitive Compared one painting or set with another on formal, content or<br />

other grounds directly.<br />

Form <strong>of</strong> <strong>Study</strong>, what type <strong>of</strong> material did the researcher extract from the study:<br />

Formal Objective or stylistic elements.<br />

Content analysis Interpretation <strong>of</strong> subjects <strong>of</strong> painting, <strong>of</strong> relationships<br />

between elements <strong>of</strong> painting as representing patient.<br />

Mix <strong>of</strong> content and style.<br />

Behaviour Analysis <strong>of</strong> how the patient behaved in the session,<br />

emotional, quiet etc.<br />

Verbal analysis Psychotherapy with the pictorial element as a backdrop.<br />

Other<br />

Benefit to the patient, through expression <strong>of</strong> feelings from the making <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>:<br />

Cath<strong>art</strong>ic/ Intense emotional sense <strong>of</strong><br />

reflective release with new insight.<br />

Communication Able to talk about or refer to emotional experience.<br />

Healing/symptom Patient feels better, disturbing<br />

relief behaviour declines.<br />

Developmental/ Patient behaves more emotionally<br />

social mature, and interacts socially.<br />

Relationship Patient draws comfort/value from the relationship with the<br />

therapist.<br />

13 scales to categorise Controlled Studies. Papers reported an empirical study <strong>of</strong> a psychiatric group with<br />

one or more normal or patient control groups.<br />

338


Demographic<br />

Sex, Age, Diagnosis all as cases.<br />

Number <strong>of</strong> subjects in experimental group<br />

Total no subjects in study<br />

No. judges in study: authors, other than authors.<br />

No. independent measures in this study<br />

Orientation - What the therapist was trying to achieve/ type <strong>of</strong> therapy <strong>of</strong>fered by the therapist.<br />

Comparison test only between groups<br />

Expressive To elicit expression <strong>of</strong> feelings through the <strong>art</strong>work.<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapeutic/ To help the patient solve problems or gain insight through<br />

self actualising rendition <strong>of</strong> experience through <strong>art</strong>.<br />

Behavioural/ Provide peaceful/friendly environment where patient feels<br />

non-<strong>art</strong> comfortable or can be active.<br />

Measurement method - what the instrument was designed to measure<br />

Direct Comparison <strong>of</strong> visual characteristics <strong>of</strong> the patient/control <strong>art</strong>work, e.g. circles<br />

or shapes or lines or colour.<br />

Meaning <strong>The</strong>rapist interprets patient's <strong>art</strong>work and compares meaning, e.g. happy or<br />

lonely.<br />

Mixed Mixed visual and interpreted characteristics.<br />

non-<strong>art</strong> Non-projective, non-<strong>art</strong> measure <strong>of</strong> behaviour, e.g. quiet, lively.<br />

verbal Verbal interactions, where therapist records quantity, or quality <strong>of</strong> language.<br />

Design <strong>of</strong> the study<br />

Pre and post intervention<br />

Post intervention only<br />

Direct comparison <strong>of</strong> current <strong>art</strong>work<br />

Retrospective comparison <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>work.<br />

Derivation <strong>of</strong> the main study measure<br />

One test used, developed for this study<br />

One test used, adapted from a previous test<br />

Established and validated test used<br />

Measure by observation <strong>of</strong> patient<br />

Measurement form, what type <strong>of</strong> elements <strong>of</strong> the picture was the measure actually sensitive to.<br />

Form observable visual detail, including DAP<br />

Content subjects or themes<br />

Mix <strong>of</strong> form and content<br />

Behaviour <strong>of</strong> subject<br />

Verbal elements explained by the subject verbally<br />

Expressive elements <strong>of</strong> the pictures which the therapist felt expressed the emotional tenor<br />

<strong>of</strong> the patient<br />

Results<br />

Difference in experimental group and control<br />

Difference in subtypes <strong>of</strong> the same diagnostic group<br />

No difference<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy groups (effect <strong>of</strong> therapy recognised)<br />

No association between score and criterion, inconsistent<br />

339


Validity Analysis <strong>of</strong> controlled studies. 70 studies reported criterion validity <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> test.<br />

5 fields were tested.<br />

Demographic<br />

Sex, Age and Diagnosis as before.<br />

No. <strong>of</strong> criterion measures - Diagnosis and test = 1, any more added on.<br />

Form <strong>of</strong> Art Test - Form <strong>of</strong> information the <strong>art</strong> test collected.<br />

KFD Emotional indicators.<br />

DAP Cognitive/Developmental indicators<br />

Formal other<br />

Copy Copy drawing test measuring Control<br />

Other theme<br />

Free Spontaneous <strong>art</strong>work no theme/instruction<br />

Form <strong>of</strong> criterion measure.<br />

Diagnosis<br />

Picture or self Objective visual (DAP) or descriptive.<br />

report<br />

Content or story Meaning <strong>of</strong> visual image or narrative.<br />

Mix content/form<br />

Non projective Non-<strong>art</strong> measure; I.Q. etc.<br />

behavioral;<br />

Verbal Patient describes experience <strong>art</strong>iculately.<br />

Comparison: the type <strong>of</strong> quality the instruments were compared on.<br />

Cognitive function Ability or intelligence level.<br />

Emotional function Expressive assessment.<br />

Development According to set criteria.<br />

Diagnosis<br />

Statistics:<br />

Percentage agreement<br />

Correlation coefficient<br />

Mann-Whitney or _ 2 (non parametric association)<br />

Kappa<br />

Analysis <strong>of</strong> variance<br />

Results: association <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong> test with the main criterion measure<br />

Differentiation among grades<br />

Differentiation towards objective elements<br />

Differentiation towards subjective elements<br />

Significant association no direction<br />

Test did not match or no significant association.<br />

340


Standard differences from the mean for DAPA variables from the results section, Chapter 5.<br />

6a. Standard differences from mean <strong>of</strong> each diagnostic group<br />

PLOT OF TRED<br />

________________________________________<br />

_ 1 _<br />

_ 1 1 _<br />

.4_ 1 _<br />

_ 1 1 1 1 _<br />

_ 4 1 2 4 1 _<br />

_ 3 3 1 5 1 _<br />

R _ 7 8 5 4 _<br />

E 0___7________3_______1_______2________1__<br />

D _ 1 2 2 4 _<br />

_ 1 2 1 _<br />

_ 3 3 3 _<br />

_ 1 4 1 _<br />

-.4_ 5 _<br />

_ 2 _<br />

_ 3 _<br />

________________________________________<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

Diagnostic group<br />

6b. Standard differences from mean each diagnostic group<br />

PLOT OF YELLOW<br />

________________________________________<br />

_ 1 _<br />

_ _<br />

_ 1 _<br />

.35_ 3 1 1 _<br />

_ 5 3 2 _<br />

_ 4 2 1 3 1 _<br />

Y _ 1 3 4 5 2 _<br />

E _ 3 5 2 1 2 _<br />

L 0___3________2_______2_______1 ________2__<br />

L _ 2 2 1 1 _<br />

O _ 3 2 2 3 _<br />

W _ 1 2 1 1 _<br />

_ 10 1 1 _<br />

-.35_ 3 _<br />

_ 2 1 _<br />

_ 4 _<br />

________________________________________<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

Diagnostic group<br />

6c. Standard differences from mean <strong>of</strong> each diagnostic group<br />

PLOT OF GREEN<br />

________________________________________<br />

.75_ _<br />

_ 1 _<br />

_ _<br />

_ 1 _<br />

_ 2 1 _<br />

G .375_ 1 1 1 _<br />

R _ 1 2 1 1 1 _<br />

E _ 2 1 1 2 1 _<br />

E _ 4 2 1 3 _<br />

N _ 2 2 3 4 _<br />

0___2________3_______1_______3________2__<br />

_ 4 6 3 1 4 _<br />

_ 9 7 1 1 _<br />

_ 7 4 1 1 _<br />

_ 2 _<br />

-.375_ 5 _<br />

________________________________________<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

Diagnostic group<br />

6d. Standard differences from mean <strong>of</strong> each diagnostic group<br />

PLOT OF BLUE<br />

________________________________________<br />

_ 2 1 _<br />

341


_ 2 _<br />

_ _<br />

_ 2 _<br />

.3_ 1 1 _<br />

B _ 2 1 2 _<br />

L _ 2 1 2 _<br />

U _ 3 1 2 1 2 _<br />

E _ 3 8 2 4 _<br />

0___7________________3_______2________1__<br />

_ 4 6 2 3 1 _<br />

_ 4 3 2 1 _<br />

_ 2 2 1 _<br />

_ 7 4 2 2 1 _<br />

-.3_ _<br />

_ 4 _<br />

________________________________________<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

Diagnostic group<br />

6e. Standard differences from mean <strong>of</strong> each diagnostic group<br />

PLOT OF BROWN<br />

________________________________________<br />

_ _<br />

_ 1 _<br />

_ 1 _<br />

_ 1 1 2 _<br />

.325_ 1 1 3 2 _<br />

_ 1 1 1 2 _<br />

B _ 4 1 1 1 1 _<br />

R _ 4 6 2 2 1 _<br />

O _ 5 2 1 2 3 _<br />

W 0___1________3_______3_______3________2__<br />

N _ 1 2 1 2 _<br />

_ 1 1 _<br />

_ 1 1 _<br />

_ 14 6 1 _<br />

-.325_ 6 1 _<br />

_ 5 _<br />

________________________________________<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

Diagnostic group<br />

6f. Standard differences from mean <strong>of</strong> each diagnostic group<br />

PLOT OF BLACK<br />

________________________________________<br />

_ _<br />

_ 1 1 _<br />

_ 1 2 _<br />

.35_ 2 2 2 1 _<br />

_ 2 2 1 _<br />

B _ 2 3 2 1 1 _<br />

L _ 3 6 2 1 1 _<br />

A _ 8 5 1 1 1 _<br />

C 0___1________1_______1_______4________1__<br />

K _ 2 3 3 1 _<br />

_ 5 2 1 1 _<br />

_ 5 2 1 1 _<br />

_ 4 2 1 1 _<br />

-.35_ 1 1 1 _<br />

_ 1 1 _<br />

_ 1 1 4 _<br />

________________________________________<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

Diagnostic group<br />

342


g. Standard differences from mean <strong>of</strong> each diagnostic group<br />

PLOT OF COLOUR INTENSITY<br />

________________________________________<br />

I _ _<br />

N _ 1 _<br />

T .8_ 3 1 1 _<br />

E _ 3 1 _<br />

N _ 3 1 3 5 _<br />

S _ 2 2 2 3 _<br />

I _ 4 4 3 5 2 _<br />

T 0___6________6_______3_______1________2__<br />

Y _ 5 6 1 1 _<br />

_ 1 3 2 4 _<br />

_ 3 1 1 _<br />

_ 2 1 1 2 _<br />

-.8_ 1 2 1 _<br />

_ 1 _<br />

_ 2 1 _<br />

_ _<br />

________________________________________<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

Diagnostic group<br />

6h. Standard differences from mean <strong>of</strong> each diagnostic group<br />

PLOT OF PAINTED LINE<br />

________________________________________<br />

_ 1 _<br />

_ 1 _<br />

_ 1 _<br />

_ 3 1 1 _<br />

.6_ 3 1 3 _<br />

P _ 6 2 1 4 1 _<br />

A _ 1 1 1 1 _<br />

I _ 3 5 1 _<br />

N _ 2 1 2 _<br />

T 0___3________1_______3________________2__<br />

E _ 2 1 1 1 _<br />

D _ 13 2 _<br />

_ 4 1 1 _<br />

L _ 3 _<br />

I -.6_ 12 5 _<br />

N _ 7 _<br />

E ________________________________________<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

Diagnostic group<br />

6i. Standard differences from mean <strong>of</strong> each diagnostic group<br />

PLOT OF DRAWN LINE<br />

________________________________________<br />

_ _<br />

4_ 1 _<br />

_ _<br />

_ _<br />

_ 1 _<br />

_ 1 _<br />

D 2_ 1 3 _<br />

R _ 1 1 2 _<br />

A _ 2 2 1 _<br />

W _ 2 1 3 4 1 _<br />

N _ 2 5 1 2 _<br />

0___5________3_______4________________1__<br />

L _ 6 4 4 15 _<br />

I _ 14 6 4 _<br />

N _ 2 4 _<br />

E _ _<br />

________________________________________<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

Diagnostic group<br />

343


6j. Standard differences from mean <strong>of</strong> each diagnostic group<br />

PLOT OF SPACE IN PICTURE<br />

________________________________________<br />

S _ _<br />

P _ 1 _<br />

A 1_ 1 _<br />

C _ 2 3 1 _<br />

E _ 2 1 2 _<br />

_ 3 4 2 3 _<br />

_ 6 6 2 5 4 _<br />

0__10________3_______1_______2________3__<br />

_ 3 3 3 3 1 _<br />

_ 6 3 3 5 _<br />

_ 1 2 1 _<br />

_ 1 2 2 _<br />

-1_ 1 1 _<br />

_ _<br />

_ 1 _<br />

_ _<br />

________________________________________<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

Diagnostic group<br />

6k. Standard differences from mean <strong>of</strong> each diagnostic group<br />

PLOT OF SUBJECTIVE EMOTIONAL TONE<br />

________________________________________<br />

E _ 1 _<br />

M _ _<br />

O _ 1 _<br />

T .6_ 1 2 1 _<br />

I _ 1 3 _<br />

O _ 3 5 1 1 _<br />

N _ 4 2 3 2 2 _<br />

A _ 6 2 3 2 1 _<br />

L 0___5________1_______3_______5_______1___<br />

_ 10 4 6 3 _<br />

T _ 1 2 1 1 _<br />

O _ 2 1 4 1 _<br />

N _ 1 3 1 1 _<br />

E -.6_ 1 _<br />

_ 2 _<br />

_ 1 1 _<br />

________________________________________<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

Diagnostic group<br />

6l. Standard differences from mean <strong>of</strong> each diagnostic group<br />

PLOT OF DOMINANT FORM<br />

________________________________________<br />

_ 1 _<br />

_ _<br />

.35_ 1 1 _<br />

_ 2 2 1 1 1 _<br />

_ 1 1 2 _<br />

_ 5 3 2 3 1 _<br />

F _ 8 6 3 4 1 _<br />

O 0___6________5_______4_______1________3__<br />

R _ 7 5 1 3 2 _<br />

M _ 1 3 _<br />

_ 2 5 2 _<br />

_ _<br />

-.35_ 3 _<br />

_ 3 1 _<br />

_ _<br />

_ 2 _<br />

________________________________________<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

Diagnostic group<br />

344


7a. Standard differences from mean <strong>of</strong> each diagnostic group<br />

PLOT OF ORANGE<br />

________________________________________<br />

_ _<br />

.4_ 1 _<br />

_ 1 _<br />

_ 2 _<br />

O _ _<br />

R _ 2 1 _<br />

A .2_ 1 2 _<br />

N _ 2 1 3 _<br />

G _ 2 1 _<br />

E _ 2 3 1 _<br />

_ 3 2 1 3 _<br />

0___6________8_______3________________2__<br />

_ 3 9 1 2 _<br />

_ 16 9 1 1 _<br />

_ 10 3 _<br />

_ 1 _<br />

________________________________________<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

Diagnostic group<br />

7b. Standard differences from mean <strong>of</strong> each diagnostic group<br />

PLOT OF PURPLE<br />

________________________________________<br />

_ _<br />

.45_ 1 _<br />

_ _<br />

_ _<br />

P _ 1 _<br />

U _ 1 1 _<br />

R .225_ 1 1 _<br />

P _ 3 1 1 1 _<br />

L _ 2 1 2 _<br />

E _ 3 2 2 _<br />

_ 4 1 1 1 1 _<br />

0___1________7_______2_______2___________<br />

_ 10 14 11 2 1 _<br />

_ 12 2 3 _<br />

_ 1 2 _<br />

_ 7 _<br />

________________________________________<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

Diagnostic group<br />

7c. Standard differences from mean <strong>of</strong> each diagnostic group<br />

PLOT OF WHITE<br />

________________________________________<br />

_ _<br />

_ 1 _<br />

.35_ _<br />

_ _<br />

_ _<br />

W _ 1 _<br />

H _ 1 _<br />

I .175_ 1 2 _<br />

T _ 1 1 _<br />

E _ 4 1 1 _<br />

_ 3 1 1 1 _<br />

_ 1 2 1 _<br />

0___1________1______17________________7 _<br />

_ 25 19 1 _<br />

_ 14 _<br />

_ _<br />

________________________________________<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

Diagnostic group<br />

345


Correlation table 16 for DAPA variables measured from the pictures <strong>of</strong> Schizophrenics: a. colours, and b. structure<br />

DAPA variables 1. Pearson's Product Moment Correlation Analysis<br />

N <strong>of</strong> cases: 35 2-tailed Signif: * .01 # .001<br />

Red<br />

Yellow<br />

Green<br />

Blue<br />

Brown<br />

Black<br />

Intensity<br />

Line-paint<br />

Line-draw<br />

Space<br />

Em-Tone<br />

Form<br />

Red Yellow Green Blue Brown Black<br />

1.0000 .3604 .4498* .3409 .2366 -.2392<br />

.3604 1.0000 .7880# .5083* .5327# .0984<br />

.4498* .7880# 1.0000 .6074# .6303# -.0796<br />

.3409 .5083* .6074# 1.0000 .2706 .0213<br />

.2366 .5327# .6303# .2706 1.0000 -.0111<br />

-.2392 .0984 -.0796 .0213 -.0111 1.0000<br />

.5791# .6586# .6732# .5751# .3489 .2365<br />

.4853* .3902 .4364* .4036 .0879 -.2120<br />

.1772 .2616 .2699 .2713 .3247 .0383<br />

.2521 -.0318 .0166 .0264 .1028 .0745<br />

.0116 .1564 .0404 .1680 -.0354 .0151<br />

.2649 -.0718 -.1720 .1975 -.2605 .0271<br />

DAPA variables 2. Pearson's Product Moment Correlation Analysis<br />

N <strong>of</strong> cases 35 2-tailed sig *.01 #.001<br />

Red<br />

Yellow<br />

Green<br />

Blue<br />

Brown<br />

Black<br />

Intensity<br />

Line-paint<br />

Line-draw<br />

Space<br />

Em-Tone<br />

Form<br />

Intensity Line-pnt Line-draw S pace Em-Tone Form<br />

.5791# .4853* .1772 .2521 .0116 .2649<br />

.6586# .3902 .2616 -.0318 .1564 -.0718<br />

.6732# .4364* .2699 .0166 .0404 -.1720<br />

.5751# .4036 .2713 .0264 .1680 .1975<br />

.3489 .0879 .3247 .1028 -.0354 -.2605<br />

.2365 -.2120 .0383 .0745 .0151 .0271<br />

1.0000 .3566 .3319 .4098 .3551 .1667<br />

.3566 1.0000 -.4563* .1008 .1048 .0714<br />

.3319 -.4563* 1.0000 .1408 .0047 .1381<br />

.4098 .1008 .1408 1.0000 .4216 .1353<br />

.3551 .1048 .0047 .4216 1.0000 .2572<br />

.1667 .0714 .1381 .1353 .2572 1.0000<br />

Correlation table 17 for DAPA variables measured from the pictures <strong>of</strong> Substance Abusers: a. colours, and b. structure<br />

DAPA variables 1. Pearson's Product Moment Correlation Analysis<br />

N <strong>of</strong> cases: 24 2-tailed Signif: * .01 # .001<br />

Red<br />

Yellow<br />

Green<br />

Blue<br />

Brown<br />

Black<br />

Intensity<br />

Line-paint<br />

Line-draw<br />

Space<br />

Em-Tone<br />

Form<br />

Red Yellow Green Blue Brown Black<br />

1.0000 .1202 .1591 .0345 .2509 -.0975<br />

.1202 1.0000 .2480 -.2749 .3064 .0492<br />

.1591 .2480 1.0000 .3457 .6377# -.0064<br />

.0345 -.2749 .3457 1.0000 -.0055 -.5103<br />

.2509 .3064 .6377# -.0055 1.0000 .1927<br />

-.0975 .0492 -.0064 -.5103 .1927 1.0000<br />

.2696 .2968 -.0543 -.3901 .3358 .2510<br />

.2723 .0004 .3538 .0279 .3176 -.0047<br />

.0087 -.4361 -.1727 -.3542 .0441 .3803<br />

-.0542 -.1900 -.5125 -.1555 -.3847 -.0569<br />

.3369 .3586 .3802 .1277 .3853 -.2912<br />

-.0218 .1279 -.5107 -.7608# -.1848 .3760<br />

DAPA variables 2. Pearson's Product Moment Correlation Analysis<br />

N <strong>of</strong> cases 24 2-tailed sig *.01 #.001<br />

Red<br />

Yellow<br />

Green<br />

Blue<br />

Brown<br />

Black<br />

Intensity<br />

Line-paint<br />

Line-draw<br />

Space<br />

Em-Tone<br />

Form<br />

Intensity Line-paint Ln-draw Space Em-Tone Form<br />

.2696 .2723 .0087 -.0542 .3369 -.0218<br />

.2968 .0004 -.4361 -.1900 .3586 .1279<br />

-.0543 .3538 -.1727 -.5125 .3802 -.5107<br />

-.3901 .0279 -.3542 -.1555 .1277 -.7608#<br />

.3358 .3176 .0441 -.3847 .3853 -.1848<br />

.2510 -.0047 .3803 -.0569 -.2912 .3760<br />

1.0000 .2961 .0519 -.2999 -.1355 .3715<br />

.2961 1.0000 .0461 -.4074 -.0350 .0193<br />

.0519 .0461 1.0000 .2736 -.0167 .1786<br />

-.2999 -.4074 .2736 1.0000 .1562 -.0363<br />

-.1355 -.0350 -.0167 .1562 1.0000 -.4231<br />

.3715 .0193 .1786 -.0363 -.4231 1.0000<br />

346


Correlation table 18 for DAPA variables measured from the pictures <strong>of</strong> Depressives: a. colours, and b. structure<br />

DAPA variables Pearson's Product Moment Correlation Analysis<br />

N <strong>of</strong> cases: 18 2-tailed Signif: * .01 # .001<br />

Red<br />

Yellow<br />

Green<br />

Blue<br />

Brown<br />

Black<br />

Intensity<br />

Line-paint<br />

Line-draw<br />

Space<br />

Em-Tone<br />

Form<br />

Red Yellow Green Blue Brown Black<br />

1.0000 .2190 -.2439 .4108 .3544 -.5008<br />

.2190 1.0000 .5233 .0933 .0456 .0773<br />

-.2439 .5233 1.0000 .2061 -.1223 .6256*<br />

.4108 .0933 .2061 1.0000 .3385 -.0127<br />

.3544 .0456 -.1223 .3385 1.0000 -.1375<br />

-.5008 .0773 .6256* -.0127 -.1375 1.0000<br />

.2665 .5996* .6132* .0600 -.1543 .3504<br />

.4585 .3449 .1690 .2415 .0960 -.0436<br />

-.3769 -.0852 .2575 -.2248 -.0563 .5134<br />

.3684 -.1372 -.4742 -.1645 .1790 -.1690<br />

-.1489 -.3002 .0787 .0370 .0386 .2029<br />

.2613 -.2483 -.4253 -.1762 .5708 -.2897<br />

DAPA variables Pearson's Product Moment Correlation Analysis<br />

N <strong>of</strong> cases: 18 2-tailed sig *.01 #.001<br />

Red<br />

Yellow<br />

Green<br />

Blue<br />

Brown<br />

Black<br />

Intensity<br />

Line-paint<br />

Line-draw<br />

Space<br />

Em-Tone<br />

Form<br />

Intensity Line-pnt Line-draw Space Em-Tone Form<br />

.2665 .4585 -.3769 .3684 -.1489 .2613<br />

.5996* .3449 -.0852 -.1372 -.3002 -.2483<br />

.6132* .1690 .2575 -.4742 .0787 -.4253<br />

.0600 .2415 -.2248 -.1645 .0370 -.1762<br />

-.1543 .0960 -.0563 .1790 .0386 .5708<br />

.3504 -.0436 .5134 -.1690 .2029 -.2897<br />

1.0000 .5619 .0912 .1631 -.1758 -.1220<br />

.5619 1.0000 -.6944* .1272 -.0563 .1079<br />

.0912 -.6944* 1.0000 .1170 -.0541 -.1363<br />

.1631 .1272 .1170 1.0000 .0990 .4917<br />

-.1758 -.0563 -.0541 .0990 1.0000 .1832<br />

-.1220 .1079 -.1363 .4917 .1832 1.0000<br />

Correlation table 19 for DAPA variables measured from the pictures <strong>of</strong> Controls: a. colours, and b. structure<br />

DAPA variables 1. Pearson's Product Moment Correlation Analysis<br />

N <strong>of</strong> cases: 23 2-tailed Signif: * .01 # .001<br />

Red<br />

Yellow<br />

Green<br />

Blue<br />

Brown<br />

Black<br />

Intensity<br />

Line-paint<br />

Line-draw<br />

Space<br />

Em-Tone<br />

Form<br />

Red Yellow Green Blue Brown Black<br />

1.0000 .5257* .3993 .2524 .0536 -.1223<br />

.5257* 1.0000 .4753 .2503 .0811 -.3953<br />

.3993 .4753 1.0000 .4316 .3416 -.4588<br />

.2524 .2503 .4316 1.0000 -.1899 -.3905<br />

.0536 .0811 .3416 -.1899 1.0000 .0919<br />

.1223 -.3953 -.4588 -.3905 .0919 1.0000<br />

.6164* .4570 .3484 .3894 .1098 .1566<br />

.4431 .0635 .2293 -.1029 .2745 .1564<br />

-.0201 .0065 -.1370 .1684 -.2170 -.0372<br />

-.0202 .0456 -.0338 -.2078 .0489 .0488<br />

.1988 .3496 .0326 -.0338 .0822 -.1170<br />

.2761 .0479 .0446 .4472 -.0955 .1224<br />

DAPA variables 2. Pearson's Product Moment Correlation Analysis<br />

N <strong>of</strong> cases 23 2-tailed sig *.01 #.001<br />

Red<br />

Yellow<br />

Green<br />

Blue<br />

Brown<br />

Black<br />

Intensity<br />

Line-paint<br />

Line-draw<br />

Space<br />

Em-Tone<br />

Form<br />

Intensity Line-pnt Line-draw Space Em-Tone Form<br />

.6164* .4431 -.0201 -.0202 .1988 .2761<br />

.4570 .0635 .0065 .0456 .3496 .0479<br />

.3484 .2293 -.1370 -.0338 .0326 .0446<br />

.3894 -.1029 .1684 -.2078 -.0338 .4472<br />

.1098 .2745 -.2170 .0489 .0822 -.0955<br />

.1566 .1564 -.0372 .0488 -.1170 .1224<br />

1.0000 .5254 -.2455 .1009 .3627 .3111<br />

.5254 1.0000 -.6305* -.0161 .2395 -.0204<br />

-.2455 -.6305* 1.0000 .2629 -.1978 .1931<br />

.1009 -.0161 .2629 1.0000 .1603 -.1115<br />

.3627 .2395 -.1978 .1603 1.0000 .1114<br />

.3111 -.0204 .1931 -.1115 .1114 1.0000<br />

347


Correlation table 20 for DAPA variables measured from the pictures <strong>of</strong> Patients with Personality Disorder: a. colours, and b.<br />

structure<br />

DAPA variables 1. Pearson's Product Moment Correlation Analysis<br />

N <strong>of</strong> cases: 9 2-tailed Signif: * .01 # .001<br />

Red<br />

Yellow<br />

Green<br />

Blue<br />

Brown<br />

Black<br />

Intensity<br />

Line-paint<br />

Line-draw<br />

Space<br />

Em-Tone<br />

Form<br />

Red Yellow Green Blue Brown Black<br />

1.0000 .4291 .1037 .2665 .5060 -.7479<br />

.4291 1.0000 .0803 .6943 .6450 -.3775<br />

.1037 .0803 1.0000 .5832 .6699 -.1882<br />

.2665 .6943 .5832 1.0000 .6532 -.4694<br />

.5060 .6450 .6699 .6532 1.0000 -.5087<br />

-.7479 -.3775 -.1882 -.4694 -.5087 1.0000<br />

.0062 .4293 .0112 .5181 .1394 -.4951<br />

.1697 .2417 -.1492 .2956 .1385 -.5908<br />

.0238 -.0723 .5020 .0131 .2875 .3169<br />

-.1395 .0427 -.6277 -.3151 -.2847 .2442<br />

-.2327 .5180 .3585 .6741 .5307 -.2249<br />

.1801 -.1023 .2526 -.2675 .1488 .1142<br />

DAPA variables 2. Pearson's Product Moment Correlation Analysis<br />

N <strong>of</strong> cases 9 2-tailed sig *.01 #.001<br />

Red<br />

Yellow<br />

Green<br />

Blue<br />

Brown<br />

Black<br />

Intensity<br />

Line-paint<br />

Line-draw<br />

Space<br />

Em-Tone<br />

Form<br />

Intensity Line-pnt Line-draw Space Em-Tone Form<br />

.0062 .1697 .0238 -.1395 -.2327 .1801<br />

.4293 .2417 -.0723 .0427 .5180 -.1023<br />

.0112 -.1492 .5020 -.6277 .3585 .2526<br />

.5181 .2956 .0131 -.3151 .6741 -.2675<br />

.1394 .1385 .2875 -.2847 .5307 .1488<br />

-.4951 -.5908 .3169 .2442 -.2249 .1142<br />

1.0000 .6197 -.6751 -.1665 .4921 -.2190<br />

.6197 1.0000 -.8182* .4452 .3427 -.6997<br />

-.6751 -.8182* 1.0000 -.4715 -.0352 .5385<br />

-.1665 .4452 -.4715 1.0000 -.1114 -.6429<br />

.4921 .3427 -.0352 -.1114 1.0000 -.3661<br />

-.2190 -.6997 .5385 -.6429 -.3661 1.0000<br />

348


Appendix 2: Tables summarising raw data from the text <strong>of</strong> the thesis.<br />

Chapter 2<br />

Table 3: Summaries <strong>of</strong> impressionistic studies <strong>of</strong> or about <strong>art</strong>ists.<br />

Table 4: Summaries <strong>of</strong> papers reporting changes or signs in the theme/content <strong>of</strong> the<br />

<strong>art</strong>work.<br />

Table 5: Summaries <strong>of</strong> papers reporting general signs <strong>of</strong> psychiatric disturbance.<br />

Table 6: Summaries <strong>of</strong> papers reporting changes or signs in the form <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong>work.<br />

Table 7: Summaries <strong>of</strong> papers reporting the most important feature <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy was<br />

the therapeutic relationship.<br />

Table 8: Summaries <strong>of</strong> papers reporting behaviour changes for case studies.<br />

Crosstabulations 1-6: contingency tables for Chi-square calculation to indicate<br />

associations between method <strong>of</strong> study, form <strong>of</strong> expression and benefits for categorised<br />

commonalities <strong>of</strong> 67 studies which claimed 'expression <strong>of</strong> feelings' as main benefit for <strong>art</strong><br />

therapy.<br />

Reliability <strong>Study</strong> Chapter 4 - Methods. Tables and Plots 1-15.<br />

Tables 1-15 by variable showing: column 1, Raw data scores for each variable each rater<br />

( r1-7) over 7 pictures in sequence.<br />

Column 2, scoring differences between 6 raters and author on 7 rated pictures.<br />

Column 3, mean differences for raters 1-6 and standard difference to rater 7 (author).<br />

Plots 1-15 by variable showing individual rater differences around average rating <strong>of</strong> 6<br />

independent raters and author.<br />

Results from <strong>Study</strong> 2.<br />

Tables and Figures 1a-i showing abnormal distribution <strong>of</strong> DAPA variables which were<br />

transformed or deleted.<br />

Tables and Figures 2a-f showing normal or near normal distribution <strong>of</strong> DAPA variables<br />

which were left in their original state.<br />

Tables and Figures 3a-f showing normal distribution <strong>of</strong> DAPA variables after<br />

transformation.<br />

Figures 6a-1 showing standard differences from the mean for each diagnostic group for<br />

DAPA variables with normal distribution.<br />

Figures 7a-c showing standard differences from the mean for each diagnostic group for<br />

DAPA variables which were dropped because <strong>of</strong> infrequent use.<br />

Correlation tables 16-20 for each diagnostic group for 12 DAPA variables.<br />

Correlation table 16 for DAPA variables measured from the pictures <strong>of</strong> Schizophrenics:<br />

a. colours, and b. structure<br />

Correlation table 17 for DAPA variables measured from the pictures <strong>of</strong> Substance<br />

Abusers: a. colours, and b. structure<br />

Correlation table 18 for DAPA variables measured from the pictures <strong>of</strong> Depressives: a.<br />

colours, and b. structure<br />

349


Correlation table 19 for DAPA variables measured from the pictures <strong>of</strong> Controls: a.<br />

colours, and b. structure.<br />

Correlation table 20 for DAPA variables measured from the pictures <strong>of</strong> Patients with<br />

Personality Disorder: a. colours, and b. structure<br />

Chapter 2<br />

Table 3: Summaries <strong>of</strong> impressionistic studies <strong>of</strong> or about <strong>art</strong>ists.<br />

Table 4: Summaries <strong>of</strong> papers reporting changes or signs in the theme/content <strong>of</strong> the<br />

<strong>art</strong>work.<br />

Table 5: Summaries <strong>of</strong> papers reporting general signs <strong>of</strong> psychiatric disturbance.<br />

Table 6: Summaries <strong>of</strong> papers reporting changes or signs in the form <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong>work.<br />

Table 7: Summaries <strong>of</strong> papers reporting the most important feature <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy was<br />

the therapeutic relationship.<br />

Table 8: Summaries <strong>of</strong> papers reporting behaviour changes for case studies.<br />

Crosstabulations 1-6: contingency tables for Chi-square calculation to indicate<br />

associations between method <strong>of</strong> study, form <strong>of</strong> expression and benefits for categorised<br />

commonalities <strong>of</strong> 67 studies from the literature which claimed 'expression <strong>of</strong> feelings' as<br />

main benefit for <strong>art</strong> therapy.<br />

350


Impressionistic studies, TABLE 3 : summarising studies <strong>of</strong> or about <strong>art</strong>ists, common concepts and assumptions<br />

about their work.<br />

Artist or subject Interpretation <strong>of</strong> picture or <strong>art</strong>work, signs and relation <strong>of</strong> work<br />

to psychiatric symptoms<br />

1 Similarities between<br />

schizophrenic <strong>art</strong>, surrealism<br />

and tribal <strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> Australia and<br />

Africa<br />

2 Spontaneous <strong>art</strong> contains<br />

symbols and messages whose<br />

meaning may be more relevant<br />

than <strong>art</strong>ist realises.<br />

3 Depression and <strong>art</strong><br />

psychohygienic effect <strong>of</strong><br />

exorcising own suicidal<br />

impulses and thoughts<br />

4 Aesthetic and psychological<br />

conflicts in work <strong>of</strong> pr<strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong><br />

student 4 yrs. therapy<br />

5 Similarities and differences in<br />

the paintings <strong>of</strong> action painters<br />

and schizophrenics. <strong>The</strong> <strong>art</strong>ist<br />

deliberately attempts to<br />

relinquish intellectual control in<br />

order to express inner drives in<br />

the freest manner possible.<br />

Many mental patients paintings<br />

unconsciously expressed in the<br />

same way.<br />

6 Outsider <strong>art</strong>, individuals outside<br />

fine <strong>art</strong>s circles and outside<br />

society.<br />

7 73 yrs male <strong>art</strong>ist agnosia<br />

following occipital cerebral<br />

vascular accident - memory<br />

disturbance but essentially<br />

intact linguistic and cognitive<br />

capacities, effects on <strong>art</strong>ists<br />

capacity to draw examined<br />

8 Argued that quality <strong>of</strong><br />

creativity in paintings by<br />

Munch not compromised by his<br />

hospitalisation.<br />

9 Charles Filiger's work<br />

represents characteristic traits<br />

<strong>of</strong> schizophrenic psychosis.<br />

Exceptional man sparse<br />

biographical data, total isolation<br />

from world amid persecution<br />

delirium ending in suicide.<br />

10 Describes 12 collections <strong>of</strong><br />

psychiatric <strong>art</strong><br />

11 Rembrandt. Photo electric lux<br />

measurements from<br />

reproductions<br />

12 Joan Miro. Explores the<br />

relation <strong>of</strong> Miro's spiritual<br />

Understood in terms <strong>of</strong> phytogenetic and ontogenetic<br />

development <strong>of</strong> the personality structure<br />

Stimulates individual expression and interpersonal reaction,<br />

example van Gogh.<br />

Reactivates the nondominant hemisphere <strong>of</strong> the brain,<br />

p<strong>art</strong>icularly in chronic or recurrent depressions also opens up<br />

new perspectives for the solution <strong>of</strong> the problems that drive to<br />

depression, example Goethe<br />

351<br />

type <strong>of</strong> study<br />

discussion<br />

theory<br />

theory<br />

Identity conflicts. Description. case<br />

3 assumptions - motivation <strong>of</strong> patient is compulsive, <strong>art</strong>ist makes<br />

free choice; patient painting is incoherent and random, <strong>art</strong>ist has<br />

periods <strong>of</strong> emotional detachment/ intellectual consideration <strong>of</strong><br />

work; the <strong>art</strong> final product is an integrated whole, but not<br />

patient's.<br />

Because many practitioners emotionally disturbed, some<br />

generalisation <strong>of</strong> conclusions to all mental patients<br />

Despite inability to recognise scene or object subject was able to<br />

copy the display well. When displays were recognised or if<br />

given name <strong>of</strong> object, no effect shown. Postmorbid drawings<br />

similar to early works, but features such as insufficient<br />

differentiation <strong>of</strong> figure from ground, over elaboration <strong>of</strong> detail<br />

and areas <strong>of</strong> neglect revealed effect.<br />

Middle period <strong>of</strong> Munch best known previously repressed drive<br />

derivatives found full expression in his imagery. At the end <strong>of</strong><br />

this period disruptive personal factors contributed to the failure<br />

<strong>of</strong> his defensive structures. Psychiatric intervention prevented<br />

further <strong>psychopathology</strong> and led to an altered <strong>art</strong>istic focus.<br />

A painter's psychosis may stimulate an original creation, valid in<br />

its own right as a work <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>. Effects <strong>of</strong> disorder not<br />

necessarily destructive and work may go beyond the personal<br />

problems <strong>of</strong> the creator and acquire meaning for spectators.<br />

Concept <strong>of</strong> '<strong>psychopathology</strong> <strong>of</strong> expression' represents a<br />

distortion <strong>of</strong> truth in whatever the <strong>art</strong>ists inner experiences<br />

were, their expressive faculty remained intact or grew stronger<br />

to encompass them<br />

Differences in light and darkness in Rembrandt's self portraits<br />

would reflect his mood (p<strong>art</strong>icularly depression) in conformity<br />

with various life events. Results were inconclusive.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re seems a connection between feelings <strong>of</strong> despair and<br />

inner torment and the evolution <strong>of</strong> his <strong>art</strong>. Depressed state<br />

speculation<br />

discussion<br />

case<br />

case<br />

case<br />

discussion<br />

case<br />

case


eliefs to depression. cannot be matched to specific <strong>art</strong>works, yet the author feels it is<br />

revealed in many paintings.<br />

13 Picasso and pathology <strong>of</strong><br />

cubism.<br />

Represents less stylistic or formal revolution than p<strong>art</strong>ially<br />

successful attempt to come to terms with certain conflicts.<br />

Unwittingly therapeutic, gave him psychological control.<br />

14 Modigliani's works Illustrates expression <strong>of</strong> psychodynamic forces, principally<br />

resistance against emotional closeness and indications <strong>of</strong><br />

superego pathology. Paintings exemplify defense mechanisms<br />

which dealt with anxiety.<br />

15 Van Gogh, diagnosed by<br />

Gachet as melancholic.<br />

16 Goya - reviews the literature<br />

on illness and dynamic<br />

interpretations.<br />

17 Mondrian. Determines<br />

connection between the man<br />

and the <strong>art</strong>.<br />

18 Compares schizo-phreniform<br />

symptoms, aspects <strong>of</strong> modern<br />

<strong>art</strong> and literature, to emphasise<br />

the defensive /disruptive role <strong>of</strong><br />

forms <strong>of</strong> self-consciousness<br />

(pathological reflexivity).<br />

19 M. Ramirez, little known<br />

Mexican final 25 years <strong>of</strong><br />

severe psychosis.<br />

20 Correlation between early life<br />

<strong>of</strong> Munch and subsequent<br />

psychodynamics as revealed in<br />

his life and <strong>art</strong>.<br />

21 Jochen Seidel<br />

Psychotic episodes<br />

22 Prevalence <strong>of</strong> mood an mind<br />

disorders in 14 modern abstract<br />

expressionist <strong>art</strong>ists who, using<br />

psychic automatism to reveal<br />

unconscious material, created<br />

psychologically and spiritually<br />

significant <strong>art</strong> that addressed<br />

mythic themes <strong>of</strong> creation,<br />

birth, life and death.<br />

23 Mark Gertler<br />

Viccitudes <strong>of</strong> his career,<br />

depression and suicide with<br />

reference to primal scene<br />

trauma.<br />

24 Meaning <strong>of</strong> images <strong>of</strong> doors in<br />

20th C. paintings <strong>of</strong> mostly<br />

completely normal <strong>art</strong>ists.<br />

Portrait <strong>of</strong> Cachet is discussed as to how it expresses Gachet's<br />

own melancholia.<br />

Detailed knowledge and depictions <strong>of</strong> mental hospital scenes<br />

and recent data from Real y General Hospital in Zaragoza<br />

support affective psychosis indicate mental illness in family.<br />

Symbolistic and psychiatric perspective. To best appreciate the<br />

<strong>art</strong>, his personality should be disregarded. Artists' bio-graphical<br />

data considered poor indicators <strong>of</strong> creation.<br />

Challenges view <strong>of</strong> disordered thought <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia as<br />

manifestation <strong>of</strong> developmentally primitive modes <strong>of</strong><br />

consciousness.<br />

Similarities in theme and formal structures <strong>of</strong> patients to modern<br />

<strong>art</strong> and thought are delineated.<br />

Artwork is more than clinical material, but has expressive<br />

vitality and coherence from which one can relate its testimony<br />

to the universal psychological meaning <strong>of</strong> the struggle to be born<br />

from the forces <strong>of</strong> nonbeing and chaos. Expresses the<br />

archetypal movements within our personal psychology that can<br />

be construed as sickness, but may be that which ultimately heals<br />

blindness to the depths <strong>of</strong> the soul.<br />

Munch's <strong>art</strong> reflected his attempts to recreate the image <strong>of</strong> his<br />

dead sister. Disappointing relationships with women resulted in<br />

heavily affect laden paintings. Later work showed women<br />

more positively and aggression. Munches transitional<br />

relationship with his paintings helped him deal with his fear <strong>of</strong><br />

ego-fragmentation, serving function <strong>of</strong> completion and<br />

autonomy.<br />

Artist attempted to resolve deep-seated psychological conflicts<br />

through painting. Material from life illustrates efforts to deal<br />

with loss, depression, guilt and anxiety through <strong>art</strong>. Creative<br />

work is a response to loss and an effort at self-healing.<br />

Negative relationship between creativity and psychosis assumed<br />

by psychoanalytic studies is challenged.<br />

Data collected from published biographies and archival<br />

material. Over 50% <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>ists had some <strong>psychopathology</strong>,<br />

predominately mood disorders and preoccupation with death,<br />

<strong>of</strong>ten compounded by alcohol abuse. Subjects tended to die<br />

young from suicide, accidents, other causes. 40% sought<br />

treatment and 20% were hospitalised for psychiatric problems.<br />

Results suggest depression inevitably leads to turning inward and<br />

to painful reexamination <strong>of</strong> the purpose <strong>of</strong> living and the<br />

possibility <strong>of</strong> dying and may have put these <strong>art</strong>ists in touch with<br />

the mystery that lies at the he<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the tragic and timeless <strong>art</strong><br />

that the abstract expressionists tried to produce.<br />

Psychoanalytic interpretation using historical material and work.<br />

Gertler's <strong>art</strong> expresses the tensions between stillness and<br />

motion, inanimate and animate and the one and the many<br />

represented a struggle with the sequelae <strong>of</strong> early primal scene<br />

trauma.<br />

Human being's relationship to the environment are explored and<br />

balance, intimacy separation, limits and boundaries are<br />

considered. Doorways are seen as metaphors for the <strong>art</strong>ists'<br />

352<br />

case<br />

case<br />

discussion<br />

description<br />

discussion<br />

discussion<br />

case<br />

case<br />

case<br />

discussion<br />

case<br />

discussion


Compared with untrained<br />

therapy clients.<br />

internal psychic worlds, dialectical integration <strong>of</strong> inside and<br />

outside. Artwork <strong>of</strong> patients also seen as doorway to inner<br />

world not accessed by words.<br />

25 Michelangelo depression. <strong>The</strong> attempt to destroy the Florentine Pieta and immediate and<br />

sustained creative effort <strong>of</strong> his last 10 years to recreate another<br />

Pieta, whilst cutting it up, represent sublimation <strong>of</strong> the main<br />

infantile conflicts <strong>of</strong> his life: loss <strong>of</strong> mother in early infancy and<br />

abandonment by parents. Influence <strong>of</strong> these conflicts on his<br />

character and creativity and lifelong depression and confusion<br />

about the role <strong>of</strong> parental images and self-image.<br />

26 Female painter and female<br />

sculptor case reports to<br />

illustrate Jungian analytic<br />

concepts<br />

Central aim to bring clients to a realisation <strong>of</strong> their creative<br />

forces. Inappropriate reasons that many <strong>art</strong>ists use to avoid<br />

therapy. Jungian psychology considers the relationship between<br />

the conscious and the unconscious to be creative and analysis<br />

may help some <strong>art</strong>ists overcome disturbances that hinder them in<br />

fully realising their creative potential.<br />

27 Vincent Van Gogh as <strong>art</strong>ist Explores the psychoanalytic sources <strong>of</strong> the intensely personal<br />

quality and powerful sense <strong>of</strong> communion with nature in<br />

Vincent Van Gogh's <strong>art</strong>. Life was saga <strong>of</strong> disappointment,<br />

disillusionment and failure. Dominated by spectre <strong>of</strong> stillborn<br />

brother from early life, fantasy motif in later years. Art<br />

replaced all other attachments, goals, ambitions or meaningful<br />

involvements as van Gogh struggled with sense <strong>of</strong> self.<br />

Immersion and devotion to nature may be connected to<br />

dynamics <strong>of</strong> idealised cosmic image <strong>of</strong> nature substitutes for<br />

idealised parent. So, <strong>art</strong> may serve as form <strong>of</strong> transitional<br />

relatedness to the mother <strong>of</strong> his infant experience.<br />

28 82yrs male <strong>art</strong>ist stroke,<br />

aphasic, paralysis<br />

29 Artist not willing to use his<br />

work as p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the therapy<br />

Special measures to free the <strong>art</strong>ist from preconceived notions,<br />

pressures and expectations. Successful - renewed creative<br />

energies and desires.<br />

Illustrates some <strong>of</strong> the conflicts and complications inherent in <strong>art</strong><br />

therapy for <strong>art</strong>ist. Patient used the therapist as a stimulus to<br />

creativity, but as an <strong>art</strong>ist he was not willing to use his work as<br />

p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the therapy. He was unable to separate himself from his<br />

<strong>art</strong>istic productions and it continued to be important for him to<br />

suffer in order to paint and work.<br />

353<br />

case<br />

cases<br />

case<br />

case<br />

case


Table 4: summarising changes or signs predominantly reported in the theme/content <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong>work.<br />

<strong>Study</strong> Case description/ diagnosis Summary themes reported in<br />

<strong>art</strong>work<br />

354<br />

Interpretation<br />

1 13y. male Parental Abuse <strong>The</strong>me <strong>of</strong> ambivalence* <strong>of</strong>ten. Illustrates responses to life changes<br />

2 Adult female<br />

Psychotic (American Indian)<br />

3 70y. mute female - 49 year<br />

hospitalisation. 6m. <strong>art</strong> therapy,<br />

Paranoid Schizophrenia<br />

4 14y. male. 5 years <strong>of</strong><br />

treatment.<br />

Emotional disorder<br />

5 21y. female Chronic<br />

Schizophrenia on<br />

lithium treatment<br />

6 Case studies mixed adults.<br />

Manic/Depressive tested on<br />

serial drawings <strong>of</strong> human<br />

figure<br />

7 36y. female<br />

Reactive Depression<br />

8 24y. male<br />

Brain Injury, car accident<br />

9 40y. male with 20y. hospital<br />

record psychosis.<br />

6 week mural program<br />

10 5 male 5 female adults<br />

Psychosis<br />

11 71 y. female Alzheimer's,<br />

diagnosed cancer<br />

12 10 mixed adults<br />

Manic/Depressive<br />

13 Family, 2 adults 1 male 11y.<br />

conjoint team 6 pics evaluated..<br />

14 72y. male<br />

Aphasic<br />

15 Mixed adults over many years/<br />

variety <strong>of</strong> settings.<br />

Depression<br />

16 Nearly 2000 pics from Multiple<br />

Personality Disordered patients<br />

Static and sparse Impoverished and incongruent with<br />

instructions<br />

Hostility,* suspicious, disorientated<br />

to well orientated, thoughtful.<br />

Selected from 1,250 drawings<br />

almost all circular forms, comments<br />

referred to mouth, cat, dragon and<br />

star.<br />

Religious themes and sexual<br />

conflicts*, from threatening and<br />

dark to lighter<br />

Results indicate directional trendsmutilated<br />

figures in depressive<br />

phase, strong dense lines in manic.<br />

Regression in primitive childlike<br />

characteristics/ younger figures.<br />

Pictures contained personal<br />

information, self disclosure<br />

Empty simple pictures, repeated<br />

theme broken land, disruption*<br />

became unified with colour as<br />

progress toward normal<br />

consciousness<br />

Symbol formation which resulted in<br />

new, more balanced, less<br />

egocentric, more differentiated<br />

schema than at st<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> project<br />

Separation/individuation symbols<br />

especially when representation <strong>of</strong><br />

'three' occurred repeatedly<br />

Loss metaphors similar to cancer<br />

patients<br />

Depressed: less freq. creative,<br />

sombre gloomy themes /colour,<br />

summary drawings. Manic:<br />

rapid/expansive euphoric, warm<br />

bright colour<br />

Kwiatkowska methods. Unusual<br />

family portraits - other indicators<br />

Difficult to recognise complex<br />

imagery<br />

Bright colours, complete scenes,<br />

happy. Repeated symbols - yellow<br />

sun, flowers, trees, houses, grass<br />

extracted 10 thematic, structural and<br />

process, categories: system,<br />

Illustrates progress <strong>of</strong> therapy<br />

Mouth as archetypal image. Early drawings<br />

show the struggle* to rise out <strong>of</strong> the dark<br />

chaos <strong>of</strong> unconscious, become more focused.<br />

Preocc with mouth in autism. <strong>The</strong>ory mouth<br />

is primary site <strong>of</strong> psycho -sexual - social<br />

injury. Analogy <strong>of</strong> myth motifs and rituals to<br />

symbolic themes <strong>of</strong> mouth and transition from<br />

autistic phase ego development to symbiotic<br />

phase.<br />

Shows changes <strong>of</strong> affect, ego functions<br />

strengthened<br />

Minus phenomena in dep., plus properties in<br />

mania. Dynamic drawing tests useful for<br />

tracing changes in patient's state during<br />

therapy or as a prognostic tool<br />

Paralleled and contributed to improvement,<br />

more precise diagnosis<br />

Broken land symbolises disruption <strong>of</strong> brain<br />

damage. Body image relates to figures.<br />

Primary benefit relationship with therapist<br />

Through internalisation, S experienced a<br />

holding environment, now able to verbalise<br />

feelings<br />

Significance <strong>of</strong> numbers may relate to the<br />

number <strong>of</strong> visual symbols produced.<br />

Work expresses present situation <strong>of</strong> patient<br />

Unconsciously knew she was dying<br />

Illustrates illness, reflects disturbance in<br />

activity and thought<br />

Focus treatment and understand problem.<br />

Focus changed from boy to mother<br />

Requires different techs communication to<br />

interpret expression<br />

Symbolic <strong>of</strong> realistic hopes expressed in<br />

<strong>art</strong>work, st<strong>art</strong>ing point for therapy<br />

To aid therapist's diagnosis <strong>of</strong> multiple<br />

personality disorder, but all indicators not


over 9 years fragmentation, barrier*, switching,<br />

threat*, alert, deception*, therapy,<br />

trance, abreaction<br />

355<br />

present.<br />

Table 5: summarising general signs <strong>of</strong> psychiatric disturbance reported in case studies.<br />

No. Case description/<br />

diagnosis<br />

29 39 adult psychiatric<br />

inpatients<br />

30 177 16-71y. psychiatric<br />

outpatients<br />

31 34 adult inpatients<br />

depression &<br />

Schizophrenia<br />

Dot-to-dot geometrical<br />

figure<br />

Summary description <strong>of</strong> form reported in<br />

<strong>art</strong>work<br />

Tendency to circle in a clockwise direction<br />

(torque)<br />

Constricted or expanded figures, drawn<br />

along edges <strong>of</strong> paper, disorganised placing<br />

Right hemispheric inhibition, deformation <strong>of</strong><br />

the whole perceptive space with<br />

compression <strong>of</strong> the right and extension <strong>of</strong> the<br />

left. Left hemispheric inhibition caused<br />

initial and final p<strong>art</strong>s shift to right.<br />

Interpretation<br />

Closely associated with childhood<br />

psychosis, existing sensorimotor<br />

deficits and early cognitive<br />

dev'ment<br />

Drawing styles were found to<br />

correlate with personality traits<br />

Deformation might be based on a<br />

nonlinear distortion <strong>of</strong> the<br />

perceptive space with<br />

displacement <strong>of</strong> the initial<br />

coordinate point to the right from<br />

its geometrical centre.


Table 6: summarising changes or signs predominantly reported in the form <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong>work.<br />

S<br />

T<br />

U<br />

D<br />

Y<br />

No.<br />

Case description/<br />

diagnosis<br />

17 30y. male<br />

decline in behaviour<br />

function/<br />

psychological<br />

decompensation<br />

18 58y. female<br />

dementia (Alzheimer<br />

type)<br />

19 Adult female <strong>art</strong>ist<br />

Borderline personality<br />

disorder<br />

20 22y. female<br />

Depressed<br />

21 Adolescent female<br />

Anorexia<br />

22 24y. male<br />

Impaired short term<br />

memory and Brain<br />

Damage<br />

23 36y. female<br />

Nervous Depression<br />

<strong>art</strong> and music therapy<br />

24 3 female Adults<br />

Depression + other<br />

psychiatric state<br />

25 100 mixed sex Adult<br />

Alexythymia<br />

(depression)<br />

26 Adult male<br />

Schizophrenia<br />

Adult male<br />

Schizophrenia<br />

27 13 mixed sex<br />

Schizophrenia<br />

28 10 Adult (19-55)<br />

Manic Depressive<br />

Summary description <strong>of</strong> form<br />

reported in <strong>art</strong>work<br />

2 sets <strong>of</strong> figure drawings 10 years<br />

ap<strong>art</strong>. Regression, lack detail,<br />

simpler shapes, omissions etc.<br />

Degeneration <strong>of</strong> image, childlike<br />

forms<br />

Childlike forms, fragmentation,<br />

affect, disorganisation, splitting<br />

Black & white or pencil rigid, well<br />

defined. Post therapy<br />

impressionistic, realistic with<br />

fantasy images in colour<br />

Poor verbal interaction, good<br />

quality complete drawings/paintings<br />

with colour and subject matter<br />

Subtle differences in 30 self<br />

portraits<br />

Wild, random, uncontrolled,<br />

incoherent drawings showing lack<br />

<strong>of</strong> connections, no centre <strong>of</strong> focus.<br />

Post-therapy structured, focused,<br />

coherent<br />

Dark, sombre, gloomy pictures<br />

depressive, bright colours not<br />

depressed<br />

Patients asked to draw illness, no<br />

correlation with scores on TAS<br />

1. Ossified, static, fragmentary.<br />

2. Clear structure<br />

Distortion <strong>of</strong> form, perseveration,<br />

simplification, proportion errors,<br />

disconnections, limited colour,<br />

confusion from directions<br />

Improvement with treatment on<br />

quality <strong>of</strong> drawing; head, mouth,<br />

essential detail, richness <strong>of</strong><br />

356<br />

Interpretation<br />

Detailed scoring <strong>of</strong> elements<br />

demonstrating decline intellectual &<br />

behavioral function for later pics<br />

Severe regression, intrapsychic reactions<br />

Art reflects mental state over years -<br />

strength/ego impairment and marked<br />

improvement reflected in drawings<br />

Trauma in self relates to object relation,<br />

mother not good enough, no internal<br />

models. <strong>The</strong>rapist plays different<br />

mothers. Drawings represent possibility<br />

<strong>of</strong> new more satisfying object relations<br />

illustrated<br />

Art can be a bridge to verbal interaction<br />

illustrates issues, conflicts and defenses<br />

Important learning took place although<br />

patient couldn't recall. Paintings provide<br />

access to patient's personality<br />

Express disorientation, turbulence, lack<br />

<strong>of</strong> connections, coherence, etc. shows<br />

state <strong>of</strong> mind. Progressive focus,<br />

structure and organisation <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong><br />

promotes form and order in other areas<br />

and dialogue. Contributes to the healing<br />

process. Creative <strong>art</strong> therapy is not<br />

psychological but <strong>art</strong>istic<br />

Reflect mood state <strong>of</strong> patient<br />

All were able to graphically<br />

communicate illness but drawing<br />

promotes therapeutic alliance provides<br />

symbolic visual means to express<br />

recognise discuss feelings<br />

Drawing type agrees with formal<br />

symptoms therefore diagnosis correct.<br />

<strong>The</strong> formal symptoms disagree with<br />

drawing puts diagnosis in doubt<br />

<strong>The</strong>se signs show evidence <strong>of</strong> initial<br />

diagnosis but also suggest organic<br />

disorder.<br />

11/13 further tested showed organic signs<br />

Some patients show typical features and<br />

pictures reflect improvement or<br />

deterioration in patient


features, execution<br />

Table 7: summarising information for case studies which considered the therapeutic relationship as the most<br />

important feature <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy<br />

No. case description/<br />

diagnosis<br />

1 15y. male<br />

conduct disorder<br />

(fostered, firesetting)<br />

'Acting out'<br />

2 23y. female<br />

multiple personality<br />

disorder + childhood<br />

sex abuse by father<br />

3 30y. male<br />

Chronic Schizophrenia<br />

4 lesbian couple,<br />

deaf+suicidal, other<br />

hearing<br />

5 22y. female<br />

Anxiety Neurosis<br />

6 14y. male<br />

Behavioral Emotional<br />

Disturbance<br />

7 25y. female<br />

Schizophrenia<br />

8 24y. male<br />

Catatonic<br />

Schizophrenia<br />

severely regressed<br />

9 27y. male black<br />

Chronic Paranoid<br />

Schizophrenia<br />

10 15y. male<br />

Paranoid<br />

Schizophrenia<br />

11 15y. female<br />

Borderline personality<br />

+ suicidal<br />

12 26y. male black<br />

Alcoholic. Afraid <strong>of</strong><br />

treatment + emotional<br />

problems<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy type/ patient reaction <strong>art</strong> benefits explained by study relationship<br />

type<br />

Individual <strong>art</strong> sessions, initial<br />

resistance but progressed to<br />

good relations<br />

357<br />

greater sense <strong>of</strong> mastery and insight to<br />

focus primarily on reality<br />

Painting together providing empathetic response denied<br />

as child<br />

<strong>art</strong>, writing and verbals helped<br />

the therapist to understand S<br />

more clearly and recognize and<br />

work with his ego defenses,<br />

deficits and strength<br />

Drawing, psychodrama and clay<br />

sculpture with S's written<br />

afterthoughts<br />

Drawing together, therapist and<br />

subject experience 'merging<br />

with another person'<br />

Drawing and Painting to reverse<br />

narcissism and ego regression<br />

clay molding and drawing to<br />

reorganise and restructure the<br />

pathological development<br />

experience <strong>of</strong> the subject and<br />

encourage growth <strong>of</strong> ego<br />

functions.<br />

through visual imagery and mirroring,<br />

<strong>art</strong> helped the subject and therapist<br />

establish a symbiotic relatedness and<br />

nonverbal modality conducive to a<br />

therapeutic holding envm't<br />

psychodynamic - from the notes by<br />

patient, countertransference had<br />

taken place<br />

<strong>art</strong> provides graphic record <strong>of</strong> merger<br />

and increases receptivity to treatment,<br />

enhances p<strong>art</strong>icipation, corrective<br />

experience, continues psychological<br />

growth<br />

Artwork and therapist served as selfobjects<br />

that helped nourish his inner<br />

self and emotional needs, promote<br />

growth<br />

helped to unite the subject and<br />

therapist and provides a<br />

nonthreatening environment for<br />

mutual trust and therapeutic alliance.<br />

Drawing and interaction help patient express suffering and<br />

observing interaction good for staff<br />

relations, accept patient human-being<br />

Drawing sexualised transference and use <strong>of</strong><br />

energy toward relationship<br />

Drawing images <strong>of</strong> growth and<br />

mourning for termination <strong>of</strong><br />

therapy<br />

Use <strong>of</strong> materials, control over<br />

materials<br />

Drawing and painting in self<br />

situations for fear.<br />

Treatment incomplete but<br />

Gesticulations showed S knew the<br />

therapist was separate and imperfect.<br />

Efforts at separation accelerated as<br />

termination progressed. Disruptive<br />

behaviour coincided feelings <strong>of</strong><br />

disorganisation and despair.<br />

growing mastery <strong>of</strong> materials<br />

generalised to mastery <strong>of</strong> her<br />

environment<br />

Supported patient emotionally for eye<br />

operation and detox clinic. S became<br />

aware <strong>of</strong> himself in nonthreatening<br />

manner and <strong>art</strong> clarified vaguely<br />

nurturing<br />

psychotherapeutic<br />

nurturing<br />

psychotherapeutic<br />

nurturing non<br />

intrusive<br />

psychotherapeutic<br />

nurturing<br />

psychotherapeutic<br />

nurturing -<br />

psychotherapeutic<br />

nurturing -<br />

psychotherapeutic<br />

communicative<br />

communicative<br />

contact<br />

communication<br />

and<br />

release to<br />

indepen-dence<br />

supportive <strong>of</strong><br />

goal directed<br />

interaction<br />

Supportive<br />

emotionally


13 22y. male<br />

Violence and<br />

substance abuse<br />

14 institutionalised<br />

retardates: 30y. male<br />

pr<strong>of</strong>ound quadriplegic;<br />

10y. female, severe;<br />

27y. male, mild<br />

16 4 16-25y. males<br />

Autistic<br />

17 17y. male black Mild<br />

Retardation<br />

18 3 males 22y., 29y.,<br />

32y. Retarded.<br />

progressed further than other<br />

approaches<br />

combined cognitive behavioral<br />

and expressive therapy.<br />

combined <strong>art</strong> modalities not<br />

psychoanalytical.<br />

providing open, accepting,<br />

respecting atmosphere for<br />

expression. Subjects progressed<br />

from inhibition to spontaneity<br />

and from overactivity to self<br />

control<br />

Drawing/painting self issues.<br />

Subject demonstrated improved<br />

functioning and more successful<br />

negotiation <strong>of</strong> the behaviour<br />

mod system<br />

358<br />

perceived situation<br />

Goal to alter self-image <strong>of</strong> violent<br />

patients helps develop alternative<br />

response patterns<br />

p<strong>art</strong>icipation changes subject's<br />

perception <strong>of</strong> internal control by<br />

providing opportunity express or<br />

reflect on inner perceptions and<br />

feelings<br />

both processes reflected underlying<br />

growth in self confidence and<br />

autonomy. Reflected commonality in<br />

all human beings<br />

S achieved a sense <strong>of</strong> mastery through<br />

<strong>art</strong>, resulting in praise and<br />

encouragement improved self-esteem,<br />

supported ego-building, reorganisation<br />

and improved behaviour<br />

Opportunity for creativity environment values divergent<br />

thinking, subjects considered rigid and<br />

concrete thinkers successfully engage<br />

in creative process, no rewards/punish<br />

and natural interactions<br />

Supportive<br />

emotionally<br />

Supportive<br />

emotionally<br />

supportive<br />

emotionally<br />

supportive -<br />

opportunity for<br />

creative<br />

interaction<br />

supportive<br />

opportunity for<br />

creative<br />

interaction


Table 8: summarising reported behaviour changes for case studies.<br />

No. case description/<br />

diagnosis<br />

1 group 14y. mixed sex<br />

Emotional/ behavioral<br />

problems<br />

2 28y. female Mild<br />

retardation<br />

3 2 females 13 and 15y.<br />

Disorganised<br />

Schizophrenia<br />

summary description <strong>of</strong> reported<br />

method<br />

video-taped early later <strong>art</strong> sessions.<br />

Unstructured <strong>art</strong> sessions. Sharing<br />

noted, minimal displacement<br />

activity<br />

Guided imagery and <strong>art</strong><br />

experiences. Used beh observation<br />

form, better; work production<br />

average, increase; and House Tree<br />

Person drawing test<br />

Storytelling and drawing tasks.<br />

Verbal deficits form and content <strong>of</strong><br />

speech and nonverbal deficit in<br />

logical thinking and drawing.<br />

4 24y male Schizophrenia Exercise left parieto-occipital<br />

function, verbal expression <strong>of</strong><br />

spatial relations. Exercise right<br />

hemisphere; drawing.<br />

359<br />

Interpreted<br />

Greater awareness <strong>of</strong> needs/feelings <strong>of</strong><br />

others. Showed greater co-operation non<br />

interference behaviour and more focused<br />

activity<br />

S achieved sense <strong>of</strong> accomplishment and<br />

internal control less feeling helpless.<br />

Blocking revealed deficiency in carrying<br />

out plans, poverty <strong>of</strong> content <strong>of</strong> speech<br />

reflected central deficit in planning.<br />

Central cognitive processing deficits<br />

sensitive to relative social/cognitive<br />

demands <strong>of</strong> context.<br />

Lowest levels <strong>of</strong> hallucinatory behaviour,<br />

aggressive verbal outbursts and physical<br />

aggression in right hem exercises.


Crosstabulations 1-6: contingency tables for Chi-square calculation to<br />

indicate associations between method <strong>of</strong> study, form <strong>of</strong> expression and benefits<br />

for categorised commonalities <strong>of</strong> 67 studies from the literature which claimed<br />

'expression <strong>of</strong> feelings' as main benefit for <strong>art</strong> therapy.<br />

Crosstabulation 1: ORIENTATION By METHOD OF STUDY METHOD<br />

OF STUDY -> 1.Illustrative; 2. projective/express; 3. behaviour; 4. psychoanalytic; 5.<br />

educational/experiential<br />

Std Res 1 2 3 4 5 Row Total<br />

______________________________________________________<br />

ORIENTATION 0 _ 0 _ 1 _ 0 _ 1 _ 1 _ 3<br />

_ .0% _ 33.3% _ .0% _ 33.3% _ 33.3% _ 4.5%<br />

unknown _ .0% _ 2.6% _ .0% _ 14.3% _ 7.7% _<br />

_ -.4 _ -.6 _ -.4 _ 1.2 _ .5 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

1 _ 1 _ 1 _ 1 _ 1 _ 2 _ 6<br />

cognitive _ 16.7% _ 16.7% _ 16.7% _ 16.7% _ 33.3% _ 9.0%<br />

_ 25.0% _ 2.6% _ 25.0% _ 14.3% _ 15.4% _<br />

_ 1.1 _ -1.3 _ 1.1 _ .5 _ .8 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

2 _ 2 _ 28 _ 2 _ 1 _ 4 _ 37<br />

projective _ 5.4% _ 75.7% _ 5.4% _ 2.7% _ 10.8% _ 55.2%<br />

_ 50.0% _ 71.8% _ 50.0% _ 14.3% _ 30.8% _<br />

_ -.1 _ 1.4 _ -.1 _ -1.5 _ -1.2 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

3 _ 0 _ 0 _ 0 _ 0 _ 1 _ 1<br />

phenomen'l _ .0% _ .0% _ .0% _ .0% _ 100.0% _ 1.5%<br />

_ .0% _ .0% _ .0% _ .0% _ 7.7% _<br />

_ -.2 _ -.8 _ -.2 _ -.3 _ 1.8 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

4 _ 0 _ 3 _ 1 _ 1 _ 4 _ 9<br />

social/ _ .0% _ 33.3% _ 11.1% _ 11.1% _ 44.4% _ 13.4%<br />

occupational _ .0% _ 7.7% _ 25.0% _ 14.3% _ 30.8% _<br />

_ -.7 _ -1.0 _ .6 _ .1 _ 1.7 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

5 _ 1 _ 6 _ 0 _ 3 _ 1 _ 11<br />

analytical _ 9.1% _ 54.5% _ .0% _ 27.3% _ 9.1% _ 16.4%<br />

_ 25.0% _ 15.4% _ .0% _ 42.9% _ 7.7% _<br />

_ .4 _ -.2 _ -.8 _ 1.7 _ -.8 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

Column 4 39 4 7 13 67<br />

Total 6.0% 58.2% 6.0% 10.4% 19.4% 100.0%<br />

Chi-Square D.F. Significance Min E.F. Cells with E.F.< 5<br />

26.29640 20 .1562 .060 26 OF 30 ( 86.7%)<br />

Number <strong>of</strong> Missing Observations = 0<br />

360


Crosstabulation 2: ORIENTATION By FORM OF EXPRESSION<br />

FORM OF content style content behav.l verbal other<br />

EXPRESSION -> + style Row<br />

Std Res 1 2 3 4 5 6 Total<br />

_______________________________________________________________<br />

ORIENTATION 0 _ 2 _ 0 _ 0 _ 1 _ 0 _ 0 _ 3<br />

_ 66.7% _ .0% _ .0% _ 33.3% _ .0% _ .0% _ 4.5%<br />

unknown _ 6.1% _ .0% _ .0% _ 5.9% _ .0% _ .0% _<br />

_ .4 _ -.2 _ -.5 _ .3 _ -.6 _ -.3 _<br />

_______________________________________________________<br />

1 _ 2 _ 0 _ 0 _ 3 _ 1 _ 0 _ 6<br />

_ 6.1% _ .0% _ .0% _ 17.6% _ 11.1% _ .0% _ 9.0%<br />

cognitive _ 33.3% _ .0% _ .0% _ 50.0% _ 16.7% _ .0% _<br />

_ -.6 _ -.3 _ -.7 _ 1.2 _ .2 _ -.4 _<br />

_______________________________________________________<br />

2 _ 21 _ 0 _ 3 _ 10 _ 3 _ 0 _ 37<br />

projective _ 56.8% _ .0% _ 8.1% _ 27.0% _ 8.1% _ .0% _ 55.2%<br />

_ 63.6% _ .0% _ 60.0% _ 58.8% _ 33.3% _ .0% _<br />

_ .7 _ -.7 _ .1 _ .2 _ -.9 _ -1.1 _<br />

_______________________________________________________<br />

3 _ 0 _ 0 _ 1 _ 0 _ 0 _ 0 _ 1<br />

phenomenological _ .0% _ .0% _ 100.0% _ .0% _ .0% _ .0% _ 1.5%<br />

_ .0% _ .0% _ 20.0% _ .0% _ .0% _ .0% _<br />

_ -.7 _ -.1 _ 3.4 _ -.5 _ -.4 _ -.2 _<br />

_______________________________________________________<br />

4 _ 3 _ 0 _ 1 _ 3 _ 1 _ 1 _ 9<br />

social/ _ 33.3% _ .0% _ 11.1% _ 33.3% _ 11.1% _ 11.1% _ 13.4%<br />

occupational _ 9.1% _ .0% _ 20.0% _ 17.6% _ 11.1% _ 50.0% _<br />

_ -.7 _ -.4 _ .4 _ .5 _ -.2 _ 1.4 _<br />

_______________________________________________________<br />

5 _ 5 _ 1 _ 0 _ 0 _ 4 _ 1 _ 11<br />

analytical _ 45.5% _ 9.1% _ .0% _ .0% _ 36.4% _ 9.1% _ 16.4%<br />

_ 15.2% _ 100.0% _ .0% _ .0% _ 44.4% _ 50.0% _<br />

_ -.2 _ 2.1 _ -.9 _ -1.7 _ 2.1 _ 1.2 _<br />

_______________________________________________________<br />

Column 33 1 5 17 9 2 67<br />

Total 49.3% 1.5% 7.5% 25.4% 13.4% 3.0% 100.0%<br />

Chi-Square D.F. Significance Min E.F. Cells with E.F.< 5<br />

35.43668 25 .0806 .015 33 OF 36 ( 91.7%)<br />

Number <strong>of</strong> Missing Observations = 0<br />

361


Crosstabulation 3: ORIENTATION By BENEFIT<br />

BENEFIT -> 1. cath<strong>art</strong>ic/reflective; 2. communication; 3. healing/symptom relief; 4.<br />

developmental/social; 5. relationship<br />

Std Res 1 2 3 4 5 Row Total<br />

______________________________________________________<br />

ORIENTATION 0 _ 1 _ 1 _ 0 _ 0 _ 0 _ 2<br />

_ 50.0% _ 50.0% _ .0% _ .0% _ .0% _ 3.4%<br />

unknown _ 6.3% _ 5.0% _ .0% _ .0% _ .0% _<br />

_ .6 _ .4 _ -.6 _ -.5 _ -.4 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

1 _ 2 _ 3 _ 0 _ 1 _ 0 _ 6<br />

cognitive _ 33.3% _ 50.0% _ .0% _ 16.7% _ .0% _ 10.2%<br />

_ 12.5% _ 15.0% _ .0% _ 12.5% _ .0% _<br />

_ .3 _ .7 _ -1.0 _ .2 _ -.7 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

2 _ 12 _ 11 _ 6 _ 3 _ 3 _ 35<br />

projective _ 34.3% _ 31.4% _ 17.1% _ 8.6% _ 8.6% _ 59.3%<br />

_ 75.0% _ 55.0% _ 60.0% _ 37.5% _ 60.0% _<br />

_ .8 _ -.3 _ .0 _ -.8 _ .0 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

3 _ 0 _ 1 _ 0 _ 0 _ 0 _ 1<br />

phenomen'l _ .0% _ 100.0% _ .0% _ .0% _ .0% _ 1.7%<br />

_ .0% _ 5.0% _ .0% _ .0% _ .0% _<br />

_ -.5 _ 1.1 _ -.4 _ -.4 _ -.3 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

4 _ 0 _ 1 _ 3 _ 3 _ 0 _ 7<br />

social/ _ .0% _ 14.3% _ 42.9% _ 42.9% _ .0% _ 11.9%<br />

occupational _ .0% _ 5.0% _ 30.0% _ 37.5% _ .0% _<br />

_ -1.4 _ -.9 _ 1.7 _ 2.1 _ -.8 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

5 _ 1 _ 3 _ 1 _ 1 _ 2 _ 8<br />

analytical _ 12.5% _ 37.5% _ 12.5% _ 12.5% _ 25.0% _ 13.6%<br />

_ 6.3% _ 15.0% _ 10.0% _ 12.5% _ 40.0% _<br />

_ -.8 _ .2 _ -.3 _ -.1 _ 1.6 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

Column 16 20 10 8 5 59<br />

Total 27.1% 33.9% 16.9% 13.6% 8.5% 100.0%<br />

Chi-Square D.F. Significance Min E.F. Cells with E.F.< 5<br />

20.57888 20 .4223 .085 27 OF 30 ( 90.0%)<br />

Number <strong>of</strong> Missing Observations = 8<br />

362


Crosstabulation 4: METHOD OF STUDY By BENEFIT<br />

BENEFIT -> 1. cath<strong>art</strong>ic/reflective; 2. communication; 3. healing/symptom relief; 4.<br />

developmental/social; 5. relationship<br />

Std Res 1 2 3 4 5 Row Total<br />

______________________________________________________<br />

METHOD 1 _ 1 _ 2 _ 0 _ 0 _ 0 _ 3<br />

_ 33.3% _ 66.7% _ .0% _ .0% _ .0% _ 5.1%<br />

illustrates _ 6.3% _ 10.0% _ .0% _ .0% _ .0% _<br />

progress _ .2 _ 1.0 _ -.7 _ -.6 _ -.5 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

2 _ 9 _ 11 _ 7 _ 3 _ 4 _ 34<br />

projective _ 26.5% _ 32.4% _ 20.6% _ 8.8% _ 11.8% _ 57.6%<br />

expressive _ 56.3% _ 55.0% _ 70.0% _ 37.5% _ 80.0% _<br />

_ -.1 _ -.2 _ .5 _ -.7 _ .7 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

3 _ 3 _ 0 _ 0 _ 0 _ 0 _ 3<br />

behaviour _ 100.0% _ .0% _ .0% _ .0% _ .0% _ 5.1%<br />

_ 18.8% _ .0% _ .0% _ .0% _ .0% _<br />

_ 2.4 _ -1.0 _ -.7 _ -.6 _ -.5 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

4 _ 2 _ 1 _ 2 _ 1 _ 0 _ 6<br />

psycho- _ 33.3% _ 16.7% _ 33.3% _ 16.7% _ .0% _ 10.2%<br />

analytic _ 12.5% _ 5.0% _ 20.0% _ 12.5% _ .0% _<br />

_ .3 _ -.7 _ 1.0 _ .2 _ -.7 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

5 _ 1 _ 6 _ 1 _ 4 _ 1 _ 13<br />

education/ _ 7.7% _ 46.2% _ 7.7% _ 30.8% _ 7.7% _ 22.0%<br />

experience _ 6.3% _ 30.0% _ 10.0% _ 50.0% _ 20.0% _<br />

_ -1.3 _ .8 _ -.8 _ 1.7 _ -.1 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

Column 16 20 10 8 5 59<br />

Total 27.1% 33.9% 16.9% 13.6% 8.5% 100.0%<br />

Chi-Square D.F. Significance Min E.F. Cells with E.F.< 5<br />

19.52039 16 .2426 .254 22 OF 25 ( 88.0%)<br />

Number <strong>of</strong> Missing Observations = 8<br />

363


Crosstabulation 5: FORM OF EXPRESSION By METHOD OF STUDY<br />

METHOD OF STUDY -> 1.Illustrative; 2. projective/express; 3. behaviour; 4.<br />

psychoanalytic; 5. ed.l/experiential<br />

Std Res 1 2 3 4 5 Total<br />

______________________________________________________<br />

FORM OF 1 _ 3 _ 19 _ 1 _ 5 _ 5 _ 33<br />

EXPRESSION _ 9.1% _ 57.6% _ 3.0% _ 15.2% _ 15.2% _ 49.3%<br />

_ 75.0% _ 48.7% _ 25.0% _ 71.4% _ 38.5% _<br />

content _ .7 _ -.0 _ -.7 _ .8 _ -.6 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

2 _ 0 _ 1 _ 0 _ 0 _ 0 _ 1<br />

style _ .0% _ 100.0% _ .0% _ .0% _ .0% _ 1.5%<br />

_ .0% _ 2.6% _ .0% _ .0% _ .0% _<br />

_ -.2 _ .5 _ -.2 _ -.3 _ -.4 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

3 _ 1 _ 2 _ 0 _ 0 _ 2 _ 5<br />

content _ 20.0% _ 40.0% _ .0% _ .0% _ 40.0% _ 7.5%<br />

and style _ 25.0% _ 5.1% _ .0% _ .0% _ 15.4% _<br />

_ 1.3 _ -.5 _ -.5 _ -.7 _ 1.0 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

4 _ 0 _ 11 _ 2 _ 0 _ 4 _ 17<br />

behaviour _ .0% _ 64.7% _ 11.8% _ .0% _ 23.5% _ 25.4%<br />

_ .0% _ 28.2% _ 50.0% _ .0% _ 30.8% _<br />

_ -1.0 _ .4 _ 1.0 _ -1.3 _ .4 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

5 _ 0 _ 5 _ 1 _ 2 _ 1 _ 9<br />

verbal _ .0% _ 55.6% _ 11.1% _ 22.2% _ 11.1% _ 13.4%<br />

_ .0% _ 12.8% _ 25.0% _ 28.6% _ 7.7% _<br />

_ -.7 _ -.1 _ .6 _ 1.1 _ -.6 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

other 6 _ 0 _ 1 _ 0 _ 0 _ 1 _ 2<br />

_ .0% _ 50.0% _ .0% _ .0% _ 50.0% _ 3.0%<br />

_ .0% _ 2.6% _ .0% _ .0% _ 7.7% _<br />

_ -.3 _ -.2 _ -.3 _ -.5 _ 1.0 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

Column 4 39 4 7 13 67<br />

Total 6.0% 58.2% 6.0% 10.4% 19.4% 100.0%<br />

Chi-Square D.F. Significance Min E.F. Cells with E.F.< 5<br />

14.50528 20 .8040 .060 26 OF 30 ( 86.7%)<br />

Number <strong>of</strong> Missing Observations = 0<br />

364


Crosstabulation 6: FORM OF EXPRESSION By BENEFIT<br />

BENEFIT -> 1. cath<strong>art</strong>ic/reflective; 2. communication; 3. healing/symptom relief; 4.<br />

developmental/social; 5. relationship<br />

Std Res 1 2 3 4 5 Row Total<br />

______________________________________________________<br />

FORM OF 1 _ 9 _ 8 _ 4 _ 2 _ 4 _ 27<br />

EXPRESSION _ 33.3% _ 29.6% _ 14.8% _ 7.4% _ 14.8% _ 45.8%<br />

_ 56.3% _ 40.0% _ 40.0% _ 25.0% _ 80.0% _<br />

content _ .6 _ -.4 _ -.3 _ -.9 _ 1.1 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

2 _ 0 _ 1 _ 0 _ 0 _ 0 _ 1<br />

style _ .0% _ 100.0% _ .0% _ .0% _ .0% _ 1.7%<br />

_ .0% _ 5.0% _ .0% _ .0% _ .0% _<br />

_ -.5 _ 1.1 _ -.4 _ -.4 _ -.3 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

3 _ 1 _ 3 _ 1 _ 0 _ 0 _ 5<br />

content _ 20.0% _ 60.0% _ 20.0% _ .0% _ .0% _ 8.5%<br />

and style _ 6.3% _ 15.0% _ 10.0% _ .0% _ .0% _<br />

_ -.3 _ 1.0 _ .2 _ -.8 _ -.7 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

4 _ 4 _ 4 _ 3 _ 3 _ 1 _ 15<br />

behaviour _ 26.7% _ 26.7% _ 20.0% _ 20.0% _ 6.7% _ 25.4%<br />

_ 25.0% _ 20.0% _ 30.0% _ 37.5% _ 20.0% _<br />

_ -.0 _ -.5 _ .3 _ .7 _ -.2 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

5 _ 2 _ 3 _ 2 _ 2 _ 0 _ 9<br />

verbal _ 22.2% _ 33.3% _ 22.2% _ 22.2% _ .0% _ 15.3%<br />

_ 12.5% _ 15.0% _ 20.0% _ 25.0% _ .0% _<br />

_ -.3 _ -.0 _ .4 _ .7 _ -.9 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

6 _ 0 _ 1 _ 0 _ 1 _ 0 _ 2<br />

_ .0% _ 50.0% _ .0% _ 50.0% _ .0% _ 3.4%<br />

other _ .0% _ 5.0% _ .0% _ 12.5% _ .0% _<br />

_ -.7 _ .4 _ -.6 _ 1.4 _ -.4 _<br />

______________________________________________<br />

Column 16 20 10 8 5 59<br />

Total 27.1% 33.9% 16.9% 13.6% 8.5% 100.0%<br />

Chi-Square D.F. Significance Min E.F. Cells with E.F.< 5<br />

12.29713 20 .9055 .085 27 OF 30 ( 90.0%)<br />

Number <strong>of</strong> Missing Observations = 8<br />

365


Reliability <strong>Study</strong> Chapter 4 - Methods.<br />

Tables and Plots 1-15.<br />

Tables 1-15 by variable showing: column 1, Raw data scores for each<br />

variable each rater( r1-7) over 7 pictures in sequence.<br />

Column 2, scoring differences between 6 raters and author on 7 rated<br />

pictures.<br />

Column 3, mean differences for raters 1-6 and standard difference to rater<br />

7 (author).<br />

Plots 1-15 by variable showing individual rater differences around average<br />

rating <strong>of</strong> 6 independent raters and author.<br />

366


Tables showing mean differences between 6 raters and author on 7 rated pictures.<br />

Raters R1-7 score for RED,<br />

pics 1-7 - R7 = author*<br />

R1 R2 R3 R4 R5 R6 R7*<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

6 6 6 6 6 6 6<br />

5 9 9 8 5 6 9<br />

10 15 12 12 8 11 14<br />

5 7 5 5 5 7 6<br />

0 0 0 5 0 0 0<br />

11 11 11 12 11 9 10<br />

Total<br />

Total %<br />

agreement exact<br />

(x); ±1 point (1);<br />

±2 points (2)<br />

x 1 2<br />

6<br />

6<br />

2 1<br />

0 1 2<br />

0 6<br />

5<br />

5 1<br />

----------<br />

24 9 2<br />

57 21 5<br />

mean for raters 1-6<br />

and difference to<br />

R7 ratings<br />

mean diff.<br />

R1-6 R7<br />

.00 .00<br />

6.00 .00<br />

7.00 2.00<br />

11.33 2.67<br />

5.67 .33<br />

.83 -.83<br />

10.83 -.83<br />

1. Difference in mean interrater score/author score for RED (RATS - A)<br />

plotted against average (RATS + A)/2.<br />

________________________________________<br />

d 4_ _<br />

i _ _<br />

f _ 1 _<br />

f _ 1 _<br />

e _ _<br />

r 0_1__________2___________________________<br />

e _ 1 1 _<br />

n _ _<br />

c _ _<br />

e _ _<br />

-4_ _<br />

R-A ________________________________________<br />

0 5 10 15 20<br />

Average 6 rats + author<br />

Raters R1-7 score for YELLOW<br />

pics 1-7 - R7 = author*<br />

R1 R2 R3 R4 R5 R6 R7*<br />

5 5 8 5 5 5 5<br />

4 5 3 5 5 3 5<br />

6 6 6 5 6 6 6<br />

16 14 13 16 16 17 16<br />

13 13 10 13 13 13 13<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

8 10 10 11 8 9 12<br />

Total<br />

Total %<br />

agreement exact<br />

(x); ±1 point (1);<br />

±2 points (2)<br />

x 1 2<br />

5<br />

4 2<br />

5 1<br />

3 1 1<br />

5<br />

6<br />

0 1 2<br />

-----------<br />

28 5 3<br />

67 12 7<br />

mean for raters 1-6<br />

and difference to<br />

R7 ratings.<br />

mean diff.<br />

R1-6 R7<br />

5.50 -.50<br />

4.17 .83<br />

5.83 .17<br />

15.33 .67<br />

12.50 .50<br />

.00 .00<br />

9.33 2.67<br />

fig 2. Difference in mean interrater score/author score for YELLOW (RATS - A)<br />

plotted against average (RATS + A)/2.<br />

________________________________________<br />

d 4_ _<br />

i _ _<br />

f _ 1 _<br />

f _ _<br />

e _ 1 1 1 _<br />

r 0_1__________1___________________________<br />

e _ 1 _<br />

n _ _<br />

c _ _<br />

e _ _<br />

-4_ _<br />

R-A ________________________________________<br />

0 5 10 15 20<br />

Average 6 rats + author<br />

367


Raters R1-7 score for ORANGE,<br />

pics 1-7 - R7 = author*<br />

R1 R2 R3 R4 R5 R6 R7*<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

19 18 18 19 19 18 19<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

6 7 8 0 3 0 3<br />

Total<br />

Total %<br />

agreement exact<br />

(x); ±1 point (1);<br />

±2 points (2)<br />

x 1 2<br />

6<br />

6<br />

3 3<br />

6<br />

6<br />

6<br />

1<br />

-----------<br />

34 3<br />

81 7<br />

mean for raters 1-6<br />

and difference to<br />

R7 ratings.<br />

mean diff.<br />

R1-6 R7<br />

.00 .00<br />

.00 .00<br />

18.50 .50<br />

.00 .00<br />

.00 .00<br />

.00 .00<br />

4.00 -1.00<br />

fig 3. Difference in mean interrater score/author score for ORANGE (RATS - A)<br />

plotted against average (RATS + A)/2.<br />

________________________________________<br />

d 4_ _<br />

i _ _<br />

f _ _<br />

f _ _<br />

e _ 1 _<br />

r 0_5______________________________________<br />

e _ 1 _<br />

n _ _<br />

c _ _<br />

e _ _<br />

-4_ _<br />

R-A ________________________________________<br />

0 5 10 15 20<br />

Average 6 rats + author<br />

Raters R1-7 score for PURPLE,<br />

pics 1-7 - R7 = author*<br />

R1 R2 R3 R4 R5 R6 R7*<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

0 0 0 1 0 0 0<br />

17 14 11 14 17 9 13<br />

15 12 12 15 12 11 11<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

5 5 5 3 5 5 5<br />

0 3 0 0 1 0 0<br />

Total<br />

Total %<br />

agreement exact<br />

(x); ±1 point (1);<br />

±2 points (2)<br />

x 1 2<br />

6<br />

5 1<br />

0 2 1<br />

1 3<br />

6<br />

5 0 1<br />

4 1<br />

-----------<br />

27 7 2<br />

64 17 5<br />

mean for raters 1-6<br />

and difference to<br />

R7 ratings.<br />

mean diff.<br />

R1-6 R7<br />

.00 .00<br />

.17 -.17<br />

13.67 -.67<br />

12.83 -1.83<br />

.00 .00<br />

4.67 .33<br />

.67 -.67<br />

fig 4. Difference in mean interrater score/author score for PURPLE (RATS - A)<br />

plotted against average (RATS + A)/2.<br />

________________________________________<br />

d 4_ _<br />

i _ _<br />

f _ _<br />

f _ _<br />

e _ _<br />

r 0_3________1_____________________________<br />

e _ 1 1 _<br />

n _ 1 _<br />

c _ _<br />

e _ _<br />

-4_ _<br />

R-A ________________________________________<br />

0 5 10 15 20<br />

Average 6 rats + author<br />

368


Raters R1-7 score for GREEN<br />

pics 1-7 - R7 = author*<br />

R1 R2 R3 R4 R5 R6<br />

R7*<br />

5 5 4 5 5 5 4<br />

12 14 13 11 13 13 13<br />

3 3 3 2 3 3 3<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

13 11 11 7 9 9 7<br />

10 10 10 10 10 10<br />

10<br />

5 5 3 4 4 5 4<br />

Total<br />

Total %<br />

agreement exact<br />

(x); ±1 point (1);<br />

±2 points (2)<br />

x 1 2<br />

1 5<br />

3 2 1<br />

5 1<br />

6<br />

1 0 2<br />

6<br />

2 4<br />

-----------<br />

24 12 3<br />

57 29 7<br />

mean for raters 1-6<br />

and difference to<br />

R7 ratings.<br />

mean diff.<br />

R1-6 R7<br />

4.83 -.83<br />

12.67 .33<br />

2.83 .17<br />

.00 .00<br />

10.00 -3.00<br />

10.00 .00<br />

4.33 -.33<br />

fig 5. Difference in mean interrater score/author score for GREEN (RATS - A)<br />

plotted against average (RATS + A)/2.<br />

________________________________________<br />

d 4_ _<br />

i _ _<br />

f _ _<br />

f _ _<br />

e _ _<br />

r 0_1____1__1_________1____1_______________<br />

e _ 1 _<br />

n _ _<br />

c _ _<br />

e _ 1 _<br />

-4_ _<br />

R-A ________________________________________<br />

0 5 10 15 20<br />

Average 6 rats + author<br />

Raters R1-7 score for BLUE, pics<br />

1-7 - R7 = author*<br />

R1 R2 R3 R4 R5 R6<br />

R7*<br />

8 8 8 8 8 8<br />

8<br />

16 16 14 13 15 15<br />

13<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

0<br />

11 14 9 11 14 13<br />

9<br />

4 5 4 4 5 4<br />

4<br />

7 7 8 7 13 12<br />

7<br />

19 18 19 18 18 18<br />

16<br />

Total<br />

Total %<br />

agreement exact<br />

(x); ±1 point (1);<br />

±2 points (2)<br />

x 1 2<br />

6<br />

1 1 2<br />

6<br />

1 0 2<br />

4 2<br />

3 1<br />

0 0 4<br />

-----------<br />

21 4 8<br />

50 10 19<br />

mean for raters 1-6<br />

and difference to<br />

R7 ratings.<br />

mean diff.<br />

R1-6 R7<br />

8.00 .00<br />

14.83 -1.83<br />

.00 .00<br />

12.00 -3.00<br />

4.33 -.33<br />

9.00 -2.00<br />

18.33 -2.33<br />

fig 6. Difference in mean interrater score/author score for BLUE (RATS - A)<br />

plotted against average (RATS + A)/2.<br />

________________________________________<br />

d 4_ _<br />

i _ _<br />

f _ _<br />

f _ _<br />

e _ _<br />

r 0_1_______1______1_______________________<br />

e _ _<br />

n _ 1 1 _<br />

c _ 1 _<br />

e _ 1 _<br />

369


-4_ _<br />

R-A ________________________________________<br />

0 5 10 15 20<br />

Average 6 rats + author<br />

Raters R1-7 score for BROWN,<br />

pics 1-7 - R7 = author*<br />

R1 R2 R3 R4 R5 R6<br />

R7*<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

1 2 0 0 0 0 0<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

8 0 0 8 7 12 0<br />

4 2 0 3 0 6 0<br />

10 10 9 10 4 4 10<br />

14 10 12 13 12 16 12<br />

Total<br />

Total %<br />

agreement exact<br />

(x); ±1 point (1);<br />

±2 points (2)<br />

x 1 2<br />

6<br />

4 1 1<br />

6<br />

2<br />

2 0 1<br />

3 1<br />

2 1 2<br />

-----------<br />

25 3 4<br />

60 7 10<br />

mean for raters 1-6<br />

and difference to<br />

R7 ratings.<br />

mean diff.<br />

R1-6 R7<br />

.00 .00<br />

.50 -.50<br />

.00 .00<br />

5.83 -5.83<br />

2.50 -2.50<br />

7.83 2.17<br />

12.83 -.83<br />

fig 7. Difference in mean interrater score/author score for BROWN (RATS - A)<br />

plotted against average (RATS + A)/2.<br />

________________________________________<br />

d 4_ _<br />

i _ _<br />

f _ 1 _<br />

f _ _<br />

e _ _<br />

r 0_2______________________________________<br />

e _1 1 _<br />

n _ _<br />

c _ 1 _<br />

e _ _<br />

-4_ _<br />

_ _<br />

_ 1 _<br />

R-A ________________________________________<br />

0 5 10 15 20<br />

Average 6 rats + author<br />

Raters R1-7 score for WHITE,<br />

pics 1-7 – R7 = author*<br />

R1 R2 R3 R4 R5 R6 R7*<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

6 7 0 7 7 4 7<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

Total<br />

Total %<br />

agreement exact<br />

(x); ±1 point (1);<br />

±2 points (2)<br />

x 1 2<br />

6<br />

6<br />

3 1<br />

6<br />

6<br />

6<br />

6<br />

---------<br />

39 1<br />

93 2<br />

mean for raters 1-6<br />

and difference to<br />

R7 ratings.<br />

mean diff.<br />

R1-6 R7<br />

.00 .00<br />

.00 .00<br />

5.17 1.83<br />

.00 .00<br />

.00 .00<br />

.00 .00<br />

.00 .00<br />

fig 8. Difference in mean interrater score/author score for WHITE (RATS - A)<br />

plotted against average (RATS + A)/2.<br />

________________________________________<br />

d 4_ _<br />

i _ _<br />

f _ _<br />

f _ 1 _<br />

e _ _<br />

r 0_6______________________________________<br />

e _ _<br />

n _ _<br />

370


c _ _<br />

e _ _<br />

-4_ _<br />

R-A ________________________________________<br />

0 5 10 15 20<br />

Average 6 rats + author<br />

Raters R1-7 score for BLACK,<br />

pics 1-7 - R7 = author*<br />

R1 R2 R3 R4 R5 R6 R7*<br />

12 12 12 12 9 12 12<br />

14 15 14 14 14 14 14<br />

8 8 6 9 8 8 9<br />

19 20 18 20 18 19 20<br />

19 17 18 18 19 18 18<br />

1 1 1 0 0 0 0<br />

12 10 11 12 11 11 10<br />

Total<br />

Total %<br />

agreement exact<br />

(x); ±1 point (1);<br />

±2 points (2)<br />

x 1 2<br />

5<br />

5 1<br />

1 4<br />

2 2 2<br />

3 3<br />

3 3<br />

1 3 2<br />

-----------<br />

20 16 4<br />

48 38 10<br />

mean for raters 1-6<br />

and difference to<br />

R7 ratings.<br />

mean diff.<br />

R1-6 R7<br />

11.50 .50<br />

14.17 -.17<br />

7.83 1.17<br />

19.00 1.00<br />

18.17 -.17<br />

.50 -.50<br />

11.17 -1.17<br />

fig 9. Difference in mean interrater score/author score for BLACK (RATS - A)<br />

plotted against average (RATS + A)/2.<br />

________________________________________<br />

d 4_ _<br />

i _ _<br />

f _ _<br />

f _ _<br />

e _ 1 1 1 _<br />

r 0___________________________1______1_____<br />

e _1 1 _<br />

n _ _<br />

c _ _<br />

e _ _<br />

-4_ _<br />

R-A ________________________________________<br />

0 5 10 15 20<br />

Average 6 rats + author<br />

Raters R1-7 score for INTENSITY,<br />

pics 1-7 - R7 = author*<br />

R1 R2 R3 R4 R5 R6 R7*<br />

11 14 14 17 19 14 19<br />

16 17 20 18 19 15 18<br />

20 15 20 20 18 20 20<br />

19 13 17 15 19 14 16<br />

14 12 13 15 15 12 15<br />

9 11 10 10 9 8 10<br />

20 18 20 19 18 20 18<br />

Total<br />

Total %<br />

agreement exact<br />

(x); ±1 point (1);<br />

±2 points (2)<br />

x 1 2<br />

1 0 1<br />

1 2 2<br />

4 0 1<br />

0 2 1<br />

2 1 1<br />

2 3 1<br />

2 1 3<br />

-----------<br />

12 9 10 29 21<br />

24<br />

mean for raters 1-6<br />

and difference to<br />

R7 ratings.<br />

mean diff.<br />

R1-6 R7<br />

14.83 3.83<br />

17.44 .89<br />

18.83 1.17<br />

16.22 -.22<br />

13.44 1.89<br />

9.56 .44<br />

19.22 -.89<br />

fig 10. Difference in mean interrater score/author score for INTENSITY (RATS - A)<br />

plotted against average (RATS + A)/2.<br />

________________________________________<br />

d 4_ 1 _<br />

i _ _<br />

f _ _<br />

f _ 1 _<br />

e _ 1 1 1 _<br />

r 0______________________________1_________<br />

e _ 1 _<br />

n _ _<br />

c _ _<br />

371


e _ _<br />

-4_ _<br />

R-A ________________________________________<br />

0 5 10 15 20<br />

Average 6 rats + author<br />

372


Raters R1-7 score for PAINTED<br />

LINE, pics 1-7<br />

R7 = author*<br />

R1 R2 R3 R4 R5 R6 R<br />

7*<br />

8 12 9 7 7 9<br />

11<br />

4 6 6 15 4 6<br />

7<br />

6 6 6 4 5 4<br />

7<br />

14 14 11 7 15 9<br />

13<br />

4 8 8 12 11 11<br />

12<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

0<br />

4 4 3 5 6 4<br />

4<br />

Total<br />

Total %<br />

agreement exact<br />

(x); ±1 point (1);<br />

±2 points (2)<br />

x 1 2<br />

0 1 2<br />

0 3<br />

0 3 1<br />

0 2 2<br />

1 2<br />

6<br />

3 2 1<br />

-----------<br />

10 13 6<br />

24 31 14<br />

mean for raters 1-6<br />

and difference to<br />

R7 ratings.<br />

mean diff.<br />

R1-6 R7<br />

8.78 2.56<br />

6.83 .17<br />

5.22 1.78<br />

11.72 1.61<br />

8.94 2.72<br />

.00 .00<br />

4.33 -.33<br />

fig 11. Difference in mean interrater score/author score for PAINTED LINE (RATS - A)<br />

plotted against average (RATS + A)/2.<br />

________________________________________<br />

d 4_ _<br />

i _ _<br />

f _ 11 _<br />

f _ 1 1 _<br />

e _ _<br />

r 0_1_______1____1_________________________<br />

e _ _<br />

n _ _<br />

c _ _<br />

e _ _<br />

-4_ _<br />

R-A ________________________________________<br />

0 5 10 15 20<br />

Average 6 rats + author<br />

Raters R1-7 score for DRAWN<br />

LINE, pics 1-7 - R7 = author*<br />

R1 R2 R3 R4 R5 R6 R7*<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

5 4 4 10 6 5 5<br />

1 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

Total<br />

Total %<br />

agreement exact<br />

(x); ±1 point (1);<br />

±2 points (2)<br />

x 1 2<br />

6<br />

6<br />

6<br />

6<br />

6<br />

2 3<br />

5 1<br />

-------<br />

37 4<br />

88 10<br />

mean for raters 1-6<br />

and difference to<br />

R7 ratings.<br />

mean diff.<br />

R1-6 R7<br />

.00 .00<br />

.00 .00<br />

.00 .00<br />

.00 .00<br />

.00 .00<br />

5.61 -.28<br />

.17 -.17<br />

fig 12. Difference in mean interrater score/author score for DRAWN LINE (RATS - A)<br />

plotted against average (RATS + A)/2.<br />

________________________________________<br />

d 4_ _<br />

i _ _<br />

f _ _<br />

f _ _<br />

e _ _<br />

r 0_6_________1____________________________<br />

e _ _<br />

n _ _<br />

373


c _ _<br />

e _ _<br />

-4_ _<br />

R-A ________________________________________<br />

0 5 10 15 20<br />

Average 6 rats + author<br />

Raters R1-7 score for SPACE,<br />

pics 1-7 - R7 = author*<br />

R1 R2 R3 R4 R5 R6 R7*<br />

15 14 15 10 14 13 12<br />

10 9 9 5 10 9 8<br />

4 4 4 4 4 4 4<br />

6 7 8 7 6 6 6<br />

4 4 4 4 4 4 4<br />

16 15 15 15 14 16 14<br />

4 4 4 4 4 4 4<br />

Total<br />

Total %<br />

agreement exact<br />

(x); ±1 point (1);<br />

±2 points (2)<br />

x 1 2<br />

0 1 3<br />

0 3 2<br />

6<br />

3 2 1<br />

6<br />

1 3 2<br />

6<br />

-----------<br />

22 9 8<br />

52 21 19<br />

mean for raters 1-6<br />

and difference to<br />

R7 ratings.<br />

mean diff.<br />

R1-6 R7<br />

13.37 -1.77<br />

8.67 -.87<br />

4.00 .00<br />

6.80 -1.00<br />

4.00 .00<br />

15.13 -.93<br />

4.00 .00<br />

fig 13. Difference in mean interrater score/author score for SPACE (RATS - A)<br />

plotted against average (RATS + A)/2.<br />

________________________________________<br />

d 4_ _<br />

i _ _<br />

f _ _<br />

f _ _<br />

e _ _<br />

r 0________3_______________________________<br />

e _ 1 1 1 _<br />

n _ 1 _<br />

c _ _<br />

e _ _<br />

-4_ _<br />

R-A ________________________________________<br />

0 5 10 15 20<br />

Average 6 rats + author<br />

Raters R1-7 score for<br />

EMOTIONAL TONE, pics 1-7 -<br />

R7 = author*<br />

R1 R2 R3 R4 R5 R6 R7*<br />

11 11 18 14 12 13 15<br />

15 16 19 18 16 16 15<br />

19 18 20 17 19 16 20<br />

7 10 11 9 7 9 7<br />

10 14 10 8 9 8 8<br />

14 14 15 16 17 16 18<br />

15 14 16 13 8 13 13<br />

Total<br />

Total %<br />

Agreement exact<br />

(x); ±1 point (1);<br />

±2 points (2)<br />

x 1 2<br />

0 1 1<br />

1 3<br />

1 2 1<br />

2 0 2<br />

2 1 2<br />

0 1 2<br />

2 1 1<br />

-----------<br />

8 9 9<br />

19 21 21<br />

mean for raters 1-6<br />

and difference to<br />

R7 ratings.<br />

mean diff.<br />

R1-6 R7<br />

13.28 1.39<br />

16.56 -1.22<br />

18.17 1.83<br />

8.83 -2.17<br />

9.83 -1.83<br />

15.22 3.11<br />

13.28 .06<br />

fig 14. Difference in mean interrater score/author score for EMOTIONAL TONE (RATS - A)<br />

plotted against average (RATS + A)/2.<br />

________________________________________<br />

d 4_ _<br />

i _ 1 _<br />

f _ _<br />

f _ 1 1 _<br />

e _ _<br />

r 0_________________________1______________<br />

e _ _<br />

n _ 1 1 _<br />

c _ 1 _<br />

e _ _<br />

374


-4_ _<br />

R-A ________________________________________<br />

0 5 10 15 20<br />

Average 6 rats + author<br />

Raters R1-7 score for FORM,<br />

pics 1-7 - R7 = author*<br />

R1 R2 R3 R4 R5 R6 R7*<br />

0 0 0 0 0 3 0<br />

7 8 8 7 7 7 8<br />

5 7 5 7 7 5 5<br />

0 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

8 0 0 0 0 0 0<br />

16 16 16 16 16 16 16<br />

3 3 3 3 3 3 3<br />

Total<br />

Total %<br />

agreement exact<br />

(x); ±1 point (1);<br />

±2 points (2)<br />

x 1 2<br />

5<br />

2 4<br />

3 0 3<br />

6<br />

6<br />

6<br />

6<br />

------------<br />

34 4 3<br />

81 10 7<br />

mean for raters 1-6<br />

and difference to<br />

R7 ratings.<br />

mean diff.<br />

R1-6 R7<br />

.50 -.50<br />

7.33 .67<br />

6.00 -1.00<br />

.00 .00<br />

1.33 -1.33<br />

16.00 .00<br />

3.00 .00<br />

fig 15. Difference in mean interrater score/author score for DOMINANT FORM (RATS - A)<br />

plotted against average (RATS + A)/2.<br />

________________________________________<br />

d 4_ _<br />

i _ _<br />

f _ _<br />

f _ _<br />

e _ 1 _<br />

r 0_1____1_______________________1_________<br />

e _1 1 _<br />

n _ 1 _<br />

c _ _<br />

e _ _<br />

-4_ _<br />

R-A ________________________________________<br />

0 5 10 15 20<br />

Average 6 rats + author<br />

375


Appendix 3<br />

Table <strong>of</strong> Authorities<br />

1. Impressionistic/<strong>The</strong>oretical Studies<br />

2. Case Studies <strong>of</strong> Artists<br />

3. Case Studies - change <strong>of</strong> theme or content elements<br />

4. Case Studies - change <strong>of</strong> objective or formal elements<br />

5. Case Studies - change <strong>of</strong> behaviour<br />

6. Case Studies - therapeutic benefit - relationship<br />

7. Case Studies - therapeutic benefit - expression <strong>of</strong> feelings<br />

8. Controlled Studies<br />

376


Table <strong>of</strong> Authorities<br />

Impressionistic studies<br />

Abraham, A. (1990), <strong>The</strong> projection <strong>of</strong> the inner group in drawing, Group Analysis , Dec., V.23(4):391-<br />

401.<br />

Adler, R.F., Fisher, P. (1984), My self ... through music, movement and <strong>art</strong>, Arts in Psychotherapy , Fall,<br />

V.11(3):203-8.<br />

Adelman, E., Castricone, L. (1986), An expressive <strong>art</strong>s model for substance abuse group training and<br />

treatment, Arts in Psychotherapy , Spr., V.13(1):53-9.<br />

Albert-Puleo, N. (1980), Modern psychoanalytic <strong>art</strong> therapy and its application to drug abuse, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , V.7(1):43-52.<br />

Albert-Puleo, N., Osha, V. (1976), Art therapy as an alcoholism treatment tool, Alcohol Health and<br />

Research <strong>World</strong> , Win., V.1[2]28-31.<br />

Allan, J., Clark, M. (1984), Directed <strong>art</strong> counselling, Elementary School Guidance and Counselling , Dec.,<br />

V.19(2):116-24.<br />

Allen, P.B. (1983), Group <strong>art</strong> therapy in short-term hospital settings, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Apr.,<br />

V.22(3):93-5.<br />

Allen, P.B. (1985), Integrating <strong>art</strong> therapy into an alcoholism treatment program, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy ,<br />

Aug., V.24(1):10-12.<br />

Amos, S.P. (1982), <strong>The</strong> diagnostic, prognostic, and therapeutic implications <strong>of</strong> schizophrenic <strong>art</strong>, Arts<br />

in Psychotherapy , Sum., V.9(2):131-143.<br />

Arrington, D. (1991), Thinking systems-seeing systems: an integrative model for systematically oriented<br />

<strong>art</strong> therapy, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.18(3):201-11.<br />

Atlas, J.A., Smith, P., Sessoms, L. (1992), Art and poetry in brief therapy <strong>of</strong> hospitalized adolescents,<br />

Arts in Psychotherapy , V.19(4):279-83.<br />

Assael, M., Popovici-Wacks, M. (1989), Artistic expression in spontaneous paintings <strong>of</strong> depressed<br />

patients, Israel J. <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry and Related Science s , V.26(4):223-243.<br />

Assael, M. (1978), Spontaneous painting: means <strong>of</strong> communication, Confinia Psychiatrica , V.21(1-3):10-<br />

24.<br />

Avstreih, A.K., Brown, J.J. (1979), Some aspects <strong>of</strong> movement and <strong>art</strong> therapy as related to the analytic<br />

situation, Psychoanalytic R eview , V.66(1):49-68.<br />

Ba, G. (1988), Strategies <strong>of</strong> rehabilitation in the day hospital, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics ,<br />

V.50(3):151-6.<br />

Bender, L., Wolfson, W.Q. (1983), Boats in the <strong>art</strong> and fantasy <strong>of</strong> children, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Jul.,<br />

V.22(4):125-8.<br />

Benveniste, D. (1985), Picture-time: a nondirective approach to <strong>art</strong> psychotherapy, Arts in Psychotherapy ,<br />

Fall, V.12(3):171-180.<br />

Betensky, M. (1978), Phenomenology <strong>of</strong> self-expression in theory and practice, Confinia Psychiatrica ,<br />

V.21(1-3):31-36.<br />

Betensky, M. (1973), Patterns <strong>of</strong> visual expression in <strong>art</strong> psychotherapy, Art Psychotherapy , Fall,<br />

V.1(2):121-9.<br />

Billig, O. (1973), <strong>The</strong> schizophrenic "<strong>art</strong>ist's" expression <strong>of</strong> movement, Confinia Psychiatrica , V.16(1):1-<br />

27.<br />

Bishop, J. (1978), Creativity, <strong>art</strong> and play therapy, Canadian Counsellor , Jan., V.12(2):138-146.<br />

Bowers, J.J. (1992), <strong>The</strong>rapy through <strong>art</strong>. Facilitating treatment <strong>of</strong> sexual abuse, J. <strong>of</strong> Psychosocial<br />

Nursing and Mental Health Services , Jun., V.30(6):15-24.<br />

Breslow, D.M. (1993), Creative <strong>art</strong>s for hospitals: the UCLA experiment, Patient Education and<br />

Counselling , Jun., V.21(1-2):101-110.<br />

Brown, R.J. (1993), <strong>The</strong> fishing image: a preliminary study, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.20(2):167-171.<br />

Buchalter-Katz, S. (1985), Observations concerning the <strong>art</strong> productions <strong>of</strong> depressed patients in a shortterm<br />

psychiatric facility, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.12:35-8.<br />

Buck, L.A., Kardeman, E., Goldstein, F. (1985), Artistic talent in "autistic" adolescents and young adults,<br />

Empirical Studies <strong>of</strong> the Arts , V.3(1):81-104.<br />

Buckland, A., Bennett, D.L. (1995), Youth <strong>art</strong>s in hospital: engaging creativity in care, International<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Adolescent Medicine and Health , Jan-Mar., V.8(1):17-27.<br />

Burgess, A.W., H<strong>art</strong>man, C.R., Grant, C.A., Clover, C.L., Snyder, W., King, L.A. (1991), Drawing a<br />

377


connection from victim to victimizer, J. <strong>of</strong> Psychosocial Nursing and Mental Health Services , Dec.,<br />

V.29(12):9-14.<br />

Burkett, A.D. (1974), A way to communicate, Am. J. Nursing, Dec., V.74(12)::2185-7.<br />

Cameron, C.O., Juszezak, L., Wallace, N. (1984), Using creative <strong>art</strong>s to help children cope with altered<br />

body image, Children's Health Care , Win., V.12(3):108-12.<br />

Carnes, J.J. (1979), Toward a cognitive theory <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy, Art Psychotherapy , V.6(2):69-75.<br />

Carney, S. (1986), Symbol building in schizophrenic disorders, Pratt Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy<br />

Review , V.7:31-42.<br />

Cheyne-King, S.E. (1990), Effects <strong>of</strong> brain injury on visual perception and <strong>art</strong> production, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , Spr., V.17(1):69-74.<br />

Clark, W.H. (1977), Art and psychotherapy in Mexico, Art Psychotherapy , V.4(1):41-4.<br />

Cohen, B.M., Mills, A., Kijak, A.K. (1994), An introduction to the Diagnostic Drawing Series: a<br />

standardized tool for diagnostic and clinical use, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.11(2):105-10.<br />

Conroy, R.M., McDonnell, M., Swinney, J. (1986), Process-centred <strong>art</strong> therapy in anorexia nervosa,<br />

British Journal <strong>of</strong> Occupational <strong>The</strong>rapy , Oct., V.49(10):322-3.<br />

Cox, K.L., Price, K. (1990), Breaking through: incident drawings with adolescent substance abusers, Arts<br />

in Psychotherapy , Win., V.17(4):333-7.<br />

Creadick, T.A. (1985), <strong>The</strong> role <strong>of</strong> the Expressive Arts in therapy, J. <strong>of</strong> Reading, Writing, and Learning<br />

Disabilities International , Spr., V.1(3):55-60.<br />

Cuozzi, J.L., Kaplan, F.F. (1979), Surviving the death and dying <strong>of</strong> a treatment community, Art<br />

Psychotherapy , V.6(3):191-6.<br />

Dallin, B. (1986), Art Break: a 2 day expressive therapy program using <strong>art</strong> and psychodrama to further the<br />

termination process, Arts in Psychotherapy , Sum., V.13(2):137-42.<br />

Davis, C.B. (1989), <strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy and group process with grieving children, Issues in<br />

Comprehensive Paediatric Nursing , Jul-Aug., V.12(4):269-80.<br />

De-Nobel, C.A. (1972), Creative therapy: an autonomous discipline, Confinia Psychiatrica,<br />

V.15(1):77-81.<br />

Docherty, F. (1986), Steps in the progressive treatment <strong>of</strong> depression in the elderly, Physical and<br />

Occupational <strong>The</strong>rapy in Geriatrics , Fall, V.5(1):59-76.<br />

Dougherty, C.A. (1974), Group <strong>art</strong> therapy: a Jungian approach, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Apr., V.13(3):229-<br />

36.<br />

Eckardt, M.H. (1991), <strong>The</strong> ambivalent relationship <strong>of</strong> psychoanalysis and creativity: the contributions <strong>of</strong><br />

Suzanne Langer to the formative process <strong>of</strong> our psyche, J. <strong>of</strong> the Am. Academy <strong>of</strong> Psychoanalysis , Win.,<br />

V.19(4):620-9.<br />

Edelson, R.T. (1990), ART AND CRAFTS - not "<strong>art</strong>s and crafts": alternative vocational day activities<br />

for adults who are older and mentally retarded, Activities, Adaptation and Aging , V.15(1-2):81-97.<br />

Engle, P. (1997), Art therapy and dissociative disorders, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.14(4):246-254.<br />

Erickson, J.M. (1979), <strong>The</strong> <strong>art</strong>s and healing, Am. J. Art T herapy , Apr., V.18(3):75-80.<br />

Farrelly, J., Joseph, A. (1991), Expressive therapies in a crisis intervention service, Arts in Psychotherapy ,<br />

V.18(2):131-137.<br />

Feen-Calligan, H. (1995), <strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy in treatment programs to promote spiritual recovery from<br />

addiction, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.12(1):46-50.<br />

Feinstein, H. (1985), <strong>The</strong> metaphoric interpretation <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> for therapeutic purposes, Arts in Psychotherapy ,<br />

Fall, V.12(3):157-163.<br />

Fink, P.J., Levick, M.F. (1973), Sexual problems revealed through <strong>art</strong> therapy, Art Psychotherapy , Win.,<br />

V.1(3-4):277-291.<br />

Fink, P.J., Levick, M.F. (1973), Goldman, M.J., Art therapy: a diagnostic and therapeutic tool, Int. J.<br />

Psychiatry , Mar., V.11(1):104-118.<br />

Finley, P. (1975), Dialogue drawing: an image-evoking communication between analyst and analysand,<br />

Art Psychotherapy , V.2(1):87-99.<br />

Ford-Sori, C.E. (1995), <strong>The</strong> "<strong>art</strong>" <strong>of</strong> restructuring: integrating <strong>art</strong> with structural family therapy, J. Family<br />

Psychotherapy , V.6(2):13-31.<br />

Forrest, G. (1976), An <strong>art</strong> therapist beside herself, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Jul., V.15(4):105-111.<br />

Foulke, W.E., Keller, T.W. (1976), <strong>The</strong> <strong>art</strong> experience in addict rehabilitation, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Apr.,<br />

V.15(3):75-80.<br />

Friedman, A.S., Glickman, N.W. (1986), Program characteristics for successful treatment <strong>of</strong> adolescent<br />

378


drug abuse, J. Nervous and Mental Disease , Nov., V.174(11):669-679.<br />

Frye, B. (1990), Art and multiple personality disorder: an expressive framework for occupational therapy,<br />

Am. J. <strong>of</strong> Occupational <strong>The</strong>rapy , Nov., V.44(11):1013-1022.<br />

Fuhrman, N.L., Zingaro, J.C., Kokenes, B. (1990), A preliminary comparative study <strong>of</strong> drawings<br />

produced under hypnosis and in a simulated state by both MPD and non-MPD adults, Dissociation<br />

Progress in the Dissociative Disorders , Jun. V.3(2):107-112.<br />

Garai, J.E. (1973), Reflections <strong>of</strong> the struggle for identity in <strong>art</strong> therapy, Art Psychotherapy , Win., V.1(3-<br />

4):261-275.<br />

Garai, J.E. (1976), New Vistas in the exploration <strong>of</strong> inner and outer space through <strong>art</strong> therapy, Art<br />

Psychotherapy , V.3(3-4):157-167.<br />

Garai, J.E. (1984), New horizons <strong>of</strong> holistic healing through creative expression, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , May,<br />

V.1(2):76-82.<br />

Gerber, J. (1994), <strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy in juvenile sex <strong>of</strong>fender specific treatment, Arts in Psychotherapy ,<br />

V.21(5):367-374.<br />

Gerritsen, M. (1995), Art therapy: the real <strong>art</strong> is the process, <strong>The</strong>rapeutic Communities International J. for<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapeutic and Supportive Organizations , Spr., V.16(1):25-35.<br />

Ghadirian, A.M. (1978), Graphic <strong>art</strong> and its relation to conceptual thinking, Confinia Psychiatrica , V.21(1-<br />

3):165-169.<br />

Ghadirian, A.M. (1974), Artistic expression <strong>of</strong> <strong>psychopathology</strong> through the media <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy,<br />

Confinia Psychiatrica , V.17(3-4):162-170.<br />

Glaister, J.A., McGuinness, T. (1992), <strong>The</strong> <strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> therapeutic drawing: Helping chronic trauma survivors,<br />

J. Psychosocial Nursin g and Mental Health Services , May, V.30(5):9-17.<br />

Goldstein-Roca, S., Crisafulli, T. (1994), Integrative creative <strong>art</strong>s therapy: a brief treatment model, Arts<br />

in Psychotherapy , V.21(3):219-222.<br />

Golub, D. (1985), Symbolic expression in post-traumatic stress disorder: Vietnam combat veterans in <strong>art</strong><br />

therapy, Arts in Psychotherapy , Win., V.12(4):285-296.<br />

Gorelick, K. (1989), Rapprochement between the <strong>art</strong>s and psychotherapies: metaphor the mediator, Arts<br />

in Psychotherapy , Fall V.16(3):149-155.<br />

Hagood, M.M. (1991), Group <strong>art</strong> therapy with mothers <strong>of</strong> sexually abused children, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , Spr., V.18(1):17-27.<br />

Halbreich, U., Assael, M. (1979), Drawings <strong>of</strong> cephalopods by schizophrenic patients, and their meaning,<br />

Art Psychotherapy , V.6(1):19-23.<br />

Halbreich, U. (1978), <strong>The</strong> application <strong>of</strong> principles <strong>of</strong> short-term, problem-oriented psychotherapy to <strong>art</strong><br />

psychotherapy, Art Psychotherapy , V.5(4):181-9.<br />

Halbreich, U. (1978), A nonverbal dialogue as a treatment <strong>of</strong> schizophrenic patients, Confinia Psychiatrica ,<br />

V.21(1-3):58-67.<br />

Harlan, J.E. (1990), <strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy for older adults with developmental disabilities, Activities,<br />

Adaptation and Aging , V.15(1-2):67-79.<br />

Harlan, J.E. (1993), <strong>The</strong> therapeutic value <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> for persons with Alzheimer's disease and related disorders,<br />

Loss, Grief and Care , V.6(4):99-106.<br />

Harms, E. (1973), Art psychotherapy and the prophylaxis <strong>of</strong> psychic healing, Art Psychotherapy , Win.,<br />

V.1(3-4):185-192.<br />

Harvey, S. (1990), Dynamic paly therapy: An integrative expressive <strong>art</strong>s approach to the family therapy<br />

<strong>of</strong> young children, Arts in Psychotherapy , Fall, V.17(3):239-246.<br />

Head, V.B. (1975), Experiences with <strong>art</strong> therapy in short term groups <strong>of</strong> day clinic addicted patients,<br />

Ontario Psychologist , Oct., V.7(4):42-49.<br />

Heimlich, M., (1972), Paraverbal techniques in the therapy <strong>of</strong> childhood communication disorder Int. J.<br />

Child Psychotherapy , Jan. V.1(1):65-83.<br />

Heine, D., Steiner, M. (1986), Standardized paintings as a proposed adjunct instrument for longitudinal<br />

monitoring <strong>of</strong> mood states: a preliminary note, Occupational <strong>The</strong>rapy in Mental Health , Fall, V.6(3):31-7.<br />

Heineman, L. (1992), Seeking a good enough mirror: <strong>art</strong> therapy and mirroring in a prevention program's<br />

parent training group, Pratt Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.13:23-30.<br />

Henley, D.R. (1992), Aesthetics in <strong>art</strong> therapy: theory into practice, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.19(3):153-<br />

161.<br />

Henley, D.R. (1989), Art therapeutic interventions using Lowenfeld's motivational techniques: stereotypes<br />

in children's <strong>art</strong>, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , May, V.27:116-125.<br />

379


Hodnett, M.L. (1973), A broader view <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy, Art Psychotherapy , Fall, V.1(2):75-79.<br />

Honig, S. (1975), Ideation in the <strong>art</strong>work <strong>of</strong> suicidal patients, Art Psychotherapy , V.2:77-85.<br />

Hunyady, H. (1984), A report <strong>of</strong> a drawing therapy for children's nightmares, J. Evolutionary Psychology ,<br />

Mar., V.5(1-2):129-30.<br />

Hymes, S.M. (1983), <strong>The</strong> therapeutic nature <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> in self reparation, Psychoanalysis Review , Spr.,<br />

V.70(1):57-68.<br />

Jakab, I. (1976), Comprehensive view <strong>of</strong> the creative process in psychopathological <strong>art</strong>: a panel discussion<br />

at the 7th International Congress <strong>of</strong> Psychopathology <strong>of</strong> Expression, Confinia Psychiatrica , V.19(4):177-<br />

206.<br />

James, R.T., Burrows, T.M. (1981), Right brain exercises and whole brain medicine, J. Holistic<br />

Medicine , Fall-Win., V.3(2):152-6.<br />

Jenkins, H., Donnelly, M. (1983), <strong>The</strong> therapist's responsibility: a systemic approach to mobilizing family<br />

creativity, J. Family <strong>The</strong>rapy , Aug., V.5(3):199-218.<br />

Johnson, C., Lahey, P.P., Shore, A. (1992), An exploration <strong>of</strong> creative <strong>art</strong>s therapeutic group work on an<br />

Alzheimer's unit, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.19(4):269-277.<br />

Johnson, D.R. (1987), <strong>The</strong> role <strong>of</strong> the creative <strong>art</strong>s therapies in the diagnosis and treatment <strong>of</strong><br />

psychological trauma, Arts in Psychotherapy , Spr., V.14(1):7-13.<br />

Johnson, L. (1990), Creative therapies in the treatment <strong>of</strong> addictions: the <strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> transforming shame, Arts<br />

in Psychotherapy , Win., V.17(4):299-308.<br />

Jones, D. (1978), Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, Art Psychotherapy , V.5(1):11-12.<br />

Joraski, M.F. (1986), <strong>The</strong> role <strong>of</strong> creative <strong>art</strong>s in cognitive rehabilitation, Cognitive Rehabilitation , Mar-<br />

Apr., V.4(2):18-23.<br />

Kagin, S.L., Lusebrink, V.B. (1978), <strong>The</strong> expressive therapies continuum, Art Psychotherapy , V.5(4):171-<br />

180.<br />

Kaslow, N.J., Eichner, V.W. (1988), Body image therapy: a combined creative <strong>art</strong>s therapy and verbal<br />

psychotherapy approach, Arts in Psychotherapy , Fall, V.15(3):177-188.<br />

Kelly, C.R. (1988), Expressive therapy assessment, Arts in Psychotherapy , Spr., V.15(1):63-70.<br />

Kidd, J., Wix, L. (1996) Images <strong>of</strong> the he<strong>art</strong>: archetypal imagery in therapeutic <strong>art</strong>work, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy ,<br />

V.13(2):108-13.<br />

Killick, K. (1993), Working with psychotic processes in <strong>art</strong> therapy, Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy ,<br />

V.7(1):25-38.<br />

Kivalo, A. (1978), Art therapy with children, Psychiatria Fennica p.93-102.<br />

Kramer, E.S. (1982), <strong>The</strong> history <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy in a large mental hospital, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Apr.,<br />

V.21(3):75-84.<br />

Kramer, E.S. (1977), Art therapy and play, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Oct., V.17(1):3-11.<br />

Kramer, E. (1986), <strong>The</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapist's third hand: reflections on <strong>art</strong>, <strong>art</strong> therapy, and society at large, Am.<br />

J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Feb., V.24(3):71-86.<br />

Kramer, E., Schehr, J. (1983), An <strong>art</strong> therapy evaluation session for children, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Oct.,<br />

V.23(1):3-12.<br />

Lachman, M., Stuntz, E.C., Jones, N. (1975), Art therapy in the psychotherapy <strong>of</strong> a mother and her son,<br />

Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Jul., V.14(4):105-116.<br />

Lachman-Chapin, M. (1985), Ericksonian hypnosis and <strong>art</strong> therapy, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , May,<br />

V.23(4):115-124.<br />

Lachman-Chapin, M (1983), Making verbal the nonverbal: a commentary, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Oct., V.1(1):47-9.<br />

Lanc, J. (1982), "Encapsulating" and "examining" schizophrenics: proposal for treatment within a short<br />

term framework, Pratt Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.3:49-57.<br />

Landg<strong>art</strong>en, H., Junge, M., Tasem, M., Watson, M. (1978), Art therapy as a modality for crisis<br />

intervention: children express reactions to violence in their community, Clinical Social Work Journal ,<br />

Fall, V.6(3):221-229.<br />

Landg<strong>art</strong>en, H. (1983), Art psychotherapy for depressed elders, Clinical Gerontologist , Fall, V.2(1):45-53.<br />

Landg<strong>art</strong>en, H. (1981), Family <strong>art</strong> psychotherapy, Int. J. Family Psychiatry , V.2(3-4):379-395.<br />

Landg<strong>art</strong>en, H. (1975), Adult <strong>art</strong> psychotherapy, Art Psychotherapy , V.2(1):65-76.<br />

Lawlor, E.M. (1992), Creativity and change: the two-tiered creative <strong>art</strong>s therapy approach to co-dependency<br />

treatment, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.19(1):19-27.<br />

Leedy, J.J. (1973), Poetry therapy and some links to <strong>art</strong> therapy, Art Psychotherapy , Fall, V.1(2):145-151.<br />

380


Lerner, A. (1984), Some observations and comments on symposium held June 22-24 1984: <strong>The</strong> creative<br />

<strong>art</strong>s in therapy as an integral p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> treatment for the 90s: Looking ahead - planning together, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , Win., V.11(4):293-5.<br />

Levens, M. (1990), Borderline aspects in eating disorders: <strong>art</strong> therapy's contribution, Group Analysis ,<br />

Sep., V.23(3):277-284.<br />

Levick, M. (1975), Transference and counter-transference as manifested in graphic productions, Art<br />

Psychotherapy , V.2(3-4):203-215.<br />

Levick, M. (1978), Response to paper by Dr. Edwin Hammer, Art Psych otherapy , V.5(1):31-33.<br />

Levine, S.K. (1994), Order and chaos in therapy and the <strong>art</strong>s: an encounter with Rudolf Arnheim, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , V.21(4):269-278.<br />

Levy, B. (1978), Art therapy in a women's correctional facility, Art Psychotherapy , V.5(3):157-166.<br />

Lewis, P.P. (1988), <strong>The</strong> transformative process within the imaginal realm, Arts in Psychotherapy , Win.,<br />

V.15(4):309-316.<br />

Lincoln, L. (1987), Body image remediation through creative <strong>art</strong>s therapy, Pratt Institute Creative Arts<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.8:35-44.<br />

Linden, J. (1985), Insight through metaphor in psychotherapy and creativity, Psychoanalysis and<br />

Contemporary Thought , V.8(3):375-406.<br />

Linesch, D. (1994), Interpretation in <strong>art</strong> therapy research and practice: the hermeneutic circle, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , V.21(3):185-195.<br />

Loo, C.M. (1974), <strong>The</strong> self-puzzle: a diagnostic and therapeutic tool, J. Personality Assessment , Jun.,<br />

V.38(3):236-242.<br />

Lorenzetti, M. (1994), (trans. A. C<strong>of</strong>fetti), Perspectives on integration between <strong>art</strong>s therapy areas, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , V.21(2):113-117.<br />

Lund, C., Ormerod, E., George, K. (1986), Art group psychotherapy in a psychiatric day unit, British<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry , Oct., V.149:152-515.<br />

Lyon, J.G. (1995), Communicative drawing: an augmentative mode <strong>of</strong> interaction, Aphasiology,<br />

Jan-Feb.,<br />

V.9(1):84-94.<br />

Lyon, J.G.; Helm-Estabrooks, N. (1987), Drawing: Its communicative significance for expressively<br />

restricted aphasic adults, Topics in Language Disorders , Dec. V.8(1):61-71.<br />

Maclagan, D. (1995), Fantasy and the aesthetic: have they become the uninvited guests at <strong>art</strong> therapy's<br />

feast? Arts in Psychotherapy , V.22(3):217-221.<br />

Macrae, M., Smith, G. (1973), Combining music with <strong>art</strong> psychotherapy, Art Psychotherapy , Win.,<br />

V.1(3-4):229-241.<br />

Marion, P., Felix, M. (1980), From denial to self-esteem: <strong>art</strong> therapy with the mentally retarded, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , V.7(3):201-5.<br />

Mango, C. (1993), <strong>The</strong> oral matrix, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.20(50:403-10.<br />

M<strong>art</strong>in, E. (1997), <strong>The</strong> symbolic graphic life line: integrating the past and present through graphic<br />

imagery, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy V.14(4):261-267.<br />

McNeilly, G. (1983), Directive and non-directive approaches in <strong>art</strong> therapy, Arts in Psychotherapy , Win.,<br />

V.10(4):211-219.<br />

McNeilly, G. (1990), Group analysis and <strong>art</strong> therapy: a personal perspective, Group Analysis , Sep.,<br />

V.23(3):215-24.<br />

McNiff, S. (1979), From shamanism to <strong>art</strong> therapy, Art Psychotherapy , V.6(3):155-161.<br />

McNiff, S. (1975), On <strong>art</strong> therapy: a conversation with Rudolf Arnheim, Art Psychotherapy , V.2(3-4):195-<br />

202.<br />

McSweeney, M. (1990), <strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> transitional space within an expressive therapy relationship, Pratt<br />

Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.11:63-71.<br />

McWhinnie, H.J. (1985), Carl Jung and Heinz Werner and implications for foundational studies in <strong>art</strong><br />

education and <strong>art</strong> therapy, Arts in Psychothe rapy , Sum., V.12(2):95-99.<br />

Melanson, G.M. (1985), Gesture drawing: an avenue to personal myth, Saybrook Review , Fall-Win.,<br />

V.5(2):73-82.<br />

Miller, M.G. (1986) Art - a creative teaching tool, Academic <strong>The</strong>rapy , Sep., V.22(1):53-6.<br />

Mitchell, D. (1978), A note on <strong>art</strong> psychotherapy and poetry therapy: the coordination <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> and poetry<br />

as an expressive technique, Art Psychotherapy , V.5(4):223-5.<br />

Mitzushima, K. (1971), Art therapies in Japan, Interpersonal Development , V.2(4):213-221.<br />

381


Muller-Braunschweig, H. (1975), Psychopathology and creativity, Psychoanalytic <strong>Study</strong> <strong>of</strong> Society ,<br />

V.67:1-99.<br />

Mullins, J.B. (1973), <strong>The</strong> expressive therapies in special education, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Oct., V.13(1):52-<br />

8.<br />

Nagaraja, J. (1975), Psycho-iconography and the mentally sick, Child Psychiatry Qu<strong>art</strong>erly , Jan., V.8(1):6-<br />

13.<br />

Naitove, C.E. (1988), Arts therapy with child molesters: an historical perspective on the act and an<br />

approach to treatment, Arts in Psychotherapy , Sum., V.15(2):151-160.<br />

Naitove, C.E. (1985), Protecting our children: the fight against molestation, Arts in Psychotherapy , Sum.,<br />

V.12(2):115-6.<br />

Naitove, C.E. (1978), Symbolic patterns in drawings by habitual users <strong>of</strong> street drugs: a pilot study,<br />

Confinia Psychiatrica , V.21(1-3):112-8.<br />

Nathan, T.S., Hesse, P.P. (1978), Developmental and interactional aspects <strong>of</strong> creative expression in the<br />

course <strong>of</strong> group therapy, Confinia Psychiatrica , V.21(1-3):119-132.<br />

Nez, D. (1991), Persephone's return: archetypal <strong>art</strong> therapy and the treatment <strong>of</strong> a survivor <strong>of</strong> abuse, Arts<br />

in Psychotherap y , V.18(2):123-130.<br />

Nielsen, K.E. (1982), Creative <strong>art</strong>s therapy as a tool in promoting ego integration in delinquent adolescent<br />

girls, Pratt Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.3:21-31.<br />

Noah-Cooper, C.L., Richards, R.G. (1983), Art therapy for an angry child: a case study, Academic<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy , V.18(5):575-81.<br />

Nystul, M. (1987), Strategies for parent-centred counselling <strong>of</strong> the young, Creative Child and Adult<br />

Qu<strong>art</strong>erly , Sum., V.12(2):103-110.<br />

Obernbreit, R. (1985), Object relations theory and the language <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>: tools for treatment <strong>of</strong> the borderline<br />

patient, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Mar., V.2(1):11-18.<br />

Obernbreit, R. (1980), Art <strong>The</strong>rapy: agent in education, Pratt Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review ,<br />

V.1:59-66.<br />

Ormay, A.P. (1990), Art as communication: a group analytic view, Group Analysis , Dec., V.3(4):377-<br />

389.<br />

Parciack, R., Winnik, H.Z., Shmueli, M. (1975), Aggression in painting: painting as a means <strong>of</strong> release<br />

<strong>of</strong> aggression, Mental Health and Society , V.2(3-6):225-237.<br />

Percoskie, S. (1997) Art therapy with the Alzheimer's client, Humanistic Psychologist , Summer,<br />

V.25(2):208-11.<br />

Perry, J.W. (1973), <strong>The</strong> creative element in madness, Art Psychotherapy Apr., V.1(1):61-5.<br />

Pickford, R.W. (1974), Aspects <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy, British J. <strong>of</strong> Projective Psychology and Personality <strong>Study</strong> ,<br />

Jun., V.19(1):16-20.<br />

Poldinger, W. (1987), <strong>The</strong> relation between depression and <strong>art</strong>. Psychopathology , Feb., V.19<br />

(Suppl.2):263-268.<br />

Poldinger, W., Krambeck, K. (1987), <strong>The</strong> relevance <strong>of</strong> creativity for psychiatric therapy and rehabilitation,<br />

Comprehensive Psychiatry , Sep-Oct., V.28(5):384-388.<br />

Poore, M. (1977), Art therapy in a vocational rehabilitation center, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Jan. V.16(2):55-9.<br />

Potocek, J. Wilder, V.N. (1989), Art/movement psychotherapy in the treatment <strong>of</strong> the chemically<br />

dependent patient, Arts in Psychotherapy , Sum., V.16(2):99-103.<br />

Powell, L., Faherty, S.L. (1990), Treating sexually abused latency age girls: a 20 session treatment plan<br />

utilizing group process and the creative <strong>art</strong>s therapies, Arts in Psychotherapy , Spr., V.17(1):35-47.<br />

Powers, P.S., Langworthy, J. (1978), Art work: another dimension in the treatment <strong>of</strong> psychiatric patients,<br />

Art Psychotherapy , V.5(2):71-9.<br />

Prager, A. (1995), Paediatric <strong>art</strong> therapy: strategies and applications, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.12(1):32-8.<br />

Pulliam, J.C. (1988), Three heads are better than one: the expressive <strong>art</strong>s group assessment, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , Spr., V.15(1):71-7.<br />

Rabinowitz, J. (1985), Time-lapse family portrait: the use <strong>of</strong> drawings in family therapy, Family <strong>The</strong>rapy ,<br />

V.12(3):303-9.<br />

Reiner, E.R., Tellin, J.A., O'Reilly, J.B. (1977), A picture regression scale for adults, Art Psychotherapy ,<br />

V.4:219-223.<br />

Rhineh<strong>art</strong>, L., Engelhorn, P. (1982), Pre-image considerations as a therapeutic process, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , Spr., V.9(1):55-63.<br />

Rhyne, J. (1973), <strong>The</strong> gestalt approach to experience, <strong>art</strong> and <strong>art</strong> therapy, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Jul.,<br />

382


V.12(4):237-248.<br />

Riley, S. (1997) Social Constructivism: the narrative approach to clinical <strong>art</strong> therapy, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy ,<br />

V.14(4):282-284.<br />

Riley, S. (1993), Illustrating the family story: <strong>art</strong> therapy, a lens for viewing the family's reality, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , V.20(3):253-264.<br />

Riley, S. (1987), <strong>The</strong> advantages <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy in an outpatient clinic, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.26(1):21-29.<br />

Riley, S. (1994), Rethinking adolescent <strong>art</strong> therapy treatment, J. Child and Adolescent Group <strong>The</strong>rapy ,<br />

Jun., V.4(2):81-97.<br />

Robbins, A. (1973), A psychoanalytic prospective towards the inter relationship <strong>of</strong> the creative process<br />

and the functions <strong>of</strong> an <strong>art</strong> therapist, Art Psychotherapy , Apr., V.1(1):7-12.<br />

Robbins, A. (1992), <strong>The</strong> play <strong>of</strong> psychotherapeutic <strong>art</strong>istry and psychoaesthetics, Arts in Psychotherapy ,<br />

V.19(3):177-186.<br />

Robbins, A. (1973), <strong>The</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapist's imagery as a response to a therapeutic dialogue, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Win.,<br />

V.1(3-4):181-184.<br />

Robbins, A. (1988), A psychoaesthetic perspective on creative <strong>art</strong>s therapy and training, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , Sum., V.15(2):95-100.<br />

Rogers, P. (1987), <strong>The</strong> healing evocation <strong>of</strong> beauty, Pratt Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.8:1-<br />

13.<br />

Roje, J. (1994) Consciousness as manifested in <strong>art</strong>: a journey from the concrete to the meaningful, Arts<br />

in Psychotherapy , V.21(5):375-385.<br />

Rosling, L.K., Kitchen, J. (1992), Music and drawing with institutionalised elderly, Activities,<br />

Adaptation and Aging , V.17(2):27-38.<br />

Rosman, Y., Assael, M., Gabbay, F. (1975), Spontaneous group drawing, Mental Health and Society ,<br />

V.2(3-6):238-242.<br />

Rothenberg, A. (1987), Empathy as a creative process in treatment, Int. Review <strong>of</strong> Psycho-Analysis ,<br />

V.14(4):445-463.<br />

Rubin, J.A. (1985), Imagery in <strong>art</strong> therapy: the source, the setting and the significance, Journal <strong>of</strong> Mental<br />

Imagery , Win., V.9(4):71-81.<br />

Rubin, J.A. (1973), A diagnostic <strong>art</strong> interview, Art Psychotherapy , Apr., V.1(1):31-43.<br />

Rubin, J.A. (1988), Art Counselling: an alternative, Elementary School Guidance and Counselling , Feb.,<br />

V.22(3):180-5.<br />

Rubin, J.A. (1981), Art therapy in a community mental health center for children: a story <strong>of</strong> program<br />

development, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.8(2):109-114.<br />

Rusek, J. (1991), A creative approach to the treatment <strong>of</strong> resistance, Pratt Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy<br />

Review , V.12:9-15.<br />

Rush, K. (1978), <strong>The</strong> metaphorical journey: <strong>art</strong> therapy in symbolic exploration, Art Psychotherapy ,<br />

V.5(3):149-155.<br />

Sagal, R. (1990), Helping older mentally retarded persons expand their socialization skills through the use<br />

<strong>of</strong> expressive therapies, Activities, Adaptation and Aging , V.15(1-2):99-109.<br />

Scanlon, K. (1993), Art therapy with autistic children, Pratt Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review ,<br />

V.14:34-43.<br />

Schaverien, J. (1994), <strong>The</strong> transactional object: <strong>art</strong> psychotherapy in the treatment <strong>of</strong> anorexia, British J.<br />

<strong>of</strong> Psychotherapy, Fall, V.11(1):46-61.<br />

Schmais, C. (1988), Creative <strong>art</strong>s therapies and shamanism: a comparison, Arts in Psychotherapy , Win.,<br />

V.15(4):281-284.<br />

Seftel, L. (1987), Understanding destruction in <strong>art</strong> therapy with children, Pratt Institute Creative Arts<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.8:27-34.<br />

Segal, R.M. (1984), Helping children express grief through symbolic communication, Social Casework ,<br />

Dec., V.65(10):590-599.<br />

Shaughnessy, M.F., Tevelowitz, N. (1981), Creativity in <strong>art</strong> with the retarded, Creative Child and Adult<br />

Qu<strong>art</strong>erly , Fall, V.6(3):141-146.<br />

Sheahan, M. (1974), Picture your problems, Menninger Perspective , Sum., V.5(2)::16-21.<br />

Sherr, C. (1973), <strong>The</strong>rapeutic use <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>work in a community mental health center, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy ,<br />

Apr., V.12(3):183-190.<br />

Shoemaker, R. (1978), <strong>The</strong> significance <strong>of</strong> the first picture in <strong>art</strong> therapy, Proceedings <strong>of</strong> the 8th Annual<br />

Conference <strong>of</strong> the American Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Association , p.156-162.<br />

383


Siegel, L. (1988), <strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> mural and metaphor with a schizophrenic population for recovery in a trauma<br />

situation, Pratt Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.9:40-53.<br />

Simon, E.G. (1989), An <strong>art</strong> therapy approach to dream interpretation, Pratt Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy<br />

Review , V.10:51-60.<br />

Simon, R.M. (1975), Art: a strategic and empirical therapy? Confinia Psychiatrica , V.18(3)::174-182.<br />

Simon, R. (1974), Pictorial style as a means <strong>of</strong> communication, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Jul., V.13(4):275-<br />

292.<br />

Sizemore, C.C. (1986), "On my life with multiple personalities": comments by Chris Costner Sizemore,<br />

Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Mar., V.3(1):18-20.<br />

Sladyk, K. (1992), Traumatic brain injury, behavioral disorder, and group treatment, Am. J. Occupational<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy , Mar., V.46(3):267-270.<br />

Slap, J.W. (1976), A note <strong>of</strong> the drawing <strong>of</strong> dream details, Psychoanalytic Qu<strong>art</strong>erly , V.45(3):455-6.<br />

Smith, D.D. (1983), <strong>The</strong> <strong>art</strong> evaluation: a triage function on a psychiatric emergency admissions unit, Arts<br />

in Psychotherapy , Fall, V.10(3):187-195.<br />

Smith, G.M. (1985), <strong>The</strong> Collaborative Drawing Technique, J. Personality Assessment , Dec.,<br />

V.49(6):582-5.<br />

Spitz, E.H. (1989), <strong>The</strong> world <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> and the <strong>art</strong>ful world: some common fantasies in creativity and<br />

<strong>psychopathology</strong>, Arts in Psychotherapy , Win., V.16(4):243-251.<br />

Strand, S. (1990), Counteracting isolation: group <strong>art</strong> therapy for people with learning difficulties, Group<br />

Analysis , Sep., V.23(3):255-263.<br />

Swenson, A.B. (1991), Relationships: <strong>art</strong> education, <strong>art</strong> therapy, and special education, Perceptual and<br />

Motor Skills , Feb., V.72(1):40-42.<br />

Syristova, E. (1989), <strong>The</strong> creative potential <strong>of</strong> schizophrenic psychosis: its importance for psychotherapy,<br />

Studia Psychologica , V.31(4):283-294.<br />

Talerico, C.J. (1986), <strong>The</strong> expressive <strong>art</strong>s and creativity as a form <strong>of</strong> therapeutic experience in the field <strong>of</strong><br />

mental health, J. Creative Behavior , V.20(4):229-247.<br />

Tate, F.B. (1989), Symbols in the graphic <strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the dying, Arts in Psychotherapy , Sum., V.16(2):115-<br />

120.<br />

Tate, F.B., Allen, H. (1985), Color preferences and the aged individual: implications for <strong>art</strong> therapy, Arts<br />

in Psychotherapy , Fall, V.12(3):165-169.<br />

Taylor, J.W. (1990), <strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> nonverbal expression with incestuous clients, Families in Society , Dec.,<br />

V.71(10):597-601.<br />

Temple, S. (1988), Erickson's model <strong>of</strong> personality development related to clinical material, British J.<br />

Occupational <strong>The</strong>rapy , Nov., V.51(11):399-402.<br />

Tokuda, Y. (1973), Image and <strong>art</strong> therapy, Art Psychotherapy , Win., V.1(3-4):169-176.<br />

Tokuda, Y. (1980), <strong>The</strong>ory and practice <strong>of</strong> image <strong>art</strong> psychotherapy, Confinia Psychiatrica , V.23(4):193-<br />

208.<br />

Ulman, E. (1977), Art education for the emotionally disturbed, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Oct., V.17(1):13-16.<br />

Vaccaro, V.M. (1973), Specific aspects <strong>of</strong> the psychology <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy, Art Psychotherapy , Fall,<br />

V.1(2):81-89.<br />

Van-Krevelen, D.A. (1975), On the use <strong>of</strong> the family drawing test, Acta Paedopsychiatrica,<br />

V.41(3):104-9.<br />

Vassiliou, G.A. (1983), Analogic communication as a means <strong>of</strong> joining the family system in therapy, Int.<br />

J. Family Psychiatry , V.4(3):173-9.<br />

Vogl, J.M., Vogl, G.M. (1983), Group <strong>art</strong> therapy: an eclectic approach, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Jul.,<br />

V.22(4):129-135.<br />

Wadeson, H. (1973), Separateness, Art Psychotherapy , Fall, V.1(2):131-3.<br />

Wadeson, H. (1975), Suicide: expression in images, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Apr., V.14(3):75-82.<br />

Wadeson, H. (1976), Combining expressive therapies, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Jan., V.15(2):43-6.<br />

Wadeson, H. (1976), <strong>The</strong> fluid family in multi-family <strong>art</strong> therapy, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Jul. V.15(4):115-<br />

8.<br />

Wald, J. (1983), Alzheimer's disease and the role <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy in its treatment, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Jan.,<br />

V.22(2):57-64.<br />

Wald, J. (1986), Art therapy for patients with dementing illnesses, Clinical Gerontologist , Feb.,<br />

V.4(3):29-40.<br />

Weininger, O. (1987), <strong>The</strong> Differential Diagnostic Technique, a visual motor projective test, Perceptual<br />

384


and Motor Skills , Aug., V.65(1):76-8.<br />

Weiss, J.C. (1984), Expressive therapy with elders and the disabled: Touching the he<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> life, Activities,<br />

Adaptation and Aging, May, V.5(1-2):213.<br />

Wilkinson, S. (1985), Drawing up boundaries: a technique, J. Family <strong>The</strong>rapy , May, V.7(2):99-111.<br />

Willmuth, M., Boedy, D.L. (1979), <strong>The</strong> verbal diagnostic and <strong>art</strong> therapy combined: an extended<br />

evaluation procedure with family groups, Art Psychotherapy , V.6(1):11-18.<br />

Wilson, L. (1985), Symbolism and <strong>art</strong> therapy: I. Symbolism's role in the development <strong>of</strong> ego functions,<br />

Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Feb., V.23(3):79-88.<br />

Wilson, L. (1985) Symbolism and <strong>art</strong> therapy: II. Symbolism's relationship to basic psychic functioning,<br />

Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , May, V.23(4):129-133.<br />

Wilson, L. (1977), <strong>The</strong>ory and practice <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy with the mentally retarded, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy ,<br />

Apr., V.16(3):87-97.<br />

Wittels, B. (1978), Jung, <strong>art</strong> therapy and the psychotic patient, Art Psychotherapy , V.5(3):115-121.<br />

Wittels, B. (1975), <strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> to master symptoms as demonstrated in the <strong>art</strong> work <strong>of</strong> acutely<br />

psychotic patients, Art Psychotherapy , V.2(3-4):217-224.<br />

Wittenberg, D. (1974), Art therapy for adolescent drug abusers, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Jan., V.13(2):141-9.<br />

Wolf, R. (1979), Re-experiencing Winnicott's environmental mother: implications for <strong>art</strong> psychotherapy<br />

<strong>of</strong> anti-social youth in special education, Art Psychotherapy , V.6(2):95-102.<br />

Wolff, R.A. (1975), <strong>The</strong>rapeutic experiences through group <strong>art</strong> expression, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Apr.,<br />

V.14(3):91-8.<br />

Ziegler, R.G. (1976), Winnicott's squiggle game: Its diagnostic and therapeutic usefulness, Art<br />

Psychotherapy , V.3(3-4):177-185.<br />

Zigmund, J. (1986), Relating developmental level to <strong>art</strong> materials in work with paranoid and schizoid<br />

personalities, Pratt Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.7:1-12.<br />

Zlatin, H.P. (1979), "I never had a chance": Art therapy at a geriatric center, Art Psychotherapy ,<br />

V.6(2):119-123.<br />

Case studies <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>ists<br />

9 Bader, A. (1971), Psychopathological discovery <strong>of</strong> Charles Filiger, symbolist painter, Confinia<br />

Psychiatrica , V.14(1):18-35.<br />

1 Billig, O. (1972), Is schizophrenic expression <strong>art</strong>? Confinia Psychiatrica , V.15(1):49-54.<br />

26 Dreifuss, G. (1978), Artists in the creative process <strong>of</strong> Jungian analysis, Confinia Psychiatrica , V.21(1-<br />

3):45-50.<br />

16 Fernandez, A., Seva, A. (1994), A discovery throwing light on the illness <strong>of</strong> F. de Goya Lucientes,<br />

History <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry , Mar., V.5(17 p<strong>art</strong> 1):97-102.<br />

14 Gaillard, J.M. (1992), <strong>The</strong> expression <strong>of</strong> psychodynamic forces in the paintings <strong>of</strong> Modigliani,<br />

International J. <strong>of</strong> Short Term Psychotherapy , Apr., V.7(2):109-122.<br />

4 Garai, J.E. (1974), <strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> painting to resolve an <strong>art</strong>ist's identity conflicts, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Jan.,<br />

V.13(2):151-164.<br />

28 Genser, L. (1985), Art as therapy with an aging <strong>art</strong>ist, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, Feb. V.23(3):93-99.<br />

24 Junge, M.B. (1994), <strong>The</strong> perception <strong>of</strong> doors: A sociodynamic investigation <strong>of</strong> doors in 20th century<br />

painting, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.21(5):343-357.<br />

10 MacGregor, J.M. (1977), European collections <strong>of</strong> psychiatric <strong>art</strong>: A brief chronological and very<br />

personal account <strong>of</strong> a 1-month tour <strong>of</strong> collections in Switzerland, Italy, Germany and Austria undertaken<br />

in March and April, 1976, Confinia Psychiatrica , V.20(1):1-9.<br />

19 M<strong>art</strong>in, S.A. (1988), M<strong>art</strong>in Ramirez: Psychological hero, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.15(3):189-205.<br />

27 Meissner, W.W. (1994), Vincent Van Gogh as <strong>art</strong>ist: a psychoanalytic reflection, Annual <strong>of</strong><br />

Psychoanalysis , V.22:111-141.<br />

Miller, R. (1990), Empathy and hierarchy: a response to Sass, New Ideas in Psychology , V.8(3):305-7.<br />

29 Muenchow, D.C., Aresenian, J. (1974), An <strong>art</strong>ist in turmoil during <strong>art</strong> therapy, Am. J. <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapy ,<br />

Oct., V.14(1):18-23.<br />

5 Or-Halbreich, T., Halbreich, U. (1977), A comparison between mental patients' paintings and the action<br />

painting movement, Art Psychotherapy , V.4(1):15-18.<br />

6 Parsons, P. (1986), Outsider <strong>art</strong>: patient <strong>art</strong> enters the <strong>art</strong> world, Am. J. <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Aug.,<br />

V.25(1):3-12.<br />

25 Peto, A. (1979), <strong>The</strong> Rondanini Pieta: Michelanelo's infantile neurosis, International Review <strong>of</strong><br />

385


Psycho-Analysis , V.6(2):183-200.<br />

3 Poldinger, W. (1986), <strong>The</strong> relation between depression and <strong>art</strong>, Psychopathology , V.19 suppl.2:263-8.<br />

11 Postma, J.U. (1993), Did Rembrandt suffer from depressive periods? A photo-analytic study <strong>of</strong> his self<br />

portraits, European J. <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry , Jul-Sep., V.7(3):180-184.<br />

21 Roman, M., Stastny, P. (1987), An inquiry into <strong>art</strong> and madness: <strong>The</strong> career <strong>of</strong> Jochen Seidel, Annual<br />

<strong>of</strong> Psychoanalysis , V.15:269-291.<br />

18 Sass, L.A. (1985), Time, space, and symbol: A study <strong>of</strong> narrative form and representational structure<br />

in madness and modernism, Psychoanalysis and Contemporary Thought , V.9(1):45-85.<br />

22 Schildkraut, J.J. (1994), Hirshfeld, A.J., Murphy, J.M., Mind and mood in modern <strong>art</strong>: II. Depressive<br />

disorders, spirituality, and early deaths in the abstract expressionist <strong>art</strong>ists <strong>of</strong> the New York School, Am.<br />

J. <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry , Apr., V.151(4):482-488.<br />

12 Schildkraut, J.J., Hirshfeld, A.J. (1995), Mind and mood in modern <strong>art</strong> I: Miro and "melancholie",<br />

Creativity Re search J. , V.8(2):139-156.<br />

23 Simon, N. (1977), Primal scene, primary objects and nature morte: A psychoanalytic study <strong>of</strong> Mark<br />

Gertler, International Review <strong>of</strong> Psycho-Analysis , V.4(1):61-70.<br />

15 Starobinski, J. (1993), A modern melancholia: Van Gogh's portrait <strong>of</strong> Dr. Gachet (Trans. M.<br />

Bradshaw), Psychological Medicine , Aug., V.23(3):565-568.<br />

2 Syristova, Eve (1989) <strong>The</strong> creative potential <strong>of</strong> schizophrenic psychosis: its importance for<br />

psychotherapy, Studia Psychologica , V.31(4):283-294.<br />

13 Taylor, B. (1981), Picasso and the pathology <strong>of</strong> cubism, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.8(3-4):165-173.<br />

7 Wapner, W., Judd, T., Gardner, H. (1978), Visual agnosia in an <strong>art</strong>ist, Cortex , Sep., V.14(3):343-364.<br />

20 Warick, L.H., Warick, E.R. (1984), Transitional process and creativity in the life and <strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> Edvard<br />

Munch, J. <strong>of</strong> the Am. Academy <strong>of</strong> Psychoanalysis , Jul., V.12(3):413-424.<br />

17 Wijsenbeek, H. (1978), <strong>The</strong> <strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> Pieter Mondrian, Confinia Psychiatrica , V.21(1-3):156-160.<br />

8 Wylie, M.L., Wylie, H.W. (1989), <strong>The</strong> creative relationship <strong>of</strong> internal and external determinants in the<br />

life <strong>of</strong> an <strong>art</strong>ist, Annual <strong>of</strong> Psychoanalysis , V.17:73-128.<br />

Case studies - change <strong>of</strong> sign/theme - subjective measures<br />

14 Bauer, A., Kaiser, G.(1995), Drawing on drawings, Aphasiology , Jan-Feb., V.9(1):68-78.<br />

3 Benveniste, D.(1983), <strong>The</strong> archetypal image <strong>of</strong> the mouth and its relation to autism, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , Sum., V.10(2):99-112.<br />

15 Buchalter-Katz, S. (1985), Observations concerning the <strong>art</strong> productions <strong>of</strong> depressed patients in a shortterm<br />

psychiatric facility, Arts in Psychotherapy , Spr., V.12(1):35-38.<br />

16 Cohen, Barry M., Cox, Carol T. (1989), Breaking the code: identification <strong>of</strong> multiplicity through <strong>art</strong><br />

productions, Dissociation Progress in the Dissociative Disorders , Sep. V.2(3):132-137.<br />

8 Dodd, F.G. (1975), Art therapy with a brain injured man, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Apr., V.14(3):83-89.<br />

12 Enachescu, C. (1971), Aspects <strong>of</strong> pictorial creation in manic-depressive psychosis, Confinia<br />

Psychiatrica , V.14(2):133-142.<br />

7 Forrest, G. (1978), An <strong>art</strong> therapist's contribution to the diagnostic process, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Apr.,<br />

V.17(3):99-105.<br />

1 Galbraith, N. (1978), A foster child's pictorial expression <strong>of</strong> ambivalence, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Jan.,<br />

V.17(2):39-49.<br />

13 Garcia, V.L. (1975), Case study: Family <strong>art</strong> evaluation in a Brazilian guidance clinic, Am. J. Art<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy , Jul., V.14(4):132-9.<br />

6 Hardi, I. (1972), Reflection <strong>of</strong> manic-depressive psychoses in dynamic drawing tests, Confinia<br />

Psychiatrica , V.15(1):64-70.<br />

10 Levinson, C.P. (1986), Patient drawings and growth toward mature object relations: Observations <strong>of</strong><br />

an <strong>art</strong> therapy group in a psychiatric ward, Arts in Psychotherapy , Sum., V.13(2):101-6.<br />

2 L<strong>of</strong>ren, D.E. (1981), Art therapy and cultural difference, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Oct., V.21(1):25-30.<br />

4 Lowe, M.E. (1984), Smoke gets in your eyes, sometimes, Arts in Psychotherapy , Win., V.11(4):267-<br />

277. mute 1912<br />

11 Mango, C. (1992), Emma: Art therapy illustrating personal and universal images <strong>of</strong> loss, Omega J.<br />

<strong>of</strong> Death and Dying , V.25(4):259-269.<br />

9 M<strong>art</strong>ineau, M. (1986), From symptom to symbol: Group intervention as a catalyst in establishing an<br />

effective holding environment, Pratt Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.7:22-30.<br />

5 Perez, L., Marcus-Ofseyer, B. (1978), <strong>The</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> lithium treatment on the behavior and paintings <strong>of</strong><br />

386


a psychotic patient with religious and sexual conflicts, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Apr., V.17(3):85-90.<br />

Case studies - change <strong>of</strong> sign theme - objective measure<br />

23 Aldridge, D., Brandt, G., Wohler, D. (1990), Toward a common language among the creative <strong>art</strong>s<br />

therapies, Arts in Psychotherapy , Fall, V.17(3):189-195.<br />

20 Cagnoletta, M.D. (1983), Art work as a representation <strong>of</strong> object relations in the therapeutic practice,<br />

Pratt Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.4:46-52.<br />

26 Cronin, S.M., Werblowsky, J.H. (1979), Early signs <strong>of</strong> organicity in <strong>art</strong> work, Art Psychotherapy ,<br />

V.6(2):103-8.<br />

19 Fink, P.J. (1973), Art as a reflection <strong>of</strong> mental status, Art Psychotherapy , Apr., V.1(1):17-30.<br />

27 Gerevich, J., Ungvari, G., Karczag, I. (1979), Further data on the diagnostic value <strong>of</strong> spontaneous<br />

drawing, Confinia Psychiatrica , V.22(1):34-48.<br />

28 Hardi, I. (1977), Alcoholic diseases in the light <strong>of</strong> dynamic drawing tests, Psychiatrica Fennica , p.47-<br />

61.<br />

25 Heiman, M., Strnad, D., Weiland, W. (1994), Art therapy and alexithymia, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.11(2):143-<br />

6.<br />

24 Heine, D., Steiner, M. (1986), Standardized paintings as a proposed adjunct instrument for longitudinal<br />

monitoring <strong>of</strong> mood states: a preliminary note, Occupational <strong>The</strong>rapy in Mental Health , Fall, V.6(3):31-7.<br />

22 Hendrixson, B.N. (1986), A self-portrait project for a client with short-term memory dysfunction, Am.<br />

J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Aug., V.25(1):15-24.<br />

30 Holmes, C.B. (1983), Memory For Designs drawing styles <strong>of</strong> psychiatric patients, J. Clin.<br />

Psychology , Jul., V.39(4):563-566.<br />

29 Kay, S.R. (1979), Significance <strong>of</strong> torque in retarded mental development and psychosis: relationship<br />

to antecedent and current pathology, Am. Psychologist , Apr., V.34(4):357-362.<br />

31 Nikolaenko, N.N., Menshutkin, V.V. (1993), Co-ordinate displacement and visual space compression<br />

during right hemisphere inhibition, Human Physiology , Mar-Apr., V.19(2):104-8.<br />

17 Roback, H.B., Gunby, L. (1984), A ten year comparison <strong>of</strong> human figure drawings by a<br />

psychologically decompensating patient, International J. Symbolo gy , Nov., V.8(3):103-111.<br />

18 Wald, J. (1984), <strong>The</strong> graphic representation <strong>of</strong> regression in an Alzheimer's disease patient, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , Fall, V.11(3):165-175.<br />

21 Wolf, J.M., Willmuth, M.E., Watkins, A. (1986), Art <strong>The</strong>rapy's role in treatment <strong>of</strong> anorexia nervosa,<br />

Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Nov., V.25(2):39-46.<br />

Case studies, behaviour change<br />

Bowen, C.A., Rosal, M.L. (1989), <strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy to reduce the maladaptive behaviors <strong>of</strong> a<br />

mentally retarded adult, Arts in Psychotherapy , Fall, V.16(3):211-218.<br />

Gale, I.G. (1990), Neuropsychological rehabilitation technique with a chronic schizophrenic patient,<br />

Behaviour-Change , V.7(4):179-184.<br />

Goren, A.R., Fine, J. (1995), Manaim, H., Apter, A., Verbal and nonverbal expressions <strong>of</strong> central deficits<br />

in schizophrenia., J. Nervous and Mental Disease , Nov. V.183(11):715-719.<br />

Olive, J.S. (1991), Development <strong>of</strong> group interpersonal skills through <strong>art</strong> therapy, Maladjustment and<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapeutic Education , Win., V.9(3):174-180.<br />

Case studies - main therapeutic benefit - relationship<br />

7 Bondesen, C.L. (1984), Transference and countertransference in schizophrenia: <strong>The</strong> paranoic defense<br />

against homosexuality, Pratt Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.5:22-32.<br />

8 Buck, L.A., Goldstein, F., Kardeman, E. (1984), Art as a means <strong>of</strong> interpersonal communication in<br />

autistic young adults, J. Psychology and Christianity , Fall, V.3(3):73-84.<br />

13 Davis, D.L., Boster, L. (1988), Multifaceted therapeutic interventions with the violent psychiatric<br />

inpatient, Hospital and Community Psychiatry , Aug., V.39(8):867-869.<br />

18 Dreifuss, E. (1978), Some notes on a relationship between <strong>art</strong> therapist and patient, Am. J. Art<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy , Jan., V.17(2):57.<br />

17 Dunne, M. (1993), <strong>The</strong> integration <strong>of</strong> two theoretical models in treatment with <strong>art</strong> therapy, Pratt<br />

Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review, V.14:17-24.<br />

387


10 Fagin, I. (1983), Images <strong>of</strong> growth and mourning in the process <strong>of</strong> termination, Pratt Institute Creative<br />

Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.4:53-60.<br />

6 Ferrara, N. (1992), Adolescent narcissism and ego regression: an <strong>art</strong> therapy case illustration, J. Child<br />

and Youth Care , V.7(1):49-56.<br />

12 Forrest, G. (1975), <strong>The</strong> problems <strong>of</strong> dependency and the value <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy as a means <strong>of</strong> treating<br />

alcoholism, Art Psychotherapy , V.2(1):15-43.<br />

9 Gunther, M. (1992), Eros and the ego: the use <strong>of</strong> ego assessment in creatively addressing a sexualized<br />

transference, Pratt Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.13:31-40.<br />

4 Horovitz-Darby, E.G. (1992), Reflections: Countertransference: Implications in treatment and post<br />

treatment, Arts in P sychotherapy , V.19(5):379-389.<br />

3 Izhak<strong>of</strong>f, S. (1993), Symbiosis and symbiotic relatedness: A bridge to schizophrenia, Pratt Institute<br />

Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.14:25-33.<br />

5 Kaplan, F.F. (1983), Drawing together: <strong>The</strong>rapeutic use <strong>of</strong> the wish to merge, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Apr.,<br />

V.22(3):79-85.<br />

2 Shapiro, J. (1988), Moments with a multiple personality disorder patient, Pratt Institute Creative Arts<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.9:61-72.<br />

14 Stamelos, T., Mott, D.W. (1983), Learned helplessness in persons with mental retardation: <strong>art</strong> as a<br />

client centred treatment modality, Arts in Psychotherapy , Win., V.10(4):241-249.<br />

19 Stamelos, T., Mott, D.W. (1986), Creative potential among persons labelled developmentally<br />

delayed:II. Meditation as a technique to release creativity, Arts in Psychotherapy , Fall, V.13(3):229-234.<br />

11 Teirstein, E.G. (1991), Developing: Art, mastery, self, Pratt Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review ,<br />

V.12:16-29.<br />

1 Wolf, R. (1975), Art psychotherapy with acting-out adolescents: an innovative approach for special<br />

education, Art Psychotherapy , Vol.2(3-4):255-266.<br />

Case studies - main therapeutic benefit - expression <strong>of</strong> feelings<br />

Alanko, A. (1973), Psychosis and <strong>art</strong>, Psychiatria Fennica , p.153-158.<br />

Bemtovegna, S., Schw<strong>art</strong>z, L., Deschner, D. (1983), Case study: the use <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> with an autistic child in<br />

residential care, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Jan., V.22(2):51-6.<br />

Berkowitz, S. (1990), Art therapy with a Vietnam veteran who has post traumatic stress disorder, Pratt<br />

Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.11:47-62.<br />

Bertoia, J., Allan, J. (1988), Counselling seriously ill children: use <strong>of</strong> spontaneous drawings, Elementary<br />

School Guidance and Counselling , Feb., V.22(3):206-221.<br />

Blasco, S.P. (1978), Case study: <strong>art</strong> expression as a guide to music therapy, Am. J. Art Th erapy , Jan.,<br />

V.17(2):51-56.<br />

Branch, J. (1992), Depression and feminine personality development, Pratt Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy<br />

Review , V.13:9-15.<br />

Cardone, L., Marengo, J., Calisch, A. (1982), Conjoint use <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> and verbal techniques for the<br />

intensification <strong>of</strong> the psychotherapeutic group experience, Arts in Psychotherapy , Win., V.9(4):263-268.<br />

Carozza, P.M., Heirsteiner, C.L.(1982), Young female incest victims in treatment: stages <strong>of</strong> growth seen<br />

with a group <strong>art</strong> therapy model, Clinical Social Work J. , Fall, V.10(3):165-175.<br />

Ciornai, S. (1983), Art therapy with working class Latino women, Arts in Psychotherapy , Sum.,<br />

V.10(2):63-76.<br />

Cohen, F.W. (1974), Art therapy in the diagnosis and treatment <strong>of</strong> a transsexual, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy ,<br />

Oct., V.14(1):3-11.<br />

Cohn, R. (1984), Resolving issues <strong>of</strong> separation through <strong>art</strong>, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.11(1):29-35.<br />

Colli, L.M. (1994), Aims in therapy and directives in society: observations on individuation and<br />

adaptation (Trans. J. Leyland), Arts in Psychotherapy , V.21(2):107-112.<br />

David, I.R., Sageman, S. (1987), Psychological aspects <strong>of</strong> AIDS as seen in <strong>art</strong> therapy, Am. J. Art<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy , Aug., V.26(1):3-10.<br />

Drachnik, C. (1978), Case study: <strong>art</strong> therapy with a girl who lived in two worlds, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy ,<br />

Oct., V.18(1):19-27.<br />

Eskridge, J.H. (1993), Healing the wounded female self, Pratt Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review ,<br />

V.14:50-55.<br />

388


Evans, E. (1986), Facilitating the reparative process in depression: images for healing, Pratt Institute<br />

Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.7:43-53.<br />

Evans, E. (1984), Transpersonal aspects <strong>of</strong> transference and countertransference, a Jungian perspective, Pratt<br />

Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.5:1-11.<br />

Fleming, M., Nathans, J. (1979), <strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> in understanding the central treatment issues in a female<br />

to male transsexual, Art Psychotherapy , B.6(1):25-35.<br />

Fogle, D.M. (1980), Art and poetry therapy combined with talking therapy with a family <strong>of</strong> four in an<br />

outpatient clinic, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.7(1):27-34.<br />

Gillespie, A. (1986), "Art <strong>The</strong>rapy" at the Familymakers project, Adoption and Fostering , V.10(1):19-23.<br />

Glaister, J.A. (1994), Clara's story: post traumatic response and therapeutic <strong>art</strong>, Perspectives in Psychiatric<br />

Care , Jan-Mar., V.230(1):17-22.<br />

Green, P., Bertrand, D. (1975), Art therapy with a schizophrenic patient, Bulletin <strong>of</strong> the Menninger Clinic ,<br />

Jan., V.39(1):83-92.<br />

Greenspoon, D.B. (1984), <strong>The</strong> role <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong> therapist as an adjunctive member <strong>of</strong> a residential treatment<br />

team, Residential Group Care and Treatment , Spr., V.2(3):15-31.<br />

Greenspoon, D.B. (1982), Case study: the development <strong>of</strong> self expression a severely disturbed adolescent,<br />

Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Oct., V.22(1):17-22.<br />

Halbreich, U., Assael, M., Dreifus, D. (1980), Premonition <strong>of</strong> death in painting, Confinia Psychiatrica ,<br />

V.23(2):74-81.<br />

Harriss, M., Landg<strong>art</strong>en, H. (1973), Art therapy as an innovative approach to conjoint treatment: a case<br />

study, Art Psychotherapy , Win., V.1(3-4):221-228.<br />

Hatfield, F.M., Zangwill, O.L. (1974), Ideation in aphasia: the picture-story method, Neuropsychologia ,<br />

Jul., V.12(3):389-393.<br />

Heiney, S.P. (1991), Sibling grief: a case report, Archives <strong>of</strong> Psychiatric Nursing , Jun., V.5(3):121-127.<br />

Hook-Wheelhouwer, J. (1991), Protective custody: a lifestyle in prison, Pratt Institute Creative Arts<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.12:36-40.<br />

Jones, D.L., Rush, K. (1979), Treatment <strong>of</strong> psychotic patients with preoccupations <strong>of</strong> demon possession,<br />

Art Psychotherapy , V.6(1):1-9.<br />

Katz, S.L. (1987), Photocollage as a therapeutic modality for working with groups, Social Work with<br />

Groups , Win., V.10(4):83-90.<br />

Kornreich, S. (1988), An Alzheimer patient's use <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> as a form <strong>of</strong> constancy, Pratt Institute Creative Arts<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.9:29-39.<br />

Kunkle-Miller, C. (1978), Art therapy with mentally retarded adults, Art Psychotherapy , V.5(3):123-133.<br />

Landg<strong>art</strong>en, H. (1975), Art therapy as primary mode <strong>of</strong> treatment for an elective mute, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy ,<br />

Jul., V.14(4):121-5.<br />

Landg<strong>art</strong>en, H. (1975), Group <strong>art</strong> therapy for mothers and daughters, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Jan.,<br />

V.14(2):31-35.<br />

Levick, M., Herring, J. (1973), Family dynamics - as seen through <strong>art</strong> therapy, Art Psychotherapy , Apr.,<br />

V.1(1):45-54.<br />

Lehtonen, K., Shaughnessy, M.F. (1992), Projective drawings as an aid to music therapy, Acta<br />

Paedopsychiatrica International J. <strong>of</strong> Child and Ad olescent Psychiatry , Dec., V.55(4):231-233.<br />

Lub<strong>art</strong>, L. (1985), <strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> structured <strong>art</strong> and movement with chronic schizophrenic adults, Pratt<br />

Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.6:37-43.<br />

Marinow, A. (1980), Symbolic self-expression in drug addiction, Confinia Psychiatrica , V.23(2):103-8.<br />

Mashiah, T. (1996), Painting as language for a stroke patient, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.13(4):265-9.<br />

McIntyre, B.P. (1990), Art therapy with bereaved youth, J. Palliative Care , Spr., V.6(1):16-25.<br />

McNiff, S. (1975), Anthony: a study in parallel <strong>art</strong>istic and personal development, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy ,<br />

Jul., V.14(4):126-131.<br />

Mills, A. (1985), Art therapy on a residential treatment team for troubled children, J. Child Care ,<br />

V.2(4):61-71 - reprint J. Child and Youth Care , 1991 V.6(4):49-59.<br />

M<strong>of</strong>fat, J.P., Friedman, I. (1973), Utilization <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy in the hospital management <strong>of</strong> a schizophrenic<br />

patient, Art Psychotherapy , Win., V.1(3-4):301-6.<br />

Musick, P.L. (1980), Creativity: abreaction for the therapist, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.7(3):197-199.<br />

Myers-Garrett, E.A. (1987), <strong>The</strong> role <strong>of</strong> contours in symbol building with a victim <strong>of</strong> sexual abuse, Pratt<br />

389


Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.8:45-51.<br />

Naitove, C.E. (1986), "Life's but a walking shadow": treating anorexia nervosa and bulimia, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , Sum., V.13(2):107-119.<br />

Nelson, A. (1986), <strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> early recollection drawings in children's group therapy, Individual<br />

Psychology Journal <strong>of</strong> Adlerian <strong>The</strong>ory, Research and Practice , Jun., V.42(2):288-291.<br />

Pitak, S. (1982), Bridging isolation: a synthesis <strong>of</strong> Eastern and Western perspectives in creative group<br />

work with addiction recovery unit patients, Pratt Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.3:67-76.<br />

Potocky, M. (1993), An <strong>art</strong> therapy group for clients with chronic schizophrenia, Social Work with<br />

Groups , V.16(3):73-82.<br />

Quail, J.M., Peavy, R.V. (1994), A phenomenologic study <strong>of</strong> a client's experience in <strong>art</strong> therapy, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , V.21(1):45-57.<br />

Robbins, A. (1984), <strong>The</strong> struggle for self-cohesion: an analytically orientated <strong>art</strong> therapy case study, Art<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy , Oct., V.1(3):107-118.<br />

Shennum, W.A. (1987), Expressive activity therapy in residential treatment: effects on children's behavior<br />

in the treatment milieu, Child and Youth Care Qu<strong>art</strong>erly , Sum., V.16(2):81-90.<br />

Sikelianos, M. (1975), <strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> symbolic drawing, metaphor and illusion in a therapeutic-creative<br />

process, Israel Annals <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry and Related Disciplines , Jun., V.13(2):142-161.<br />

Sinai, J. (1997) <strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> metaphor by an <strong>art</strong>less first time psychotherapist, Am. J. Psychotherapy ,<br />

Spring, V.51(2):273-88.<br />

Small, J., Greenway, M. (1988), <strong>The</strong> rediscovery <strong>of</strong> the lost twin: an account <strong>of</strong> therapy for a child and<br />

his parents, British J. <strong>of</strong> Psychotherapy , Fall, V.5(1):19-28.<br />

Stember, C.A. (1978), Change in maladaptive growth <strong>of</strong> abused girl through <strong>art</strong> therapy, Art<br />

Psychotherapy , V.5(2):99-109.<br />

Uhlin, D.M. (1977), <strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> drawings for psychiatric evaluation <strong>of</strong> a defendant in a case <strong>of</strong> homicide,<br />

Mental Health and Society , V.4(1-2):61-73.<br />

Tyler, J. (1998), Nonverbal communication and the use <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> in the care <strong>of</strong> the dying, Palliative Medicine ,<br />

March, V.12(2):123-6. Ulak, B.J., Cummings, A.L. (1997) Using client's <strong>art</strong>istic experiences as metaphor<br />

in counselling: a pilot study, Can. J. Counselling , Oct., V.31(4):305-16.<br />

Vishup, E. (1985), Group <strong>art</strong> therapy in a methadone clinic lobby, J. <strong>of</strong> Substance Abuse Treatment ,<br />

V.2(3):153-158.<br />

Vogli-Phelps, V. (1985), Letting the monsters out, Pointer , Spr., V.29(3):35-9.<br />

Weston, D. (1988), Development <strong>of</strong> a holding space for children hospitalized in a psychiatric unit, Pratt<br />

Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.9:54-60.<br />

Wickersham, K. (1982), Imagery as a bridge to reorganizing secondary process skills in a ten year old<br />

aphasic child, Pratt Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review , V.3:56-7.<br />

Williams, S., Tamura, T.A., Rosen, D.H. (1977), An outpatient <strong>art</strong> therapy group, Art Psychotherapy ,<br />

V.4(3-4):199-214.<br />

Williams, S. (1976), Short term <strong>art</strong> therapy, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Jan., V.15(2):35-41.<br />

Zambelli, G.C., Clark, E.J., de-Jong-Hodgson, A. (1994), <strong>The</strong> constructive use <strong>of</strong> ghost imagery in<br />

childhood grief, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.21(1):17-24.<br />

67<br />

Controlled studies<br />

[C controlled studies; V Validity study; R Reliability <strong>Study</strong>; M Meta analysis]<br />

1. Aikman, K.G., Belter, R.W., Finch, A.J. (1992), Human figure drawings: validity in assessing<br />

intellectual level and academic achievement, J. <strong>of</strong> Clinical Psychology , Jan., V.48(1):114-120. [V]<br />

2. Anstadt, T., Krause, R. (1989), <strong>The</strong> expression <strong>of</strong> primary affects in portraits drawn by schizophrenics,<br />

Psychiatry , Feb., V.52(1):13-24. [C, V]<br />

3. Bergland, C., Moore Gonzalez, R. (1993), Art & madness: can the interface be quantified? (SPAR),<br />

Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Feb., V.31:81-90. [C, V, R]<br />

4. Brems, C., Adams, R.L., Skillman, G.D. (1993), Person drawings by transsexual clients, psychiatric<br />

clients and nonclients compared: indications <strong>of</strong> sex-typography, Archives <strong>of</strong> Sexual Behavior , Jun.,<br />

V.22(3):253-264. [C, V]<br />

5. Brooke, S.L. (1995) Art <strong>The</strong>rapy: An approach to working with sexual abuse survivors, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , V.22(5):447-466. [C, V]<br />

6. Castilla, L.M., Klyczek, J.P. (1993), Comparison <strong>of</strong> the Kinetic Person Drawing Task <strong>of</strong> the Bay Area<br />

390


Functional Performance Evaluation with measures <strong>of</strong> functional performance, Occupational <strong>The</strong>rapy in<br />

Mental Healt h , V.12(2):27-38. [V]<br />

7. Cermak, S.A., Eimon, M. & P., H<strong>art</strong>well, A. (1991), Constructional abilities in persons with chronic<br />

schizophrenia, Occupational <strong>The</strong>rapy in Mental Health V.11(4):21-39. [C, V]<br />

8. Cohen, B., Hammer, J.S., Singer, S. (1988), <strong>The</strong> Diagnostic Drawing Series: A systematic approach<br />

to <strong>art</strong> therapy evaluation and research, Arts in Psychotherapy , Spr., V.15(1):11-21 [C, V]<br />

9. Cohen, F., Phelps, R.E. (1985) Incest markers in children's <strong>art</strong>work, Arts in Psychotherapy Win.<br />

V.12(4):265-283. [V, R]<br />

10. Cohen, F.W., Phelps, R.E. (1985), Incest markers in children's <strong>art</strong>work, Arts in Psychotherapy,<br />

Win.,<br />

V.12(4):265-283. [R]<br />

11. Couch, J.B. (1994), Diagnostic Drawing Series: research with older people diagnosed with organic<br />

mental syndromes & disorders, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.11(2):111-115. [C, V]<br />

12. Cressen, R. (1975), Artistic quality <strong>of</strong> drawings and judges' evaluations <strong>of</strong> the Draw-A-Person, J.<br />

Personality Assessment , Apr., V.39(2)132-137. [R]<br />

13. De Fazio, A. (1985), An abberant drawing sequence <strong>of</strong> the human figure and its relation to<br />

<strong>psychopathology</strong>, Perceptual Motor Skills , Dec., V.61(3pt1):785-802. [C, V]<br />

14. Dent, J.K., Kwiatkowska, H.Y. (1970), Aesthetic preferences <strong>of</strong> young adults for pictures drawn by<br />

mental patients and by members <strong>of</strong> their immediate families, Sciences de l'Art , V.7(1-2):43-54. [R]<br />

15. Dodrill, C.B. (1985), Incidence and doubtful significance <strong>of</strong> nonstandard orientations in reproduction<br />

<strong>of</strong> key, Perceptual Motor Skills , Apr., V.60(2):411-415. [C, V]<br />

16. Dudley, H.K. (1973), <strong>The</strong> Draw-A-Person Test and young state hospital patients, J. <strong>of</strong> Youth &<br />

Adolescence , Dec., V.2(4): 313-330. [V]<br />

17. Dykens, E. (1996), <strong>The</strong> Draw-A-Person task in persons with mental retardation: what does it<br />

measure?, Research in Developmental Disabilities , Jan-Feb., V. 17(1):1-13. [V]<br />

18. Ericson, R., Hill, K., Eras, P., Holmen, K., Jorm, A. (1994) <strong>The</strong> short human figure drawing scale<br />

for the evaluation <strong>of</strong> suspect cognitive dysfunction in old age, Archives <strong>of</strong> Gerontology & Geriatrics , Nov-<br />

Dec., V.19(3):243-251. [C, V]<br />

19. Feher, E., Vandecreek, L., Teglasi, H. (1983), <strong>The</strong> problem <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> quality in the use <strong>of</strong> human figure<br />

drawing tests, J. Clin. Psychology Mar., V.39(2):268-275. [R]<br />

20. Forstl, H., Burns, A., Levy, R., Cairns, (1993) N. Neuropathological basis for drawing disability<br />

(constructional apraxia) in Alzheimer's disease, Psychological Medicine , V.23:623-629. [V, R]<br />

21. Gantt, L. (1990), A validity study <strong>of</strong> the Formal Elements Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Scale (FEATS) for diagnostic<br />

information in patients' drawing, unpublished Doctoral dissertation, University Pittsburgh, USA. [C, V,<br />

R, M]<br />

22. Gounard, B.R., Pray, R.C. (1975), Human figure drawings <strong>of</strong> learning disabled and normal children<br />

at three age levels, Perceptual and Motor Skills , V.40:914. [V]<br />

23. Green, B.L., Wehling, C., Talsky, G.J. (1987) Group <strong>art</strong> therapy as an adjunct to treatment for chronic<br />

outpatients, Health and Community Psychiatry , Sep., V.38(9):988-991. [C, V]<br />

24. Grodner, S., Braff, D.L., Janowsky, D.S., & Clopton, P.L. (1982), Efficacy <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>/movement therapy<br />

in evaluating mood, Arts in Psychotherapy Fall V.9(3):217-225. [C, V]<br />

25. Grossman, M. (1993), Semantic evaluation <strong>of</strong> perceptual errors in aphasics' freehand category drawing,<br />

Neuropsychology , Jan.V.7(1):27-40. [C, V]<br />

26. Gustafson, J.L., Waehler, C.A. (1992) Assessing concrete and abstract thinking with the Draw-A-<br />

Person technique, J. Personality Assessment , V.59(3):439-447. [C, V]<br />

27. Heiman, M., Strnad, D., Weiland, W., Wise, T.N. (1994), Art therapy and alexythymia, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy ,<br />

V.11(2):143-146. [V]<br />

28. Heine, D., Steiner, M. (1986), Standardized paintings as a proposed adjunct instrument for<br />

longitudinal monitoring <strong>of</strong> mood states: a preliminary note, Occupational <strong>The</strong>rapy in Mental Health , Fall,<br />

V.6(3):31-37. [V]<br />

29. Holmes, C.B., Wiederholt, J. (1982), Depression and figure size on the draw-a-person test, Perceptual<br />

and Motor Skills , Dec., V.55(3 pt.1):825-826. [C, V]<br />

30. John, K.B. (1974), Variations in bilateral symmetry <strong>of</strong> human figure drawings associated with 2 levels<br />

<strong>of</strong> adjustment, J. Clin Psychology Jul., V.30(3):401-404. [C, V]<br />

31. Kaplan, F.F. (1991), Drawing assessment and <strong>art</strong>istic skill, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.18:347-352. [V,<br />

R]<br />

32. Kay, S.R. (1980), Progressive figure drawings in the developmental assessment <strong>of</strong> mentally retarded<br />

391


psychotics, Perceptual and Motor Skills , Apr., V.50(2):583-590. [V]<br />

33. Kay, S.R. (1978), Qualitative differences in human figure drawings according to schizophrenic<br />

subtype, Perceptual and Motor Skills , 47:923-932. [C, V, R, M]<br />

34. Kessler, K. (1994), A study <strong>of</strong> the Diagnostic Drawing Series with eating disordered patients, Art<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy , V.11(2):116-118. [C, V]<br />

35. Kirk, A., Kertesz, A. (1989), Hemispheric contributions to drawing, Neuropsychologia ,<br />

V.27(6):881-886. [C, V, R, M]<br />

36. Knapp, N.M. (1994), Research with diagnostic drawings for normal and Alzheimer's subjects, Art<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy , V.11(2):131-138. [C, V, R, M]<br />

37. Langevin, R., Raine, M., Day, D., Waxer, K. (1975), Art experience, intelligence and formal features<br />

in psychotics' paintings, Arts in Psychothera py (study 1), V.2(2):149-158. [C, V, R]<br />

38. Langevin, R., Raine, M., Day, D., Waxer, K. (1975), Art experience, intelligence and formal features<br />

in psychotics' paintings, Arts in Psychotherapy (study 2), V.2(2):149-158. [C, V, R, M]<br />

39. Langevin, R., Hutchins, L.M. (1973), An experimental investigation <strong>of</strong> judges' ratings <strong>of</strong><br />

schizophrenics' and non-schizophrenics' paintings, J. Personality Assessment , Dec., V.37(6):537-543. [V,<br />

R]<br />

40. Larrabee, G.J., Kane, R.L. (1983), Differential drawing size associated with unilateral brain damage,<br />

Neuropsychologia , V.21(2):173-177. [C, V]<br />

41. Lehman, E.B., Levy, B.I. (1971), Discrepancies in estimates <strong>of</strong> children's intelligence: WISC and<br />

human figures drawing, J. Clin Psychology , V.27:74-76. [V]<br />

42. Lerner, C., Ross, G. <strong>The</strong> magazine picture collage: development <strong>of</strong> an objective scoring system, Am.<br />

J. <strong>of</strong> Occupational <strong>The</strong>rapy , Mar., V.31(3):156-161. [C, V]<br />

43. Levy, B.I., Ulman, E. (1974), <strong>The</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> training on judging <strong>psychopathology</strong> from paintings, Am.<br />

J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Oct., V.14:24-25. [C, V]<br />

44. McGlashan, T.H., Wadeson, H.S., Carpenter, W.T., Levy, S.T. (1977), Art and recovery style from<br />

psychosis, J. Nervous and Mental Disease , V.164(3):182-190. [C, V, R, M]<br />

45. McNiff, S., Oelman, R. (1975), Images <strong>of</strong> fear, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.2(3-4):267-277. [C, V]<br />

46. Miljkovitch, M., Irvine, G.M. Comparison <strong>of</strong> drawing performances <strong>of</strong> schizophrenics, other<br />

psychiatric patients and normal schoolchildren on a draw-a-village task, Arts in Psychotherapy ,<br />

V.9:203-216. [C, V, R, M]<br />

47. Miller, A.L., Atlas, J.A., Arsenio, W.F. (1993), Self-other differentiation among psychotic and<br />

conduct-disordered adolescents as measured by human figure drawings, Percep Motor Skills , Apr.,<br />

V.76(2):397-8. [C, V]<br />

48. Mills, A., Cohen, B.M., Meneses, J.Z. (1993), Reliability and validity tests <strong>of</strong> the Diagnostic<br />

Drawing Series, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.20:83-88. [R]<br />

49. DDS study 77 naive raters, unpublished reported in Mills, A., Cohen, B.M., Meneses, J.Z. (1993),<br />

Reliability and validity tests <strong>of</strong> the Diagnostic Drawing Series, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.20:83-88. [R]<br />

50. Phillips, E.L., Geller, S.K. (1983), Ireland, M., Research on the use <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy in a university<br />

setting, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Oct., V.23(1):26-29. [R]<br />

51. Phillips, W.M., Phillips, A.M. (1976), Similarity between complexity on Role Construct Repertory<br />

Tech and <strong>art</strong>iculation <strong>of</strong> Draw-A-Person test for patients and nonpatients, Perceptual Motor Skills , Dec.<br />

V.43(3):1256-1258. [C, V]<br />

52. Rankin, A. (1994), Tree drawings and trauma indicators: a comparison <strong>of</strong> past research with current<br />

findings from the DDS, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , V.11(2):127-130. [C, V, R]<br />

53. Robins, C., Edward, B., Sidney, J., Ford, R.Q. (1991), Changes in human figure drawings during<br />

intensive treatment, J. Personality Assessment , Dec. V.57(3):477-97. [C, V]<br />

54. Rosal, M.L. (1993), Changes in locus <strong>of</strong> control in behaviour disordered children, Arts in<br />

Psychotherapy , V.20(3:231-241. [C, V]<br />

55. Rubin, J.A., Ragins, N., Shachter, J., Wimberly, F. (1979), Drawings by schizophrenic and<br />

non-schizophrenic mothers and their children, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.6(3):163-175. [C, V]<br />

56. Russell-Lacy, S., Robinson, B., Benson, J., Cranage, J. (1979), An experimental study <strong>of</strong> pictures<br />

produced by acute schizophrenic subjects, B. J. Psychiatry , V.134:195-200. [C, V, R, M]<br />

57. Sidun, N.M., Rosenthal, R.H. (1987), Graphic indicators <strong>of</strong> sexual abuse in Draw-A-Person tests <strong>of</strong><br />

psychiatrically hospitalized adolescents, Arts in Psychotherapy , Spr., V.14(1):25-33. [C, V, R, M]<br />

8. Silver, R., Ellison, J. (1995), Identifying and assessing self-images in drawings by delinquent<br />

adolescents, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.22(4):339-352. [R]<br />

392


59. p<strong>art</strong> 2. Silver, R., Ellison, J. (1995), Identifying and assessing self-images in drawings by delinquent<br />

adolescents, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.22(4):339-352. [R]<br />

60. Simmonds, D.W., Koocher, G.P. (1973), Perceptual rigidity in paranoid schizophrenics; use <strong>of</strong><br />

projective animal drawings, Perceptual Motor Skills , Aug., V.37(1):247-250. [C, V]<br />

61. Sims, J., Bolton, B., Dana, R.H. (1983), Dimensionality & concurrent validity <strong>of</strong> the Handler DAP<br />

anxiety index, Multivariate Experimental Clinical Research , V.6(2):69-79. [V, R]<br />

62. Sullivan, E.V., Mathalon, D.H., Nim Ha, C., Zipursky, R.B., Pfefferbaum, A. (1992), <strong>The</strong><br />

contribution <strong>of</strong> constructional accuracy and organizational strategy to nonverbal recall in schizophrenia and<br />

chronic alcoholism, Biological Psychiatry V.32(4):312-333. [C, V]<br />

63. Tharinger, D.J.; Stark, K.D. (1990), A qualitative versus <strong>quantitative</strong> approach to evaluating the Draw-<br />

A-Person and Kinetic Family Drawing:a study <strong>of</strong> mood and anxiety disorder children, Psychological<br />

Assessment , V.2(4):365-375. [V]<br />

64. Van Ho<strong>of</strong>, J.J., Hulstijn, W., Van Mier, H., Pagen, M. (1993),<br />

Figure drawing and psychomotor retardation: preliminary report, J. <strong>of</strong> Affective Disorders , Dec.,<br />

V.29(4):263-6. [C, V]<br />

65. Verinis, J.S., Lichtenberg, E.F., Henrich, L. (1974), <strong>The</strong> Draw A Person in the rain technique: Its<br />

relationship to diagnostic categories and other personality indicators, (experiment 1), J. Clin. Psychology ,<br />

Jul. V.30(3):407-414. [C, V]<br />

66. Verinis, J.S., Lichtenberg, E.F., Henrich, L. (1974), <strong>The</strong> Draw A Person in the rain technique: Its<br />

relationship to Drawing categories and other personality indicators (experiment 2), J. Clin Psychology Jul.<br />

V.30(3): 407-414 [C, V]<br />

67. Verinis, J.S Lichtenberg, E.F., Henrich, L.(1974), <strong>The</strong> Draw A Person in the rain technique: Its<br />

relationship to Drawing categories and other personality indicators (experiment 3), J. Clin Psychology Jul.<br />

V.30(3): 407-414 [C, V]<br />

68. Wadeson, H., Carpenter, W.T. (1976), A comparative study <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> expression <strong>of</strong> schizophrenic unipolar<br />

depressives and bipolar manic-depressive patients, J. Nervous Mental Disease , May, V.162(5):334-344.<br />

[C, V]<br />

69. Wadlington, W.L., McWhinnie, H.J. (1973), <strong>The</strong> development <strong>of</strong> a rating scale for the study <strong>of</strong> formal<br />

aesthetic qualities in the paintings <strong>of</strong> mental patients, Arts in Psychotherapy , Win., V.1(3-4):201-220. [C,<br />

V, R, M]<br />

70. Waldman, T.L., Silber, D.E., Holmstrom, R.W., Karp, S.A. (1994), Personality characteristics <strong>of</strong><br />

incest survivors on the draw-a-person questionnaire, J. Personality Assessment , V.63(1):97-104. [C, V]<br />

71. Walsh, F.W. (1979), Breaching <strong>of</strong> family generation boundaries by schizophrenics, disturbed and<br />

normals, Int. J. <strong>of</strong> Family Th erapy , Fall, V.1(3):254-75. [C, V]<br />

72. Walsh, S.M. (1993), Future images: an <strong>art</strong> intervention with suicidal adolescents, Applied Nursing<br />

Research , Aug., V.6(3):111-8. [C, V]<br />

73. Wittels, B. (1982), Interpretation <strong>of</strong> the 'body <strong>of</strong> water' metaphor in patient <strong>art</strong>work as p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the<br />

Diagnostic process, Arts in Psychotherapy , Fall, V.9(3):177-182. [C, V]<br />

74. Wittlin, B.W., Augusthy, R. (1988), Comparison <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> <strong>psychopathology</strong> and discharge diagnoses <strong>of</strong><br />

diagnostic unit patients, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy , Dec., V.5(1):94-98. [V]<br />

75. Wright, J.H., Macintyre, M.P. (1982), <strong>The</strong> family drawing depression scale, J. Clin. Psychology ,<br />

V.38(4): 853-861. [C, V, R, M]<br />

76. Wright, S.K., Ashman, A.F. (1991), <strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> symbols in drawings by children, nondisabled<br />

adolescents and adolescents with an intellectual disability, Developmental Disabilities Bulletin ,<br />

V.19(2):105-128. [C, V]<br />

77. Yaguchi, K. (1981), A study <strong>of</strong> tree drawings in aged groups: An examination <strong>of</strong> formal indices <strong>of</strong><br />

the drawings, J. Child Development , Jan., V.17:32-34. [C, V]<br />

78. Young, N.A. (1975), Art therapy with chronic schizophrenic patients <strong>of</strong> a low socio-economic class<br />

in a short term treatment facility, Arts in Psychotherapy , V.2(1):101-117. [C, V]<br />

79. Zucker, K.J., Finegan, J.K., Doering, R.W., Bradley, S.J. (1983), Human figure drawings <strong>of</strong> gender<br />

problem children: A comparison to sibling, psychiatric, and normal controls, J. Abnormal Child<br />

Psychology , 11:287-298. [V]<br />

393


Appendix 4<br />

Development <strong>of</strong> the Descriptive Assessement for Psychiatric Artwork<br />

(DAPA)<br />

DAPA version 3: the rating guide used in the main study.<br />

Rating sheet for DAPA version 3.<br />

Helpsheet for DAPA rating guide version 3.<br />

Rating sheet for casenotes (main study).<br />

DAPA version 2: the rating guide used in the pilot study.<br />

Research questionnaire on dominant form for <strong>art</strong>ists.<br />

Research rating sheet on dominant form for re-rating <strong>of</strong> form version 3.<br />

394


Page 1. Rating guide<br />

DAPA Version 3. <strong>The</strong> rating guide used in the main study<br />

DESCRIPTIVE ASSESSMENT FOR PSYCHIATRIC ART<br />

RATING GUIDE<br />

S. HACKING AND D. FOREMAN 1999.<br />

395


D.A.P.A. DESCRIPTIVE ASSESSMENT OF PSYCHIATRIC ARTWORK - S.<br />

HACKING AND D. M. FOREMAN © 1999.<br />

RATING GUIDE<br />

<strong>The</strong> pictures must be rated for the presence <strong>of</strong> 15 elements on 6 scales: colour, intensity,<br />

line, space, emotional tone and form.<br />

A 5 column x 4 row grid drawn on acetate lies over the picture dividing it<br />

into 20 squares whatever the size <strong>of</strong> the paper. Borderlines done by the<br />

painter are ignored. This is laid according to the intended ‘right way up’<br />

<strong>of</strong> the picture. <strong>The</strong> rating is done on the rating sheet which corresponds to<br />

the grid. Each scale in each division <strong>of</strong>fers a choice which is marked <strong>of</strong>f<br />

by the rater. Colour rating forces a present/absent in that square decision<br />

on each <strong>of</strong> 9 colours; the other scales use gradations <strong>of</strong> high/medium/low.<br />

A total <strong>of</strong> 300 decisions are required to rate one picture. However, time<br />

for picture rating typically varies from 5-15m.<br />

Each <strong>of</strong> the 20 scoring squares on the rating sheet contains 5 rows <strong>of</strong> boxes. Rows are<br />

identified left. Colour; Intensity; Line; Space; Emotional Tone. Example below. To score<br />

a box use diagonal line corner to corner.<br />

Each rating square scores the same grid square on the picture. If a picture square is unused,<br />

cross the whole rating square completely through.<br />

COLOUR<br />

R Y O P G B N W K<br />

INTENSITY H N L<br />

PAINTED H<br />

LINE<br />

V N<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

DRAWN LINE H<br />

N<br />

V<br />

396


COLOUR<br />

COLOUR R Y O P G B N<br />

W K<br />

Colour is simple, one you know what the letters mean! Red, Yellow, Orange, Purple,<br />

Green, Blue, browN, White, blacK.<br />

DEFINITION: Colours are defined by their hue. Mark the letter <strong>of</strong> any colour appearing<br />

in the grid square no matter how little. Writing by the painter is included. Grey and<br />

pencil marks are scored ‘blacK’. <strong>The</strong> colour <strong>of</strong> the paper is not scored. See HELP for<br />

more information on colour description and examples.<br />

397


INTENSITY<br />

DEFINITION: Firstly, the brightness or pureness <strong>of</strong> the media used; secondly, the<br />

densit over the area covered by the media (not the unused portion <strong>of</strong> the square). If two<br />

intensities occur in the same square, use whichever covers the most area. If there is equal<br />

cover, score the highest.<br />

H – High strong, dense saturated colour – e.g. thick pure paint or very heavy pressured<br />

pencil or crayon.<br />

N – Neutral. ‘Muddy’ colour; e.g. the colour has been ‘greyed’ or toned down, perhaps<br />

mixed to darker or lighter shades. Medium pressure pencil lines are scored as ‘neutral’.<br />

L – Low intensity or ‘watery’ colour (especially paint), little pigment over a large area,<br />

e.g. light pressured crayon or pencil. See HELP for examples.<br />

If colour is used in a single intensity or pressure, i.e. felt pen, pencils,<br />

crayon, neutral is scored when lines are deliberately overmarked in<br />

different colours. I.e. yellow overscored with black forms a ‘muddy<br />

yellow’ as it would if mixed together.<br />

LINE<br />

INTENSITY H N L<br />

L<br />

I<br />

N<br />

E<br />

PAINT H<br />

V<br />

N<br />

DRAW<br />

H<br />

V<br />

Line is scored for Paint (media which fill the area; paint, pastel) and Draw (media which<br />

use marks such as crayon, pencil, felt tip). Both lines or one line can be used.<br />

DEFINITION: A line must be a distinct drawn or painted mark, loose scribble covering<br />

an area, pattern marks such as dots, outlines and writing ARE lines. Two areas <strong>of</strong> colour<br />

which come together or thin filled shapes or areas <strong>of</strong> colour tightly filled by drawing<br />

media ARE NOT lines. Leave blank if no lines appear in the square.<br />

Guideline: Scan the whole picture to identify the range <strong>of</strong> line. If the line appears<br />

constant, do not make very sensitive discriminations. Differences should be apparent.<br />

Lines such as pencil or felt pen used at the same pressure over the whole picture score<br />

as thin. Thick lines must be distinct. If the line is emphasised, i.e. redrawn 2 or 3 times,<br />

even at the same pressure, mark as thick. Refer to HELP for examples <strong>of</strong> varied lines.<br />

398<br />

N


H – Majority <strong>of</strong> tHick, or heavy lines with high pressure.<br />

V – Varied lines when both thick and thin lines are present in roughly the same quantity.<br />

N – ThiN, or the majority at light pressure.<br />

399


SPACE<br />

>10% >25% >55% >80% >100%<br />

DEFINITION: <strong>The</strong> largest UNUSED area <strong>of</strong> the square.<br />

Guideline: Identify the largest UNMARKED area in the square. It doesn’t matter if<br />

there are 2 or 3 unmarked areas, use only one. Imagine drawing a bubble shape around<br />

this space (bubbles can bend, be triangular, circular, square or elipse, but if you have a<br />

bubble with a narrow waist, you have two areas!). Does the bubble represent (1) 0-10%;<br />

(2) 10-25%; (3) 25-55%; (4) 55-80%; (5) 80-100% <strong>of</strong> the area <strong>of</strong> the square.<br />

Simply by qu<strong>art</strong>ers. (1-2) up to _; (3) up to _; (4) _ to _ ; (5) more than _ . Empty<br />

squares score 5.<br />

EMOTIONAL TONE<br />

E-TONE +<br />

0 -<br />

Guideline: Scan the picture as a whole, decide which elements <strong>of</strong> the picture, taking<br />

account <strong>of</strong> what communicates TO YOU <strong>of</strong> the maker’s intent in content, colour,<br />

intensity, line and form. (+) positive or (-) negative. Mark the squares containing these<br />

elements first. <strong>The</strong> other squares are (0) neutral. Leave empty squares blank.<br />

DOMINANT FORM<br />

DEFINITION: A shape enclosed by a boundary, explicit or implicit. It should be<br />

exceptional in, size or colour (contrasting hue, intensity or saturation). It is not always<br />

a recognisable shape or person. It is a LARGE SINGLE SHAPE. When multiple,<br />

400


there is NO dominant form! <strong>The</strong> shape may be repeated, but repetitions will be smaller<br />

or less intense.<br />

Guideline: Scan the picture as a whole, decide where the dominant form is. Use closure<br />

for open forms. If more than 25% <strong>of</strong> the square is covered by the form, shade the<br />

corresponding square in the small grid at the top <strong>of</strong> the rating sheet .<br />

Half Rating sheet for DAPA. Template. 1999 Hacking and Foreman ©<br />

COLOUR<br />

INTENSITY<br />

L<br />

PAINT I<br />

N<br />

DRAW<br />

SPACE<br />

E-TONE<br />

COLOUR<br />

INTENSITY<br />

L<br />

PAINT I<br />

N<br />

DRAW<br />

SPACE<br />

E-TONE<br />

COLOUR<br />

INTENSITY<br />

L<br />

I<br />

N<br />

DRAW<br />

SPACE<br />

E-TONE<br />

COLOUR<br />

INTENSITY<br />

L<br />

PAINT I<br />

N<br />

DRAW<br />

SPACE<br />

E-TONE<br />

R Y O P G B N W K<br />

H N L<br />

H V N<br />

H V N<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

+ 0 -<br />

R Y O P G B N W K<br />

H N L<br />

H V N<br />

H V N<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

+ 0<br />

R Y O P G B N W K<br />

H N L<br />

H V N<br />

H V N<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

+ 0<br />

R<br />

Y O P G B N W K<br />

H N L<br />

H V N<br />

H V N<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

+ 0 401 -<br />

FORM ---<br />

R Y O P G B N W K<br />

H N L<br />

H V N<br />

H V N<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

+ 0 -<br />

R Y O P G B N W K<br />

H N L<br />

H V N<br />

H V N<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

+ 0<br />

R Y O P G B N W K<br />

H N L<br />

H V N<br />

H V N<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

+ 0<br />

R Y O P G B N W K<br />

H N L<br />

H V N<br />

H V N<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

+ 0 -


P<strong>art</strong> 2 Rating Sheet for DAPA: Hacking and Foreman 1999 ©<br />

R Y O P G B N W K<br />

H N L<br />

H V N<br />

H V N<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

+ 0<br />

R Y O P G B N W K<br />

H N L<br />

H V N<br />

H V N<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

+ 0 -<br />

R Y O P G B N W K<br />

H N L<br />

H V N<br />

H V N<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

+ 0 -<br />

R Y O P G B N W K<br />

H N L<br />

H V N<br />

H V N<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

+ 0<br />

R Y O P G B N W K<br />

H N L<br />

H V N<br />

H V N<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

+ 0<br />

R Y O P G B N W K<br />

H N L<br />

H V N<br />

H V N<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

+ 0 -<br />

R Y O P G B N W K<br />

H N L<br />

H V N<br />

H V N<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

+ 0 -<br />

R Y O P G B N W K<br />

H N L<br />

H V N<br />

H V N<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

+ 0<br />

402<br />

R Y O P G B N W K<br />

H N L<br />

H V N<br />

H V N<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

+ 0<br />

R Y O P G B N W K<br />

H N L<br />

H V N<br />

H V N<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

+ 0 -<br />

R Y O P G B N W K<br />

H N L<br />

H V N<br />

H V N<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

+ 0 -<br />

R Y O P G B N W K<br />

H N L<br />

H V N<br />

H V N<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

+ 0


Help Sheet for DAPA version 3. Main study. Hacking and Foreman 1999 ©<br />

403


Rating Sheet for Casenotes DAPA version 3. 1999.<br />

KEELE UNIVERSITY STAFFORDSHIRE. DEPT. OF PSYCHIATRY, SCHOOL OF<br />

P.G. MEDICINE. ICHRC. N. STAFFORDSHIRE HOSPITAL.<br />

UNIT NAME<br />

WARD M / F MARRIED/SINGLE/SEP RACE AGE<br />

ICD-10 MEDICATION<br />

DIAGNOSIS<br />

OCCUPATION/TRADE<br />

ED LEVEL<br />

ECT YES/NO INPATIENT/OUT EVER SECTIONED<br />

404


Rating Guide Version 2. S. Hacking and D. Foreman 1994. ©<br />

Descriptive Assessment for Psychiatric Art.<br />

Rating Guide Descriptive Assessment for Psychiatric Art<br />

V.2<br />

Used in the Pilot <strong>Study</strong> 1996.<br />

405


D.A.P.A. DESCRIPTIVE ASSESSMENT OF PSYCHIATRIC ARTWORK Pilot <strong>Study</strong><br />

- S. HACKING AND D. M. FOREMAN © 1994.<br />

RATING GUIDE<br />

<strong>The</strong> pictures must be rated for the presence <strong>of</strong> 13 elements on 5 primarily structural<br />

areas: colour, intensity, line, area, emotional tone. <strong>The</strong>se items are designed to describe<br />

the picture as objectively as possible. A grid is drawn over the picture forming 20<br />

squares. Rating looks at each category one at a time in each division. Colour rating <strong>of</strong>fers<br />

two options forcing a present/not present decision; the others <strong>of</strong>fer optional descriptors<br />

<strong>of</strong> the form high, medium or low. A total <strong>of</strong> 260 decisions are required to rate one<br />

picture. However, time for picture rating typically varies from 5-15m. A transparent<br />

overlay divides the picture into 20 squares; 5 across and 4 down, taking account <strong>of</strong> the<br />

intended ‘right way up’. <strong>The</strong> squares are drawn from the edge <strong>of</strong> the paper, ignoring<br />

borderlines done by the painter.<br />

Each <strong>of</strong> the 20 scoring squares on the rating sheet contains 5 rows <strong>of</strong> boxes. Each row is<br />

identified on the left; Colour; Intensity; Line; Area; Emotional Tone. See the example<br />

below<br />

COLOUR<br />

INTENSITY H N L<br />

LINE H V<br />

N<br />

10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90<br />

100<br />

AREA<br />

Colour<br />

R Y O P G B N W K<br />

COLOUR R Y O P G B N<br />

W K<br />

406


Colour is simple, one you know what the letters mean! Red, Yellow, Orange, Purple,<br />

Green, Blue, browN, White, blacK.<br />

DEFINITION: Colours are defined by their hue. It doesn’t matter if the shade is dark<br />

or See HELP for more information on colour description and examples. If the colour<br />

appears anywhere in the square mark the identifying letter. Only the media on the paper<br />

is scored, not the colour <strong>of</strong> the paper. Writing on the picture done by the painter is<br />

scored in the same way. Pencil is scored black.<br />

407


Intensity<br />

DEFINITION: <strong>The</strong> brightness or pureness <strong>of</strong> the media used, not the surrounding<br />

space. If a hgih intensity dot sits in the middle <strong>of</strong> an otherwise empty square, the square<br />

is scored high. If the surrounding area is slightly tinted, however, the square is scored<br />

low. Durll and strong mixes <strong>of</strong> colour occurring in the same square are scored on<br />

whichever covers most <strong>of</strong> the area. If there is equal cover, score whichever colour is<br />

highest. Leave blank if the square is empty.<br />

H – High strong, dense saturated colour – e.g. thick pure paint or very heavy pressured<br />

pencil or crayon.<br />

N – Neutral or neither, also ‘muddy’ colour; e.g. the colour has been ‘greyed’ or toned<br />

down, perhaps mixed to darker shades.<br />

L – Low intensity or ‘watery’ colour especially paint, little pigment, e.g. light pressured<br />

crayon or pencil or thinly scattered media over the area. See HELP for examples.<br />

Line<br />

DEFINITION: - Refer to help for examples. A line must be a distinct drawn or painted<br />

mark. A line is not two areas <strong>of</strong> colour which come together. Line can be used for<br />

scribble covering an area or pattern marks, but is not used to describe thin shapes filled<br />

with paint. Leave blank if no lines appear in the picture.<br />

Scored as H – Majority <strong>of</strong> tHick, or heavy lines with high pressure; V – Varied lines<br />

when both thick and thin lines are present; N – ThiN, or light pressure.<br />

Area<br />

INTENSITY H N L<br />

AREA<br />

LINE H V<br />

N<br />

10<br />

20<br />

30<br />

40<br />

Guidelines: Estimate how much <strong>of</strong> the square is covered by the media and mark the<br />

coverage on the scale in tenths or by 10%. A used area has media over it, no matter how<br />

408<br />

50<br />

60<br />

70<br />

80<br />

90<br />

100


thin or scattered. An unused area is completely empty.<br />

Emotional Tone<br />

E-TONE +<br />

0 -<br />

DEFINITION: subjective overall assessment <strong>of</strong> whether the square, its colour, intensity,<br />

line and content TO THE RATER seems (+) positive or happy; (0) neutral or neither<br />

(-) negative or sad. This is the only category where if the square is empty, score as 0 or<br />

neutral. DO NOT LEAVE BLANK.<br />

409


Form sheet for Artists. Side 2. DAPA development 1994 © sue Hacking.<br />

Age Sex m/f<br />

Art Training (tick) A level; Foundation; Degree; Post Grad.<br />

Currently practising?<br />

Media most used: paint; print; sculpture; 3d; design; other write<br />

This test takes about 5 minutes.<br />

Thank you for your co-operation in this test. I am seeking to understand what is meant<br />

by <strong>art</strong>istic terms in practice. Please answer both questions as clearly as possible.<br />

1. Look at the sheet <strong>of</strong> reproductions <strong>of</strong> works <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> (other side).<br />

For each picture, delineate in red, the dominant form, as closely as you can to the<br />

contours <strong>of</strong> what you see as the single most dominant form in that picture. If the<br />

question is inapplicable to the picture, write ‘none’ at the bottom.<br />

2. What do the words ‘dominant form’ mean to you in the above sentence? Give a brief<br />

definition <strong>of</strong> your understanding <strong>of</strong> what a form is. You may refer to the pictures if<br />

you wish. You can take from 2 to 3 sentences to half a page.<br />

410


3. Research Questionnaire on Dominant Form for Raters. Test sheet1. DAPA Hacking<br />

and Foreman 1999 ©<br />

Dominant Form<br />

This study is p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> an experiment to find ways <strong>of</strong> describing a picture. This is not the<br />

whole <strong>of</strong> the study but one <strong>of</strong> the elements being tested. Please try to understand the<br />

explanation given by the experimenter as if you were p<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> a group trying to score<br />

exactly the same as everyone else.<br />

DEFINITION:<br />

An object or space enclosed by a boundary, explicit or implicit.<br />

It should be exceptional in<br />

Size and/or<br />

Colour<br />

Contrasting hue/intensity/saturation.<br />

It should be single (there must not be two opposing forms).<br />

<strong>The</strong> shape may be repeated, the repetitions will be smaller or less intense. It is usually<br />

a regular shape.<br />

What is going on in the picture (the content or narrative) is secondary to the structure.<br />

It need not be a recognisable shape or a person.<br />

When you have understood the explanation to the experimenter’s satisfaction, you will<br />

be asked to rate some pictures. Keep this explanation by you and refer to it every time.<br />

<strong>The</strong> experimenter will now describe the grid system.<br />

Please look at the picture as a whole first, can you see a shape which is large, singular and<br />

whose outline is generally echoed throughout the picture?<br />

Point out to the experimenter which squares the shape occupies. Cut <strong>of</strong>f extremeties<br />

which are not integral to the regular shape. <strong>The</strong> shape must occupy more than 25% <strong>of</strong><br />

the square to be counted.<br />

411


Appendix 5<br />

Permission for study, information for p<strong>art</strong>icipants, and instructions for group leaders<br />

on treatment <strong>of</strong> pictures<br />

Consent forms for all p<strong>art</strong>icipants in the pilot and for those in the main study who were<br />

saving their pictures from therapy groups were the same, pages 2 and 3 (oral and written).<br />

Information sheet for pilot study and for those in the main study who gave consent were<br />

the same (p.4).<br />

Instructions for group leaders in pilot study (p.5).<br />

Instructions for group leaders in main study (p.6).<br />

412


North Staffordshire Health Authority<br />

Research Ethics Committee<br />

PERSONAL CONSENT TO THE CONDUCT OF A RESEARCH<br />

INVESTIGATION<br />

STUDY TITLE Describing pictures by different groups <strong>of</strong> patients<br />

NAME OF CLINICIAN Dr. D. Foreman and Ms. S. Hacking<br />

<strong>The</strong> aims and procedures <strong>of</strong> the clinical investigation in which I have been asked to take<br />

p<strong>art</strong> have been explained to me by ward staff. I have read and understood the patient<br />

leaflet set out overleaf, and have been informed about the possible benefit to myself and<br />

about any foreseeable risks or discomfort.<br />

I have had the opportunity to ask questions and to consider the answers given.<br />

I understand that p<strong>art</strong>icipation in the study is voluntary and that I may withdraw from the<br />

study at any time <strong>of</strong> my own accord. If I do withdraw it will not affect the future care and<br />

attention which I will receive from my doctors.<br />

I agree that the relevant p<strong>art</strong>s <strong>of</strong> my medical records may be disclosed to Dr. Foreman<br />

provided they agree not to reveal my name.<br />

I hereby freely give my fully informed consent to taking p<strong>art</strong> in this clinical investigation.<br />

Name ……………………………….. Signature …………………………….<br />

Date ………………………………….<br />

I confirm that I have explained the nature <strong>of</strong> the above investigation to the above named<br />

patient.<br />

Name ……………………………….. Signature …………………………….<br />

Date ………………………………….<br />

413


North Staffordshire Health Authority<br />

Research Ethics Committee<br />

ORAL CONSENT TO THE CONDUCT OF A RESEARCH INVESTIGATION<br />

STUDY TITLE Describing pictures by different groups <strong>of</strong> patients<br />

NAME OF CLINICIAN Dr. D. Foreman and Ms. S. Hacking<br />

NAME OF PATIENT<br />

I have explained the aims and procedures <strong>of</strong> the above clinical investigation to the above<br />

named patient. He/she was informed <strong>of</strong> the possible benefits to him/herself and about any<br />

foreseeable risks or discomfort (and the information in the patient leaflet was also<br />

explained).<br />

He/she was given the time and opportunity to ask questions and to consider the answers<br />

given.<br />

<strong>The</strong> voluntary nature <strong>of</strong> p<strong>art</strong>icipation in the study was emphasised, as was the right to<br />

withdraw from the study for any reason without prejudicing his/her relationship with<br />

myslef or any other <strong>of</strong> his/her medical advisors.<br />

I have explained that relevant p<strong>art</strong>s <strong>of</strong> my medical records may be disclosed to Dr.<br />

Foreman. Names will not be disclosed.<br />

On this basis, I declare that the above named patient freely gave his/her consent to taking<br />

p<strong>art</strong> in this clinical investigation.<br />

Witness to Oral Consent<br />

Name ……………………………….. Signature …………………………….<br />

JOB TITLE OR RELATIONSHIP TO PATIENT ……………………………..<br />

Date ………………………………….<br />

414


Information sheet. Research into Art <strong>The</strong>rapy<br />

Painting for pleasure can have healing effects. Paintings may express what is deep inside<br />

the mind, that may not be easily put into words.<br />

Our understanding <strong>of</strong> a picture is <strong>of</strong>ten brought about through talking to people about it,<br />

but people's comments about their paintings are sometimes not helpful. This research is<br />

trying to discover a way <strong>of</strong> looking at the pictures themselves rather than what is said<br />

about them.<br />

We will be looking at a lot <strong>of</strong> pictures by people with the same kinds <strong>of</strong> problems, or with<br />

no problems at all. So any picture will be just one <strong>of</strong> a group. We do not just want 'special'<br />

pictures or p<strong>art</strong>icularly good pictures. It is more important to have a variety, so we would<br />

like a picture from anyone willing to do one. It does not matter if anyone cannot draw as<br />

well as they would wish as this is not an <strong>art</strong>istic project.<br />

You will be asked to paint a picture in a group. <strong>The</strong> picture will be numbered. Your name<br />

will not appear on it. No-one will see the picture but the people doing research. We have<br />

no connection with any treatment or ward.<br />

You will not be asked to discuss the pictures with anyone. <strong>The</strong> pictures will be taken<br />

away and used as a group study. We will keep a list <strong>of</strong> pictures for the time <strong>of</strong> the study<br />

and then the list will be destroyed.<br />

If you do not wish to help, it will not affect any treatment or care. I you want to do a<br />

picture, and then decide not to after all, or take your picture out <strong>of</strong> the study once it is<br />

done, this is your choice.<br />

Thank you for your help and co-operation.<br />

415


Psychiatry Dept., School P.G. Medicine.<br />

Researcher Sue Hacking.<br />

Research Project - DRAW A PICTURE. <strong>Study</strong> 1.<br />

PROTOCOL.<br />

If something goes wrong, or there is some alteration to the procedure, would you please<br />

write on the back <strong>of</strong> this paper what it was - and return it with the completed pictures.<br />

Allow about one hour for the session.<br />

Obtain consent from individuals before the series, using the information sheets and consent<br />

forms provided, if someone refuses, they may still attend their usual sessions with the<br />

group.<br />

On the reverse <strong>of</strong> this paper write ward no. and group leader. Give every p<strong>art</strong>icipant a<br />

number beginning ______ so the first one would be ___1. Write the number and their<br />

names on the back <strong>of</strong> this paper, so they can be identified.<br />

Note the refusers like this: male/female who chose not to take p<strong>art</strong>, no<br />

names.<br />

1. Make sure that each person has access to the following materials on their table:<br />

RED YELLOW ORANGE PURPLE GREEN BLUE BROWN WHITE BLACK, available<br />

for use, i.e. red and yellow do not provide orange, orange must be mixed and available.<br />

2. Facilities to make thick and thin lines, preferably with different colours.<br />

3. One piece <strong>of</strong> paper, A3 size - as big as 2 sheets <strong>of</strong> photocopy paper.<br />

<strong>The</strong>med Sessions, one theme per session.<br />

1. Draw yourself as you usually are.<br />

2. Draw yourself as you might look if you were an animal.<br />

3. Draw a picture <strong>of</strong> yourself doing something you like to do.<br />

4. Draw a picture <strong>of</strong> your life with the past and the future on it.<br />

5. Draw a picture <strong>of</strong> yourself as you are feeling now.<br />

Other instructions to the group:<br />

You can put anything in the picture and you can use any colours that you want to use.<br />

You will not have to show it to anyone, or talk about it, you have up to one hour to finish<br />

the picture.<br />

Function <strong>of</strong> the researcher as helper to the group leader. To ensure that the p<strong>art</strong>icipants<br />

make as free a choice as possible as to what colours they should use, and what kind <strong>of</strong><br />

picture they should draw. To encourage p<strong>art</strong>icipants and discourage dropout if possible.<br />

P<strong>art</strong>icipants who finish early may do something else.<br />

416


Psychiatry Dept., School P.G. Medicine.<br />

Researcher Sue Hacking.<br />

Research Project - DRAW A PICTURE - PROTOCOL. <strong>Study</strong> 2.<br />

KEEP THIS PAPER<br />

Instructions for group leader. Please save the paintings from your themed or recreational<br />

sessions with patients. So that we can identify patients please follow these instructions.<br />

If something goes wrong, or there is some alteration to the procedure, would you please<br />

write on the back <strong>of</strong> this paper what it was - and return it with the completed pictures.<br />

Allow about one hour for the session.<br />

Obtain consent from individuals before the series, using the information sheets and consent<br />

forms provided, if someone refuses, they may still attend their usual sessions with the<br />

group.<br />

On the reverse <strong>of</strong> this paper write ward no. and group leader and the date. Give every<br />

p<strong>art</strong>icipant, including staff who provide a picture, a number beginning ______ so the first<br />

one would be ___1. Write the number and their names on the back <strong>of</strong> this paper, so they<br />

can be identified.<br />

Note the refusers like this: male/female who chose not to take p<strong>art</strong>, no<br />

names.<br />

Try to provide these materials on every table:<br />

RED YELLOW ORANGE PURPLE GREEN BLUE BROWN WHITE BLACK, available<br />

for use, i.e. red and yellow do not provide orange, orange must be mixed and available.<br />

2. <strong>The</strong>re should be facilities to make thick and thin lines, in colour, best achieved with<br />

paint and preferable for this research.<br />

3. One piece <strong>of</strong> paper, A3 size - as big as 2 sheets <strong>of</strong> photocopy paper, placed so that the<br />

top (furthest away from the <strong>art</strong>ist) is the longer side (landscape format).<br />

If there are limitations, please note them on the back <strong>of</strong> this paper.<br />

You can use any type <strong>of</strong> sessions for this research, themed or free sessions or personal<br />

work. Self portraits are p<strong>art</strong>icularly useful. Do not direct the p<strong>art</strong>icipants as to colours<br />

used, or suggest a type <strong>of</strong> picture they should draw for the research. P<strong>art</strong>icipants should<br />

not do anything different than they usually do in their <strong>art</strong> session (i.e. talk about their<br />

pictures or show them to anyone else if they usually do not). Be encouraging and<br />

supportive and discourage dropout if possible.<br />

Write on the back <strong>of</strong> the picture, the numbers and not the names <strong>of</strong> all p<strong>art</strong>icipants<br />

including staff. Keep for collection. Thankyou for your co-operation.<br />

417


Bibliography<br />

418


Primary Sources<br />

Journals and papers<br />

Bibliography<br />

Adler, H.M. (1997), Towards a multimodal communication theory <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy: the<br />

vicarious coprocession, Am. J. Psychotherapy, Win., V.51(1): 54-66.<br />

Aikmanm K.G., Belter, R.W., and Finch, A.J. (1992), Human Figure Drawings: Validity<br />

in assessing intellectual level and academic achievement, J. Clin. Psychol., V.48(1): 114-<br />

120.<br />

Aldridge, D. (1994), Single-CAse Research Designs for the Creative Art <strong>The</strong>rapist, Art<br />

Psychotherapy, V.21(5), pp.333-342.<br />

Amos, S. (1982), <strong>The</strong> Diagnostic, Prognostic, and <strong>The</strong>rapeutic Implications <strong>of</strong><br />

Schizophrenic Art, Art Psychotherapy, V.9:131-43.<br />

Anastasi, A., Foley, J.P. (1941) A survey <strong>of</strong> the literature on <strong>art</strong>istic behavior in the<br />

abnormal: Historical and theoretical background, J. Gen. Psychol., V.25: 111-142.<br />

Anastasi, A., Foley, J.P., (1940) A survey <strong>of</strong> the literature on <strong>art</strong>istic behavior in the<br />

abnormal: Spontaneous productions. Psychol. Mon., V.52(6): 1-71.<br />

Anastasi, A., Foley, J.P. (1941) A survey <strong>of</strong> the literature on <strong>art</strong>istic behavior in the<br />

abnormal: Experimental investigations, J. Gen. Psychol., V.25:187-237.<br />

Anastasi, A., Foley, J.P. (1944) An experimental study <strong>of</strong> the drawing behavior <strong>of</strong> adult<br />

psychotics in comparison with that <strong>of</strong> a normal control group, J. Exp.l Psychol. V.34:<br />

169-94.<br />

Arnheim, R. <strong>The</strong> Thoughts That Made Me Move, Art Psychotherapy V.21(4):245-6.<br />

Arrington, D. (1991), Thinking Systems - Seeing Systems: an integrative model for<br />

systemically oriented <strong>art</strong> therapy, Art Psychotherapy, V.18:201-211.<br />

Bachant, J.L., and Adler, E. (1997), Transference: Co-constructed or brought to the<br />

interaction? J. Am. Psychoanalytic Assessment, V.45(4): 1097-1120.<br />

Ballentino, R.C. (1998), <strong>The</strong> Need for a New Ethical Model in Medicine: a challenge for<br />

conventional, alternative and complementary practitioners, Advances Win., V.14(1): 6-<br />

16.<br />

Bergland, C., and Gonzalez, M. (1993), Art and Madness, can the interface be quantified?<br />

Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, Feb., V.31: 81-90.<br />

Billig, O., (1971) Is Schizophrenic Expression Art? A comparative study <strong>of</strong> creativeness<br />

and schizophrenic thinking, J. Nervous Mental Dis. V.153(3): 149-64.<br />

Birchtnell, J. (1981), Is Art <strong>The</strong>rapeutic? Inscape, V.I, p.10-13.<br />

Borrowsky Junge, M. and Linesch, D. (1993), Our Own Voices: new paradigms for <strong>art</strong><br />

therapy research, Art Psychotherapy,V.20: 61-7.<br />

Bradley, M.T. R.D. Gupta (1997), Estimating the Effect <strong>of</strong> the File Drawer Problem in<br />

Meta Analysis, Perceptual and Motor Skills, V.65(2):719-22.<br />

Buchalter-Katz, S. (1985), Observations concerning the <strong>art</strong> productions <strong>of</strong> depressed<br />

patients in short term psychiatric facilities, Art Psychotherapy, Spring, V.12(1):35-8.<br />

Burke, J. (1988), Field trials <strong>of</strong> the 1987 draft <strong>of</strong> Chapter V. (F) <strong>of</strong> ICD-10, British J.<br />

419


Psychiatry, 152(suppl.1): 33-57.<br />

Cagnoletta, M.D. (1983), Artwork as a representation <strong>of</strong> object relation in therapeutic<br />

practice, Pratt Institute Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapy Review, V.4:46-52.<br />

Champernowe, J. (1971) Art and <strong>The</strong>rapy: an uneasy p<strong>art</strong>nership? Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy,<br />

April, X(3): 131-143.<br />

Cohen, B.M., Cox, C.T. (1989), Breaking the code: identification <strong>of</strong> multiplicity through<br />

<strong>art</strong> productions, Dissociation Progress in the Dissociative Disorders, Sep., V.2(3): 132-<br />

137.<br />

Cohen, B.M., Hammer, J., Singer, S. (1988), <strong>The</strong> Diagnostic Drawing Series: a systematic<br />

approach to <strong>art</strong> therapy evaluation and research, Art Psychotherapy, V.15(1): 11-21.<br />

Cohen, B.M., Mills, A., Kijak, A.K. (1994), An introduction to the DDS: a standardised<br />

tool for diagnostic and clinical use, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, V.11(2): 105-110.<br />

Cooper, J.E. (1988), <strong>The</strong> structure and presentation <strong>of</strong> contemporary psychiatric<br />

classifications with special reference to ICD9 and 10. British Journal Psychiatry,<br />

V.152(suppl.1):21-8.<br />

Couch, J.B. (1994), DDS research with older people diagnosed with organic mental<br />

syndrome and disorders, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy,V.11(2):111-5.<br />

Crane, R.R., Levy, B.I. (1962), Color scales in responses to emotionally laden situations,<br />

J.Consulting Psychol V.26(6):515-9.<br />

Cressen, R., Artistic quality <strong>of</strong> drawing and judges evaluations <strong>of</strong> the DAP, J. Personality<br />

Assessment, 1975, V.39: 132-137.<br />

Cupchik, G.C., and Gebotys, R.J. (1988), <strong>The</strong> search for meaning in <strong>art</strong>: interpretive<br />

styles and judgement <strong>of</strong> quality, Visual Arts Research V.14:138-50.<br />

Dalley, T. (1980), Assessing the therapeutic effects <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>: an illustrated case study, Art<br />

Psychotherapy, V.7: 11-17. Abell, S.C., Heiberg, A.M., and Johnson, J.E. (1994),<br />

Cognitive evaluation <strong>of</strong> young adults by means <strong>of</strong> human figure drawings: an empirical<br />

validation <strong>of</strong> 2 methods, J. Clin. Psychol., Nov., V.50(6):900-905.<br />

D'Andrade, R., Egan, M. (1974), <strong>The</strong> colours <strong>of</strong> emotion, Am. Ethnologist, Feb.<br />

V.1(1):49-63.<br />

Dent, J.K., and Kwiatkowska, H.Y. (1970), Aesthetic preferences in young adults for<br />

pictures drawn by mental patients and by members <strong>of</strong> their immediate families, Sciences<br />

de L'<strong>art</strong> (English Abstract), V.7(1-2): 43-54.<br />

Ellenberger, H.F. (1968), <strong>The</strong> Concept <strong>of</strong> Creative Illness, Psychoanalytic Review, 1968,<br />

V.55, pp.442-456.<br />

Enachesu, C. (1971) Aspects <strong>of</strong> pictorial creation in manic-depressive psychosis, Confina<br />

Psychiatrica, V.14(2): 133-142.<br />

Engels, G.L. (1977), <strong>The</strong> Need for a New Medical Model: a challenge for biomedicine?<br />

Science, April 8, V.196:129-37.<br />

Fabry, G.H., and Bertinetti, J.E. (1990), A Construct Validation <strong>Study</strong> <strong>of</strong> the Human<br />

Figure Drawing Test, Perceptual and Motor Skills, V.70:465-466.<br />

Feher, E., Vandicreek, L., Taglasi, H. (1983), <strong>The</strong> problem <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> quality in the use <strong>of</strong><br />

human figure drawings, J. Clin. Psychol., March, V.39(2): 268-275.<br />

Franklin, M., and Plitsky, R. (1992), <strong>The</strong> Problems <strong>of</strong> Interpretation: Implications and<br />

420


strategies for the field <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy, Art Psychotherapy, V.19(3): 163-175.<br />

Gantt, L., and Schmal, M. (1974), Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, A Bibliography (1940-73), George<br />

Washington University and National Institute <strong>of</strong> Mental Health, U.S.A. (unpublished<br />

report).<br />

Gantt, L. (1986), Systematic investigation <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> works: some research models drawn<br />

from neighboring fields, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, May, V.24(4): 111-118.<br />

Gantt, L., and Howie, J. (1979), Ch<strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> correspondences in diagnostic characteristics<br />

<strong>of</strong> mental disorder and <strong>art</strong>work (unpublished 1979).<br />

Garvey, M.J., Luxenberg, M. (1987), Comparison <strong>of</strong> color preference in derpressives and<br />

controls, Psychopathology, V.20:268-271.<br />

Gertler, L. (1985), Art as <strong>The</strong>rapy with an aging <strong>art</strong>ist, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, Feb.,<br />

V.23(3): 93-9.<br />

Gibson, J.J. (1971), <strong>The</strong> information available in pictures, Leonardo, V.4, p.27-35.<br />

Goodman, R., Agell, L., Gantt, L., and Williams, K. (1994), Are there Doctors in the<br />

House? Does Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Need a Cure? Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, August, V.33:3-13.<br />

Gorelick, K. (1989), Rapproachement between the <strong>art</strong>s and psychotherapies: Metaphor<br />

the mediator, Art Psychotherapy, Fall, V.16(3): 149-155.<br />

Gregorian, V.S., Azarian, A., DeMaria, M.B., and McDonald, L.D. (1996), Colors <strong>of</strong><br />

disaster: the psychology <strong>of</strong> the "black sun", Art Psychotherapy, V.23(1): 1-14.<br />

Gruber, H.E. (1988), Coping with multiplicity and ambiguity <strong>of</strong> meaning in works <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong>,<br />

Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, V.3(3): 183-189.<br />

Gulbro-Leavitt, C., Schimmel, B. (1991), Assessing Depression in children and<br />

adolescents using the Diagnostic Drawing Series modified for children (DDS-C), Art<br />

Psychotherapy, V.18(4): 353-356.<br />

Hacking, S., Foreman, D., Belcher, J. (1996), <strong>The</strong> Descriptive Assessment for Psychiatric<br />

Artwork (DAPA): a new way <strong>of</strong> quantifying paintings by psychiatric patients, J.<br />

Nervous and Mental Disease, V.184(7):425-430.<br />

Hagood, M. (1990), Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Research in England: impressions <strong>of</strong> an American <strong>art</strong><br />

therapist, Art Psychotherapy, V.17(1): 75-9.<br />

Healy, D. (1998), Commentry: meta analysis <strong>of</strong> trials comparing anti-depressants with<br />

active placebos, British J. Psychiatry, V.17:232-4<br />

Heine, D., and Steiner, M. (1986), Standardised paintings as a proposed adjunct<br />

instrument for monitoring mood states: a preliminary note, Occupational <strong>The</strong>rapy in<br />

Mental Health, Fall, V.6(3): 21-27.<br />

Johnson, D.R. (1987) <strong>The</strong> role <strong>of</strong> the creative <strong>art</strong>s therapies in the diagnosis and<br />

treatment <strong>of</strong> psychological trauma, Art Psychotherapy V.14: 7-13.<br />

Johnson, F.A., and Greenberg, R.P. (1978), Quality <strong>of</strong> Drawing as a Factor in the<br />

Interpretation <strong>of</strong> Figure Drawings, J. Personality Assessment, 1978, V.42(5):489-495.<br />

Kahill, S. (1984), Human Figure Drawing in Adults: an update <strong>of</strong> the empirical evidence<br />

1967-1982, Canadian Psychol. V.25(4):269-292.<br />

Kamphaus, R.W., Pleiss, K.L. (1991), Draw-A-Person techniques: tests in search <strong>of</strong> a<br />

construct, J. Sch. Psychol., Win. V.29(4):395-401.<br />

Kaplan, F.F. (1994), <strong>The</strong> imagery and expression <strong>of</strong> anger: an initial study, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy,<br />

421


V.11: 139-143.<br />

Kaplan, F.F. (1996), Positive Images <strong>of</strong> Anger in an Anger Management Workshop, Art<br />

Psychotherapy, V.23(1): 69-75.<br />

Kaplan, F.F. (1991), Drawing assessment and <strong>art</strong>istic skill, Art Psychotherapy,<br />

V.18:347-52.<br />

Kay, S.R. (1978), Qualitative differences in human figure drawings according to<br />

schizophrenic subtype, Perceptual Motor Skills, V.47: 923-932.<br />

Kirk, A., Kertesz, A. (1989), Hemispheric contributions to drawing, Neuropsychologia,<br />

V.27(6):881-6.<br />

Knapp, N.M., (1994), Research with diagnostic drawings for normal and Alzheimer's<br />

subjects, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, V.11(2):131-8.<br />

Koppitz, E. (1983), Projective Drawings in Children and Adults, School Psychol.<br />

Review, V.12: 421-427.<br />

Kramer, E.S., and Iager, A.C. (1984), <strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> in assessment <strong>of</strong> psychotic disorders:<br />

changing perspectives, Art Psychotherapy, V.11:197-201.<br />

Kris, E. (1953) Review <strong>of</strong> Schizophrenic Art by Margaret Naumberg, Psychoanalytic<br />

Qu<strong>art</strong>erly, V.22:98-101.<br />

Kulik J. (1983), Review <strong>of</strong> G.V. Glass et al. (1981) Evaluation News, V.4:101-5.<br />

Langevin, R. and Hutchins, L.M. (1973), An experimental investigation <strong>of</strong> judges ratings<br />

<strong>of</strong> schizophrenic and non-schizophrenic's paintings, J. Personality Assessment, Dec.,<br />

V.37(1): 537-543.<br />

Langevin, R., Raine, M., Day, D., and Waxer, K. (1975), Art experience, intelligence and<br />

formal features in psychotics' paintings, Art Psychotherapy, V.2(2):149-158.<br />

Levy, B.I. (1980), Research into the psychological meaning <strong>of</strong> colour, Am. J. Art<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy, V.19: 87-91 and reprint V.23 (1984).<br />

Levy, A.J., Barowsky, E.I. (1986), Comparison <strong>of</strong> computer-administered Harris-<br />

Goodenough Draw-A-Man Test with standard paper-and-pencil administration,<br />

Perceptual and Motor Skills, Oct., V.63(2, pt.1): 395-398.<br />

Levy, B.I. and Ulman, E. (1967), Judging Psychopathology from Paintings, J. Abnormal<br />

Psychol., V.72(2): 182-7, reprinted 1975, 1984, 1992.<br />

Levy, B.I. and Ulman, E. (1974), <strong>The</strong> effect <strong>of</strong> training on judging <strong>psychopathology</strong> from<br />

paintings, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, Oct. 1974, V.14: 24-25 reprinted 1984, 1992.<br />

Linesch, D. (1994), Interpretation in Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Research and Practice: <strong>The</strong><br />

Hermeneutic Circle, Art Psychotherapy, V.21(3): 185-195.<br />

Maclagan, D. (1995), Fantasy and the aesthetic: have they become the uninvited guests<br />

at <strong>art</strong> therapy's feast? Art Psychotherapy, V.22(3): 217-221.<br />

Maclagan, D. (1989), <strong>The</strong> Aesthetic Dimension <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapy: luxury or neccessity?,<br />

Inscape, Spring, pp.10-13.<br />

Maitland-Gholson, J.C. (1985), Implications <strong>of</strong> selected studies in psychology for visual<br />

<strong>art</strong>s research, Visual Arts Research, Fall, V.11(2), issue 22): 21-30.<br />

Malchiodi, C. (1993), Introduction to special issue on <strong>art</strong> therapy and pr<strong>of</strong>essionalism,<br />

Is there a crisis in Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Education? Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, 10(3): 122.<br />

422


Males, B. Is it right to carry out scientific research into <strong>art</strong> therapy? <strong>The</strong>rapy, 3 May<br />

1979:5.<br />

Males, B. (1980), Art <strong>The</strong>rapy: Investigations and implications, Inscape, 4(2): 13-15.<br />

M<strong>art</strong>in, I.G. (1982), Universal vs learned emotional responses to colors: afterthoughts<br />

to thesis research, Art Psychotherapy, V.9:245-7.<br />

McNiff, S. (1979), From Shamanism to Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, Art Psychotherapy, V.6(3):155-61.<br />

McNiff, S. (1986), Freedom <strong>of</strong> Research and Artistic Inquiry, Art Psychotherapy, V.13:<br />

279-284.<br />

McNiff, S. (1987), Research and Scholarship in the Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapies, Art<br />

Psychotherapy, V.14: 285-292.<br />

McNiff, S. (1994), Celebrating the life and work <strong>of</strong> Rudolf Arnheim, Art Psychotherapy<br />

V.21(4): 247-248.<br />

McNiff, S. (1994), Rudolf Arnheim, A Clinician <strong>of</strong> Images, Art Psychotherapy V.21(4):<br />

249-260.<br />

Miljkovitch, M., Irvine, G.M. (1982), Comparison <strong>of</strong> drawing performances <strong>of</strong><br />

schizophrenics, other psychiatric patients and normal schoolchildren on a draw-a-village<br />

task, Art Psychotherapy, V.9: 203-216.<br />

Mills, A., Cohen, B.M., Meneses, J.Z. (1993), Reliability and Validity tests <strong>of</strong> the<br />

Diagnostic Drawing Series, Art Psychotherapy, V.20: 83-88.<br />

Moon, B. (1994), What Kind <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, Art Psychotherapy V.21(4): 295-298.<br />

Moore, R.W. (1983), Art <strong>The</strong>rapy with Substance Abusers: a review <strong>of</strong> the literature,<br />

Art Psychotherapy, V.10: 251-260.<br />

Morris, M.B. (1995), <strong>The</strong> Diagnostic Drawing Series (DDS) and the Tree Rating Scale:<br />

an isomorphic representation <strong>of</strong> Multiple Personality Disorder, Manic Depressive and<br />

Schizophrenic populations, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, V.12(2): 118-128.<br />

Muenchow, D.C., Aresenian, J. (1974) An <strong>art</strong>ist in turmoil during <strong>art</strong> therapy, Am. J. Art<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy, V.14(1):18-23.<br />

Naglieri, J.A., Pfeiffer, S.I. (1992), Performance <strong>of</strong> disruptive behavior disordered and<br />

normal samples on the Draw A Person: Screening Procedure for Emotional Disturbance,<br />

Psychological Assessment, Jun., V.4(2): 156-159.<br />

Neale, E.L. (1994), <strong>The</strong> Childrens' Diagnostic Drawing Series (CDDS), Art <strong>The</strong>rapy,<br />

11(2): 119-126.<br />

Nodine, C.F., Locher, P.J. and Krupinski, E.A. (1993), <strong>The</strong> role <strong>of</strong> formal <strong>art</strong> training on<br />

perception and aesthetics in the making <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> comparisons, Leonardo, V.26: 219-27.<br />

Phillips, E.L., Geller, S.K., and Ireland, M. (1983), Research on the use <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy in<br />

a university setting, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, Oct., V.23(1): 26-29.<br />

Politsky, R.H. (1995), Towards a Typology <strong>of</strong> Research in the Creative Arts <strong>The</strong>rapies,<br />

Art Psychotherapy, V.22(4): 307-314.<br />

Quail, J., Peavy, R.W. (1994) A Phenomenological Research <strong>Study</strong> <strong>of</strong> a Client's<br />

Experience in Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, Art Psychotherapy, V.21(1): 45-57.<br />

Regier, D.A., Kaelver, C.T., Roper, M.T., Rae, D.S., S<strong>art</strong>orius, N. (1994), <strong>The</strong> ICD-10<br />

Clinical Field Train for Mental and Behavioral Disorders: results in Canada and the<br />

United States, Am. J. Psychiatry, V.151(9):1340-1350.<br />

423


Reuven, S.K. (1998), Reversal <strong>of</strong> a body image disorder (Macrosomatognosia) in<br />

Parkinson's disease by treatment with AC pulsed electromagnetic fields, Int. J. <strong>of</strong><br />

Neuroscience, V.93:1-2.<br />

Riley, S. (1997), Conflicts in Treatment Issues <strong>of</strong> Liberation, Connection and Culture:<br />

Art therapy for women and their families, Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, V.14(2): 102-8.<br />

Rogers, S. and Costall, A. (1983), On the Horizon: Picture perception and Gibson's<br />

concept <strong>of</strong> information, Leonardo, V.16(3): 180-2.<br />

Russell-Lacy, S., Robinson, V., Benson, J., Cranage, J. (1979), An Experimental <strong>Study</strong><br />

<strong>of</strong> Pictures Produced by Acute Schizophrenic Subjects, British Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry,<br />

V134: 195-200.<br />

S<strong>art</strong>orius, N. (1991), <strong>The</strong> classification <strong>of</strong> mental disorders in the Tenth Revision <strong>of</strong> the<br />

International Classification <strong>of</strong> Diseases. European Psychiatry, V.6: 315-322.<br />

S<strong>art</strong>orius, N., Bedirhan Ustan, T., Korten, A., Cooper, J.E., Van Drimmelen, J. (1995),<br />

Progress toward achieving a common language in psychiatry, II: results from the<br />

international field trials <strong>of</strong> the ICD-10 diagnostic criteria for research for mental and<br />

behavioral disorders, Am. J. Psychiatry, V.152(10):1427-1437.<br />

Schmidt, J.A., McLaughlin, J.P., Leighten, P. (1989), Novice strategies for understanding<br />

paintings, Applied Cognitive Psychology, Jan-Mar., V.3(1): 65-72.<br />

Scott, R. (1981), Measuring intelligence with the Goodenough-Harris drawing test,<br />

Psychological Bulletin, V.89:483-505.<br />

Shoemaker, R.H. (1978), <strong>The</strong> significance <strong>of</strong> the first picture in <strong>art</strong> therapy, Proceedings<br />

<strong>of</strong> the 8th Annual Conference <strong>of</strong> the American Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Association, pp.156-62.<br />

Silver, R. and Ellison, J. (1992), Identifying and Assessing Self-Images in Drawings by<br />

Delinquent Adolescents, Art Psychotherapy, V.22(4): 339-352.<br />

Sims, J., Dona, R.H., and Bolton, B. (1983), <strong>The</strong> validity <strong>of</strong> the DAP as an anxiety<br />

measure, J. Pers. Assessment, V.47:250-7.<br />

Smith, M. & G. Glass, (1980) Meta Analysis <strong>of</strong> research on class size and its<br />

relationship to attitudes and instruction, Am. Educational Research J., V.17:419-33.<br />

Smitskamp, H. (1995), <strong>The</strong> Problem <strong>of</strong> Pr<strong>of</strong>essional Diagnosis in the Arts <strong>The</strong>rapies, Art<br />

Psychotherapy, V.22, No.3, p.181-187.<br />

Slansky, L., and Short-Degraff, M. (1989), Validity and Reliability Issues with Human<br />

Figure Drawing Assessments, Physical and Occupational <strong>The</strong>rapy in Paediatrics,<br />

V.9(3):127-142.<br />

Sohn, D. (1997), Questions for meta analysis, Psychological Reports, V.81(1):3-15.<br />

Sternlicht, M., Rosenfeld, P., Siegel, L. (1973), Retesting with graphic production:<br />

resolution <strong>of</strong> a diagnostic dilemma, Art Psychotherapy, Win., V.1(3-4): 299-300.<br />

Strube, M. (1985) Combining and comparing significance levels from non-independent<br />

hypothesis tests, Psychological Bulletin V.97:334-341.<br />

Swenson, C.H. (1957), Empirical Evaluations <strong>of</strong> Human Figure Drawings, Psychological<br />

Bull., V.54: 431-466.<br />

Swenson, C.H. (1968), Empirical Evaluations <strong>of</strong> Human Figure Drawings, Psychological<br />

Bull., V.70: 20-44.<br />

Syristova, E. (1989), <strong>The</strong> creative potential <strong>of</strong> schizophrenic psychosis: Its importance<br />

424


for psychotherapy, Studia Psychologica, V.31(4): 283-294.<br />

Tate, F.B., Allen, H. (1985), Color preferences and the aged individual: implications for<br />

<strong>art</strong> therapy, Art Psychotherapy, Fall, V.12(3): 165-169.<br />

Trevisan, M.S. (1996), Review <strong>of</strong> the Draw a Person: Screening Procedure for Emotional<br />

Disturbance, Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, Jan., V.28(4):<br />

225-8.<br />

Trowbridge, M.M. (1995), Graphic indicators <strong>of</strong> sexual abuse in children's drawings: a<br />

review <strong>of</strong> the literature, Art Psychotherapy, V.22(5): 405-93.<br />

Ulman, E. and Levy, B.I. (1968), An Experimental Approach to the Judgement <strong>of</strong><br />

Psychopathology from Paintings, Bull. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, Oct., V.8(1): 3-12 reprinted 1975,<br />

1984, 1992.<br />

Ulman, E. and Levy, B.I. (1973), Art <strong>The</strong>rapists as Diagnosticians, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy,<br />

Oct., V.13: 35-38 reprinted 1984, 1992.<br />

Wadeson, H. and Bunney W.E. (1970), Manic Depressive Art: a systematic study <strong>of</strong><br />

differences in a 48 hour cyclic patient, J. Nervous and Mental Disease V.150:215-31.<br />

Wadeson, H. (1975), Is interpretation <strong>of</strong> sexual symbolism necessary? Art<br />

Psychotherapy, V.2(3-4): 233-239.<br />

Wadeson, H. (1975) Suicide: Expression in Images, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy V.14:75-82.<br />

Wadeson, H. and Carpenter, W.T. (1976), A Comparative <strong>Study</strong> <strong>of</strong> Art Expression <strong>of</strong><br />

Schizophrenic, Unipolar Depressive and Bipolar Manic-Depressive Patients, J. Nervous<br />

and Mental Disease, May, V.162(5): 334-344.<br />

Wadeson, H. (1978), Some Uses <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Data in Research, Am. J. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy,<br />

Oct., V.18(1): 11-18.<br />

Wadlington, W.L. and McWhinnie, H.J. (1973), <strong>The</strong> development <strong>of</strong> a rating scale for the<br />

study <strong>of</strong> formal aesthetic qualities in the paintings <strong>of</strong> mental patients, Art<br />

Psychotherapy, Win., Vol.1(3-4): 201-220.<br />

Woddis, J. (1986), Judging by Appearances, Art Psychotherapy, V.13(2):147-149.<br />

Wright, H. and McIntyre, M.P. (1982), <strong>The</strong> Family Drawing Depression Scale, J. Clin.<br />

Psychol., V.38(4): 853-61.<br />

Primary sources<br />

Books<br />

Adair, J.G. and Bellanger, D., Dish, k., Sabourin, M. (eds.) Advances in Psychological<br />

Science: international congress <strong>of</strong> Psychology, Montreal 1996,, V.1, Hove: Psychological<br />

Press.<br />

Altman, D.G (1991). Practical Statistics for Medical Research, 5th ed. 1994, London:<br />

Chapman and Hall.<br />

American Psychiatric Association (1987), Diagnostic and statistical manual <strong>of</strong> mental<br />

disorders (3rd ed. rev.), Washington D.C.: Author.<br />

Armitage, P. and Berry, G. (1987), Statistical Methods in Medical Research, Oxford:<br />

425


Blackwell.<br />

Arrington, D. (1992), Art-based Assessment Procedures and Instruments used in<br />

Research, in Wadeson, H., (ed.), A Guide to Conducting Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Research,<br />

Mundelein, Ill: Am. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Ass., pp.157-178.<br />

Banker, S.G. (1998), <strong>The</strong> Power <strong>of</strong> Art and Story: women therapists create their own<br />

fairy tales, in Robbins, A. (ed.) <strong>The</strong>rapeutic presence: Bridging expression and form,<br />

London: Kingsley.<br />

Barker-Bausell, R. (1986), A Practical Guide to Conducting Empirical Research, New<br />

York: Harper Row.<br />

B<strong>art</strong>al, L., and Ne'eman, N. (1993), <strong>The</strong> Metaphoric Body: Guide to expressive therapy<br />

through images and archetypes, London: Kingsley.<br />

Baynes, H.G. (1940), Mythology <strong>of</strong> the Soul, London: Tindall Cox.<br />

Borrowsky Junge, M., Pateracki Asawa, P. (1994), A History <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapy in the<br />

United States, Illinois, Mundelein: Am. Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Assn.<br />

Cox, M. (1992) Children's Drawings, Harmondsworth, Penguin.<br />

Dalley, T. and Case, C. (1992), Handbook <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, London: Routledge.<br />

Cunningham-Dax, E. (1953), Experimental Studies in Psychiatric Art, London: Faber<br />

Faber.<br />

Dalley T. And Gilroy, A. (eds.) (1989), Pictures at an Exhibition London: Routledge.<br />

Dalley, T. (1984), Art as <strong>The</strong>rapy London: Tavistock.<br />

Dalley, T., Case, C., Schaverien, J., Weir, F., Halliday, D., Nowell-Hall, P., Waller, D.,<br />

(eds.) (1987), Images <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, London: Tavistock.<br />

Dondis, D.A. (1973), A Primer <strong>of</strong> Visual Literacy, Cambridge: MIT Press.<br />

Dunn, G. (1989), Design and Analysis <strong>of</strong> Reliability Studies: the statistical evaluation <strong>of</strong><br />

measurement errors, London: Arnold.<br />

Edwards, D. (1987), Evaluation in Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, in Milne, D. (ed.), Evaluation in Mental<br />

Health Practice, Beckenham: Croom Helm, pp. 53-69.<br />

Ellenberger, H.F. (1970), <strong>The</strong> Discovery <strong>of</strong> the Unconscious - <strong>The</strong> History and Evolution<br />

<strong>of</strong> Dynamic Psychiatry, Harmondsworth: Penguin.<br />

Eysenck, H. (1961), <strong>The</strong> Effects <strong>of</strong> Psychotherapy in H.J. Eysenck, (ed.), Handbook <strong>of</strong><br />

Abnormal Psychology, London: Basic Books, pp.697-725.<br />

Farrell, B.A. (1955), <strong>The</strong> Standing <strong>of</strong> Psychoanalysis, Oxford reprint 1981: Oxford<br />

University Press.<br />

Fink, B. (1995), <strong>The</strong> Lacanian Subject, Oxford: Princeton University Press.<br />

Fuller, P. (1980) Art and Psychoanalysis, London: Writers Readers.<br />

Gantt, L. (1990), A Validity <strong>Study</strong> <strong>of</strong> the Formal Elements Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Scale (FEATS)<br />

for diagnostic information in patients' drawings, Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation,<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Pittsburgh, Pensylvania.<br />

Gibson, J.J. (1980), forward in Hagen, M.A. (ed.), <strong>The</strong> Perception <strong>of</strong> Pictures, V.1, New<br />

York: Academic Press.<br />

Gilroy, A. (1992), Research in Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, in Waller, D. and Gilroy A. (eds.), Art<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy, A Handbook, Bristol, pp.229-247.<br />

426


Gilroy, A. and Dalley, T. (1989) Pictures at an exhibition, London: Routledge.<br />

Glass, G.V., McGraw, B., Smith, M.L. (1981) Meta Analysis in Social Research,<br />

Beverley Hills, CA: Sage.<br />

Gombrich, E. (1960) Art and Illusion: a study in the psychology <strong>of</strong> pictorial<br />

representation, 5th ed. 1960, Princeton, NJ: Phaidon.<br />

Goodenough, F.L., and Harris, D.B. (1963), <strong>The</strong> Goodenough-Harris Drawing Test, New<br />

York: Harcourt Brace and <strong>World</strong>.<br />

Hagen, M.A. (ed.), <strong>The</strong> Perception <strong>of</strong> Pictures, V.1, New York: Academic Press.<br />

Hammer, E.F. (ed.) Clinical Applications <strong>of</strong> Projective Drawing, Springfield: Thomas.<br />

Harris, D. (1963), Children's Drawings as Measures <strong>of</strong> Intellectual Maturity, New York:,<br />

Harcourt Brace and <strong>World</strong>.<br />

Hill, A.(1945), Art Versus Illness, London: Allen Unwin.<br />

Hill, A.(1951), Painting out Illness, London: Allen Unwin.<br />

Hogg, J. Some Psychological <strong>The</strong>ories and the Visual Arts, in Hogg, J., (ed.), Psychology<br />

and the Visual Arts (Harmondsworth 1969) Penguin pp.78-81.<br />

Hopkins, J. (1992), Psychoanalysis, interpretation and science in Hopkins, J. and Saville,<br />

A. (eds) Psychoanalysis, Mind and Art: perspectives on Richard Wollheim, Oxford:<br />

Blackwell.<br />

Hunter, J.E., F.L. Schmidt and G.B. Jackson (1982), Meta Analysis: cumulating research<br />

findings across studies, Beverley Hills, CA: Sage.<br />

Jaspers, K. (1963), General Psychopathology, Manchester, 7th Ed. trans: Manchester<br />

University Press.<br />

Jung, C.G., and Wilhelm, R. (1931) <strong>The</strong> Secret <strong>of</strong> the Golden Flower, London:<br />

Macmillan.<br />

Kris, E. (1964), Psychoanalytic Explorations in Art, New York: International University<br />

Press.<br />

Kusbit, D. (1993), Signs <strong>of</strong> Psyche in Modern and Postmodern Art, Cambridge<br />

University Press.<br />

Lacan, J. (1988), <strong>The</strong> topic <strong>of</strong> the imaginary, in J. A. Miller (ed.) <strong>The</strong> Seminar <strong>of</strong> Jacques<br />

Lacan, Book 1 (trans. Forrester), Cambridge University Press.<br />

Leger, F.L. (1998), Beyond the therapeutic relationship: behavioural, biological and<br />

cognitive foundations <strong>of</strong> psychotherapy, New York: Haworth Press.<br />

Liebmann, M. (1990), Art in Practice, London: Tavistock.<br />

Levens, M. (1989), Working with defence mechanisms in <strong>art</strong> therapy, in Gilroy, A. and<br />

Dalley, T., Pictures at an Exhibition, London: Routledge, pp.143-6.<br />

Little, M.I. (1997), Miss Alice M. and Her Dragon, New York: Binghampton.<br />

Lombroso, C. (1891), Man <strong>of</strong> Genius, London: Scott.<br />

Lusebrink, V. (1990), Imagery and Visual Expression in <strong>The</strong>rapy New York: Plenum.<br />

Luzzatto, P. (1989) Drinking problems and short-term <strong>art</strong> therapy: working with images<br />

<strong>of</strong> withdrawal and clinging in Gilroy, A. and Dalley, T. (eds.) Pictures at an exhibition,<br />

London: Routledge, pp.207-219.<br />

Machover, K. (1949), Personality Projection in the Drawing <strong>of</strong> the Human Figure,<br />

Springfield Ill., reprint 1978 10th ed: Charles C. Thomas.<br />

427


MacGregor, J.D. (1989), <strong>The</strong> Discovery <strong>of</strong> the Art <strong>of</strong> the Insane, New Jersey and<br />

Oxford: Princetown University Press.<br />

McClelland, S. (1992), Brief Art <strong>The</strong>rapy in Acute States: a process oriented approach<br />

in Waller, D. and Gilroy, A. Handbook <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, London: Routledge, pp.189-207.<br />

Milner, M. (1969), In the Hands <strong>of</strong> the Living God, London: Virago.<br />

Miller, J.A. (ed.) <strong>The</strong> Seminar <strong>of</strong> Jacques Lacan, Book 1 (trans. Forrester), Cambridge<br />

University Press.<br />

Milne, D. (ed.), Evaluation in Mental Health Practice, Beckenham: Croom Helm.<br />

Moon, B. (1992), Essentials <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapy Training and Practice, Springfield, Ill:<br />

Charles C. Thomas.<br />

Moore, R. (1981), Art <strong>The</strong>rapy in Mental Health, Rockville, MD: NIMH.<br />

Naumberg, M. (1947), Studies <strong>of</strong> the free <strong>art</strong> expression <strong>of</strong> behaviour disturbed children<br />

as a means <strong>of</strong> diagnosis and therapy, New York: J. Nervous and Mental Disease<br />

Monographs, Cooleridge.<br />

Naumberg, M. (1950), Schizophrenic Art, Its Meaning in Psychotherapy, New York:<br />

Grune Stratton.<br />

Naumberg, M. (1950), Art <strong>The</strong>rapy: Its Scope and Function in Hammer, E.F. (ed.)<br />

Clinical Applications <strong>of</strong> Projective Drawing, Springfield: Thomas.<br />

Naumberg, M. (1966) Dynamically Orientated Art <strong>The</strong>rapy: Its Principles and Practices,<br />

New York: Grune Stratton.<br />

Oster, G.D., and Gould, P. (1987), Using Drawings in Assessment and <strong>The</strong>rapy: A guide<br />

for mental health pr<strong>of</strong>essionals, New York: Brunner Mazel.<br />

Parsons, M.J. (1987), How we understand <strong>art</strong>. A cognitive developmental area <strong>of</strong><br />

aesthetic experience, 2nd ed. 1989: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Payne, H., (ed.), (1993), Handbook <strong>of</strong> Inquiries into the Art <strong>The</strong>rapies: One river, many<br />

currents, London and Bristol, PA: Kingsley.<br />

Pickford, R. (1967) Studies in Psychiatric Art, London: Tavistock.<br />

Plokker, J.H. (1964), Artistic Self Expression in Mental Disease, London: Littlebrown.<br />

Prinzhorn, H. (1922), Artistry <strong>of</strong> the Mentally Ill, Berlin: Springer Verlag.<br />

Pollock, G. (1988), Women and Sign: psychoanalytic readings, in Pollock, G., Vision and<br />

Difference, London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.<br />

Pollock, G. and Ross, J.M. (eds), (1988) <strong>The</strong> Oedipus Papers, Conn: Madison.<br />

Reber, A.S. (1985), Dictionary <strong>of</strong> Psychology, Harmondsworth: Penguin.<br />

Reitman, F. (1950), Psychotic Art, London: Routledge Kegan Paul.<br />

Robbins, A., (ed.) (1998) <strong>The</strong>rapeutic Presence: Bridging Expression and Form, London:<br />

J. Kingsley.<br />

Rosenthal, R. (1984), Meta Analytic Pocedures for Social Research, Beverley Hills, CA:<br />

Sage, p.9-10.<br />

Rosenthal, R. (1998) Meta analysis: concepts, corollaries and controversies, in J.G. Adair<br />

et al. (eds.) Advances in Psychological Science, Hove: psychological press. V.1:371-384.<br />

Ross, R. (1963) Symbol Systems and Civilisation, New York: Harcourt Brace and<br />

Johanovich.<br />

Schafer, R. (1958) Regression in the service <strong>of</strong> the ego: the relevance <strong>of</strong> a psychoanalytic<br />

428


concept for personality assessment in G. Lindzey (ed.) Assessment <strong>of</strong> Human Motives,<br />

London: Grove.<br />

Schaverien, J. (1992), <strong>The</strong> Revealing Image: analytical <strong>art</strong> psychotherapy in theory and<br />

practice, London: Routledge.<br />

Schilder, P. (1942), Mind: perception and thought in their constructive aspects, reprint<br />

1981, Oxford University Press.<br />

Schneider-Adams, L. (1993) Art and Psychoanalysis, New York: Harper Collins.<br />

Sims, A. (1988), Symtoms in the mind: an introduction to descriptive <strong>psychopathology</strong>,<br />

London: Tindall.<br />

Strupp, H. (1973), Psychotherapy: Clinical, Research and <strong>The</strong>oretical Issues, New York:.<br />

Taylor Fitzgibbon, C. and Lyons Morris, L. (1987), How to Analyse Data, Beverley<br />

Hills, CA: Sage.<br />

Thomas, G.V., Silk, A. (1990), An Introduction to the Psychology <strong>of</strong> Children's<br />

Drawings, Herts: Harvester Wheatsheaf.<br />

Thorburn, J.M. (1925), Art as the relation <strong>of</strong> outer and inner, in Thorburn, J.M., Art and<br />

the Unconscious: a psychological approach to a problem <strong>of</strong> Philosophy, London:Kegan<br />

Paul, pp.151-6.<br />

Thorburn, J.M. (1925), Is Art Symbolic, in Thorburn, J.M., Art and the Unconscious:<br />

a psychological approach to a problem <strong>of</strong> Philosophy, London: Kegan Paul, pp. 73-9.<br />

Wadeson, H. (1980), Art Psychotherapy, New York: Wiley.<br />

Wadeson, H. (1987), <strong>The</strong> Dynamics <strong>of</strong> Art Psychotherapy, New York: Wiley.<br />

Wadeson, H. (1992), A guide to conducting <strong>art</strong> therapy research, Mundelein, Ill: Am. Art<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy Assn.<br />

West, R. (1991), Computing for Psychologists, London: Harwood.<br />

Winnicott, D.W. (1971), Playing and Reality, London: Tavistock.<br />

Waller, D., and Dalley, T. (1992), Art <strong>The</strong>rapy: A theoretical perspective, in Waller, D.<br />

and Gilroy, A., Art <strong>The</strong>rapy: A Handbook, Bristol: Open University Press.<br />

Waller, D. and Gilroy, A. (1992), Art <strong>The</strong>rapy: A Handbook, Bristol: Open University<br />

Press.<br />

Waller, D. (1991), Becoming a Pr<strong>of</strong>ession: the history <strong>of</strong> <strong>art</strong> therapy 1940-1982, London:<br />

Routledge.<br />

Waller, D. (1992), <strong>The</strong> Training <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapists in Waller, D. and Gilroy, A., Art<br />

<strong>The</strong>rapy: A Handbook, Bristol: Open University Press.<br />

Winner, E. (1982) Invented <strong>World</strong>s: the psychology <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong>s Cambridge, Mass:<br />

Harvard University Press.<br />

Woddis, J. (1992) Art therapy: New problems, new solutions, in D. Waller and A. Gilroy<br />

(eds.) Art <strong>The</strong>rapy: a handbook, Bristol: Open University Press.<br />

Wolf, F.M. (1986), Meta Analysis: <strong>quantitative</strong> methods for research synthesis,<br />

Beverley Hills, CA: Sage.<br />

Wollheim, R. (1964), Art and Its Objects, Cambridge reprint 1980: Writers Readers.<br />

<strong>World</strong> Health Organisation, Division <strong>of</strong> Mental Health <strong>of</strong> the (1993), International<br />

Criteria for Diagnoses in the Mental and Behavioral Disorders: Diagnostic Criteria for<br />

429


Research (ICD-10 DCR), Geneva: Author.<br />

Secondary Sources<br />

From Amos, S. (1982). <strong>The</strong> Diagnostic, Prognostic, and <strong>The</strong>rapeutic Implications <strong>of</strong><br />

Schizophrenic Art, Art Psychotherapy, V.9: 131-143.<br />

Pasto, T. (1968) <strong>The</strong> Biomythology <strong>of</strong> Colour: a theory, in Jakob, I., (ed.), Psychiatry<br />

and Art: <strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> interpretation and <strong>art</strong> therapy, V.2, New York: Karger.<br />

Billig, O., Burton Bradley, B.G., (1968) <strong>The</strong> Painted Message, New York: Wiley.<br />

Rapaport, D., Gill, M., Schafer, R. (1946), Diagnostic Psychological Testing Chicago:<br />

Year Book Publishers.<br />

From Dalley, T. and Case, C. Handbook <strong>of</strong> Art <strong>The</strong>rapy, London: Routledge.<br />

Meares, A. (1958) <strong>The</strong> Door <strong>of</strong> Serenity.<br />

Ehrensweig, A. (1967), <strong>The</strong> Hidden Order <strong>of</strong> Art, London: Paladin.<br />

Fuller, P. (1980) Art and Psychoanalysis, London: Writers Readers.<br />

From Goodenough, F.L., and Harris, D.B. (1963), <strong>The</strong> Goodenough-Harris Drawing<br />

Test, New York: Harcourt Brace and <strong>World</strong>.<br />

Goodenough, F. (1926), Measurement <strong>of</strong> Intelligence by Drawings, New York: Harcourt<br />

Brace and <strong>World</strong>.<br />

From Hogg, J. (1970) Some Psychological <strong>The</strong>ories and the Visual Arts, in Hogg, J.,<br />

(ed.), Psychology and the Visual Arts, Harmondsworth: Penguin pp.78-81.<br />

Nagel, E. (1959), Methodological issues in psychoanalytic theory in S. Hook, (ed.),<br />

Psychoanalysis: Scientific Method and Philosophy: A Symposium, Grove Press pp.38-<br />

56.<br />

Nagel, E. (1952), Wholes, Sums and Organic Unities, in Lerner, D., (ed.), P<strong>art</strong>s and<br />

Wholes: <strong>The</strong> Hayden Colloquium on Scientific Method and Concept, London:<br />

Macmillan.<br />

Petermann, B. (1932), <strong>The</strong> Gestalt <strong>The</strong>ory and the Problem <strong>of</strong> Configuration, London:<br />

Kegan Paul.<br />

From MacGregor, J.M. (1989) <strong>The</strong> discovery <strong>of</strong> the <strong>art</strong> <strong>of</strong> the insane, New York:<br />

Princetown University Press.<br />

Kraepelin, E. (1883) Lehrbuch der Psychiatrie.<br />

Jaspers, K. (1932) Strindberg und Van Gogh.<br />

From Schneider Adams, L. (1993) Art and Psychoanalysis, New York: Harper Collins.<br />

Freud, S. (1910), Leonardo Da Vinci - a memory <strong>of</strong> his childhood, V.XIV,<br />

Harmondsworth: Penguin.<br />

430


Freud, S. (1914) <strong>The</strong> Moses <strong>of</strong> Michelangelo, Harmondsworth: Penguin.<br />

From Wolf, F.M. (1986), Meta Analysis: <strong>quantitative</strong> methods for research synthesis,<br />

Beverley Hills, CA: Sage.<br />

Cohen, J. (1977) Statistical Power Analyses for the Behavioural Sciences, New York:<br />

Ac.Press.<br />

L.V. Hedges and I. Olkin (1985) Statistical methods for meta analysis, New York: Ac.<br />

Press<br />

431

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!