29.08.2013 Views

Connectionist Modeling of Experience-based Effects in Sentence ...

Connectionist Modeling of Experience-based Effects in Sentence ...

Connectionist Modeling of Experience-based Effects in Sentence ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Chapter 2 Issues <strong>in</strong> Relative Clause Process<strong>in</strong>g<br />

140 MARCEL ADAM JUST AND PATRICIA A. CARPENTER<br />

fc<br />

o<br />

H<br />

03<br />

Subject Relative<br />

I I I I<br />

[The] reporter senator admitted the<br />

that error,<br />

attacked<br />

the<br />

SIMULATION<br />

Object Relative<br />

a- B<br />

I I I<br />

[The] reporter attacked admitted the<br />

that error,<br />

the<br />

senator<br />

Figure 2.4: CC-READER simulation HUMAN results DATA on English subject and object extracted<br />

relative clauses (figure from Just and Carpenter, 1992; p. 140).<br />

Subject Relative<br />

Object Relative<br />

900<br />

Lowa<br />

ever, <strong>in</strong> Mandar<strong>in</strong> both assumptions would most likely result <strong>in</strong> the same predictions.<br />

Assum<strong>in</strong>g800 possible elided subjects <strong>in</strong> a ma<strong>in</strong> clause, both a ma<strong>in</strong> P'' clause and an embedded<br />

Low<br />

RC could be expected <strong>in</strong> either construction. Thus the mere frequency High <strong>of</strong> subject versus<br />

OH 700<br />

object Ed extractions could <strong>in</strong>deed be decisive <strong>in</strong> this case, predict<strong>in</strong>g a subject preference.<br />

On the other 600 hand, an overt subject gap <strong>in</strong> the SRC as assumed <strong>in</strong> the gap assumption<br />

would olower<br />

the cost at the relativizer for the SRC. Due to the ma<strong>in</strong> clause ambiguity<br />

2<br />

<strong>in</strong> the R ORC 500 a costly Highupdate<br />

<strong>of</strong> the plausibility rank<strong>in</strong>g would happen at the relativizer<br />

where, ORC becomes more 1 j_ likely i than I a ma<strong>in</strong> clause. This cost is lower I I I <strong>in</strong> the subject<br />

extraction[The] duereporter to thesenator higheradmitted rank<strong>in</strong>gthe <strong>of</strong> an embedded [The] reporter SRC. attacked Consequently, admitted theunder<br />

the Gap<br />

Assumption W athat syntactic expectationerror, theory wouldthat also predict a subject error. preference.<br />

S<br />

attacked<br />

the<br />

2.3.3 Canonicity<br />

the<br />

senator<br />

Figure 9. The number <strong>of</strong> cycles expended on various parts <strong>of</strong> the subject-relative sentences (on the left)<br />

and object-relative sentences (on the right) when the simulation, CC READER, is operat<strong>in</strong>g with more or<br />

less work<strong>in</strong>g memory capacity. (The bottom graph presents the human data for comparison with the<br />

simulation.)<br />

Consider<strong>in</strong>g Greenberg’s classification as a basis, what would a canonical word order account<br />

predict for English and German relative clauses? English subject relative clauses<br />

exhibit the canonical SVO structure whereas object relatives use an OSV order<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Therefore, a heuristic or base-generative canonicity theory would assign a higher pro-<br />

Pragmatic Influence on Syntactic Process<strong>in</strong>g<br />

cess<strong>in</strong>g cost to ORCs <strong>in</strong> English, which is consistent with empirical evidence. The same<br />

applies for German, agree<strong>in</strong>g on the widely accepted SOV classification. As illustrated <strong>in</strong><br />

example (13) German SRCs have SOV order<strong>in</strong>g and would be preferred, whereas ORCs<br />

have an OSV order<strong>in</strong>g. If we considered an SVO basis, no clear predictions would be<br />

low span simulation, which has a smaller activation maximum.<br />

The words that follow the verbs evoke fewer productions, so<br />

even though the activation maximum applies dur<strong>in</strong>g their fir<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

they complete their execution <strong>in</strong> a smaller number <strong>of</strong> cycles<br />

(compared with the verb process<strong>in</strong>g), and the high-low difference<br />

becomes smaller.<br />

In summary possible. a simulation that varies the amount <strong>of</strong> activation<br />

available for simultaneously comput<strong>in</strong>g and mn<strong>in</strong>tatMng<br />

<strong>in</strong>formation accounts for the read<strong>in</strong>g time differences between<br />

high and low span subjects deal<strong>in</strong>g with syntactic complexity<br />

provided by center-embedded clauses.<br />

The simulation demonstrates how the contribution <strong>of</strong> a pragmatic<br />

cue to syntactic aaatyas depends on an adequate supply<br />

<strong>of</strong> activation. Fust consider the simaJation <strong>of</strong> the high span<br />

subjects (<strong>in</strong> which the activation maximum is relatively high) <strong>in</strong><br />

process<strong>in</strong>g the sentences conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g reduced relative clauses.<br />

The <strong>in</strong>animacy <strong>in</strong>formation encoded with the subject noun<br />

"evidence") is still <strong>in</strong> an activated state whea the verb is<br />

be<strong>in</strong>g processed, and this <strong>in</strong>formation is used to select between<br />

the two <strong>in</strong>terpretations <strong>of</strong> the verb (past tense vs. past partici-<br />

28

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!