30.10.2012 Views

Proceedings of the Seventh Mountain Lion Workshop

Proceedings of the Seventh Mountain Lion Workshop

Proceedings of the Seventh Mountain Lion Workshop

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

90 REDUCING PUMA ATTACKS · Fitzhugh et al.<br />

attack humans. Beier believed he had<br />

discovered all fatal attacks since 1890 that<br />

met his criteria, and all non-fatal attacks<br />

since 1970. Based on <strong>the</strong> ratio <strong>of</strong> fatal to<br />

non-fatal attacks during 1970-1990, he<br />

estimated that he had failed to identify about<br />

12 non-fatal attacks between 1890 and 1970.<br />

Beier documented an increase in frequency<br />

<strong>of</strong> attacks from <strong>the</strong> 1890-1969 period to<br />

1970-1990. While Beier did not tabulate<br />

“near-attacks,” he did analyze <strong>the</strong> victim’s<br />

actions that may have served to prevent <strong>the</strong><br />

attack. “Fighting back” and shouting loudly<br />

were actions that seemed to avert or repel<br />

attacks, as did waving arms, poking or<br />

hitting with sticks, throwing rocks, etc.<br />

Beier also reported an attempt at aversive<br />

conditioning <strong>of</strong> one puma, but it failed to<br />

prevent future aggression. We substantiated<br />

most <strong>of</strong> Beier’s findings and, because we<br />

have more data, we produced some<br />

additional tentative findings.<br />

We have accounts <strong>of</strong> a total <strong>of</strong> 224<br />

attacks by pumas on humans and 155<br />

behavioral interactions that did not result in<br />

an actual attack. The number <strong>of</strong> accounts<br />

through April 2003 that have information<br />

useful for analyzing any specific question is<br />

variable, but only 108 accounts meet Beier’s<br />

(1991) criteria. Of <strong>the</strong> 116 attacks that failed<br />

Beier’s criteria, 32 were fatal and 84 were<br />

non-fatal. Reasons for failure include lack <strong>of</strong><br />

physical contact, lack <strong>of</strong> verification,<br />

occurrence in Latin America, occurrence<br />

prior to 1890, or because <strong>the</strong>y were attacks<br />

on hunters. Beier’s strict criteria avoid<br />

errors <strong>of</strong> commission, but allow for many<br />

omissions. Because we are interested in<br />

behavior more than just counting attacks, we<br />

decided to relax <strong>the</strong> Beier limitations as long<br />

as accounts seemed plausible and contained<br />

useful information. Our intent is to analyze<br />

human and puma behavior in <strong>the</strong>se<br />

situations where data are available and<br />

compare attacks with encounters to try to<br />

provide better advice for people who come<br />

PROCEEDINGS OF THE SEVENTH MOUNTAIN LION WORKSHOP<br />

face-to-face with a puma, or who want to<br />

prepare for that eventuality. We are still<br />

organizing <strong>the</strong> data, so this report is not <strong>the</strong><br />

final one, but we do have a few<br />

recommendations to make at this time, and<br />

we will make some observations about<br />

reliability <strong>of</strong> reports and frequency <strong>of</strong><br />

attacks in general.<br />

METHODS<br />

We defined “non-attack encounters” as<br />

behavioral interactions between pumas and<br />

humans at close proximity that do not result<br />

in an attack. We purposely did not define<br />

“close proximity” to place emphasis on<br />

“behavioral interactions.” We excluded<br />

incidents in which <strong>the</strong> puma was sighted and<br />

<strong>the</strong>n left, and included incidents in which <strong>the</strong><br />

puma and human exchanged multiple<br />

behaviors. “Close proximity” is necessary<br />

for this to occur, but <strong>the</strong> distance may vary,<br />

and we were more flexible regarding<br />

distance criteria than for behavioral criteria.<br />

If we believed we could learn from <strong>the</strong><br />

interaction, we included it. Most <strong>of</strong> our data<br />

are from published popular accounts,<br />

sometimes substantiated by an agency<br />

incident report. Etling (2001), in particular,<br />

solicited personal accounts from individuals<br />

in <strong>the</strong>ir own words, both before and after<br />

publication <strong>of</strong> her book. We categorized<br />

incidents in several ways to better analyze<br />

and evaluate <strong>the</strong> data. One mentioned in<br />

this paper is a category we called “attacks<br />

terminated by humans.” These are incidents<br />

in which a puma was shot while charging, or<br />

at least clearly intent on creeping up very<br />

close to a human in spite <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> person’s<br />

efforts to discourage such behavior. We<br />

have 20 such accounts, 10 <strong>of</strong> which are from<br />

hunters. In only 3 <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> hunting accounts<br />

was it clear that <strong>the</strong> hunter was doing<br />

something that might attract a puma (e.g.,<br />

using deer scent, calling turkeys, etc.). Six<br />

accounts were <strong>of</strong> agency employees<br />

investigating previous encounters between<br />

humans and pumas.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!