01.04.2015 Views

The Questions of Developmental Biology

The Questions of Developmental Biology

The Questions of Developmental Biology

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Sex Determination and Behaviors<br />

Organization/Activation Hypothesis<br />

Does prenatal (or neonatal) exposure to particular steroid hormones impose permanent<br />

sex-specific changes on the central nervous system? Such sex-specific neural changes have been<br />

shown in regions <strong>of</strong> the brain that regulate "involuntary" sexual physiology. <strong>The</strong> cyclic secretion<br />

<strong>of</strong> luteinizing hormone by the adult female rat pituitary, for example, is dependent on the lack <strong>of</strong><br />

testosterone during the first week <strong>of</strong> the animal's life. <strong>The</strong> luteinizing hormone secretion <strong>of</strong><br />

female rats can be made noncyclic by giving them testosterone 4 days after birth; conversely, the<br />

luteinizing hormone secretion <strong>of</strong> males can be made cyclical by removing their testes within a<br />

day <strong>of</strong> birth (Barraclough and Gorski 1962). It is thought that sex hormones may act during the<br />

fetal or neonatal stage <strong>of</strong> a mammal's life to organize the nervous system in a sex-specific<br />

manner, and that during adult life, the same hormones may have transitory, activational effects.<br />

This idea is called the organization/activation hypothesis.<br />

Interestingly, the hormone chiefly responsible for determining the male brain pattern is<br />

estradiol, a type <strong>of</strong> estrogen.* Testosterone in fetal or neonatal blood can be converted into<br />

estradiol by the enzyme P450 aromatase, and this conversion occurs in the hypothalamus and<br />

limbic system two areas <strong>of</strong> the brain known to regulate hormone secretion and reproductive<br />

behavior (Reddy et al. 1974; McEwen et al. 1977). Thus, testosterone exerts its effects on the<br />

nervous system by being converted into estradiol. But the fetal environment is rich in estrogens<br />

from the gonads and placenta. What stops these estrogens from masculinizing the nervous system<br />

<strong>of</strong> a female fetus? Fetal estrogen (in both males and females) is bound by a-fetoprotein.<br />

This protein is made in the fetal liver and becomes a major component <strong>of</strong> the fetal blood and<br />

cerebrospinal fluid. It will bind and inactivate estrogen, but not testosterone.<br />

Attempts to extend the organization/activation hypothesis to "voluntary" sexual behaviors<br />

are more controversial because there is no truly sex-specific behavior that distinguishes the two<br />

sexes <strong>of</strong> many mammals, and because hormonal treatment has multiple effects on the developing<br />

mammal. For instance, injecting testosterone into a week-old female rat will increase pelvic<br />

thrusting behavior and diminish lordosis a posture that stimulates mounting behavior in the<br />

male when she reaches adulthood (Phoenix et al. 1959; Kandel et al. 1995). <strong>The</strong>se behavioral<br />

changes can be ascribed to testosterone-mediated changes in the central nervous system, but they<br />

could also be due to hormonal effects on other tissues. Testosterone enables the growth <strong>of</strong> the<br />

muscles that allow pelvic thrusting. And since testosterone causes females to grow larger and to<br />

close their vaginal orifices, one cannot conclude that the lack <strong>of</strong> lordosis is due solely to<br />

testosterone-mediated changes in the neural circuitry (Harris and Levine 1965; De Jonge et al.<br />

1988; Moore 1990; Moore et al. 1992; Fausto-Sterling 1995).<br />

In addition, the effects <strong>of</strong> sex steroids on the brain are very complicated, and the steroids<br />

may be metabolized differently in different regions <strong>of</strong> the brain. Male mice lacking the<br />

testosterone receptor still retain a male-specific preoptic morphology in the brain, and male mice<br />

lacking the aromatase enzyme are capable <strong>of</strong> breeding (Breedlove 1992; Fisher et al. 1998).<br />

<strong>The</strong>se studies show that there is more to sex-specific morphology and behavior than steroid<br />

hormones. Despite best-selling books that pretend to know the answers, we have much more to<br />

learn regarding the relationship between development, steroids, and behavior. Moreover,<br />

extrapolating from rats to humans is a very risky business, as no sex-specific behavior has yet<br />

been identified in humans, and what is "masculine" in one culture may be considered "feminine"

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!