09.12.2012 Views

Second edition

Second edition

Second edition

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

01.qxd 3/10/08 9:33 AM Page 12<br />

12 Diagnostic assessment<br />

instructing the patient to bite down. Sensory testing, to<br />

both light touch and pin-prick, is checked in all three divisions,<br />

namely the ophthalmic, maxillary, and mandibular.<br />

The corneal reflex, which tests both the cranial nerves V<br />

and VII, may also be performed by lightly touching a wisp<br />

of cotton to the patient’s cornea, after which there should<br />

be a bilateral blink.<br />

CRANIAL NERVE VII<br />

The facial nerve is first tested for voluntary facial movements<br />

by asking the patient to wrinkle the forehead and<br />

subsequently to show the teeth. In cases of unilateral voluntary<br />

facial paresis note must be made of which divisions<br />

of the facial nerve are involved: the upper (controlling<br />

forehead wrinkling), the lower (controlling elevation of the<br />

side of the mouth), or both. At times facial weakness may<br />

be quite subtle, manifesting perhaps only with a slight flattening<br />

of the nasolabial fold on one side.<br />

After voluntary movements have been tested, the physician<br />

must then test for involuntary or ‘mimetic’ facial<br />

movements. This may be accomplished by telling a joke,<br />

or, if the physician is in less than a humorous mood, by<br />

simply observing the patient for any spontaneous smiling.<br />

Voluntary and involuntary facial movements are quite distinct<br />

neuroanatomically and thus both should be tested for<br />

(Hopf et al. 1992). Voluntary facial palsy affecting only the<br />

lower division indicates a lesion of the pre-central gyrus or<br />

corticobulbar fibers, whereas emotional facial palsy (Section<br />

4.8) indicates a lesion in the supplementary motor area,<br />

temporal lobe, striatum, or pons.<br />

CRANIAL NERVE VIII<br />

The vestibulocochlear nerve is generally tested by gently<br />

rubbing the fingers together about 30 cm from the patient’s<br />

ear and asking whether anything is heard; alternatively,<br />

one may bring a ticking watch in from a distance and ask<br />

the patient to indicate when it is first heard. If there are any<br />

abnormalities, both Weber and Rinne testing should be<br />

performed to determine whether the hearing loss is of the<br />

conduction or sensorineural type.<br />

In the Weber test, a vibrating tuning fork is placed<br />

square on the midline of the patient’s forehead and the<br />

patient is asked whether it sounds the same on both sides<br />

or is heard louder on one side than on the other. In the<br />

Rinne test, a vibrating tuning fork is placed against the styloid<br />

process and the patient is asked to indicate when the<br />

sound vanishes, at which point the tines of the tuning fork<br />

are immediately brought in close approximation to the ear<br />

and the patient is asked whether it can now be heard. With<br />

conductive hearing loss, the Weber lateralizes to the side<br />

with the hearing loss, and on Rinne testing, bone conduction<br />

(i.e., with the tuning fork against the styloid process)<br />

is louder than air conduction (i.e., with the tines of the fork<br />

vibrating in the air just outside the ear). With sensorineuronal<br />

loss, the Weber lateralizes to the ‘good’ side and, on<br />

Rinne testing, air conduction is better than bone conduction<br />

bilaterally.<br />

CRANIAL NERVES IX AND X<br />

The glossopharyngeal and vagus nerves are tested with the<br />

gag reflex and by observation for symmetric elevation of<br />

the palate during phonation.<br />

CRANIAL NERVE XI<br />

The spinal accessory nerve is tested by having patients shrug<br />

their shoulders against the resistance of the physician’s<br />

hand and by turning the head to one side or the other while<br />

the physician exerts contrary pressure on the jaw.<br />

CRANIAL NERVE XII<br />

The hypoglossal nerve is tested first by asking the patient to<br />

open the mouth and then observing the tongue, as it rests<br />

in the oropharynx, for any atrophy or fasciculations. Once<br />

this has been accomplished, the patient is asked to protrude<br />

the tongue as far as possible, noting especially whether it<br />

protrudes past the lips and also whether it deviates to one<br />

side or the other.<br />

Sensory testing<br />

Elementary sensory testing involves light touch, pin-prick,<br />

and vibration. Light touch may be assessed by using a wisp<br />

of cotton, or simply by a light touch with one’s finger, and<br />

pin-prick sensation is tested using a disposable safety pin.<br />

Vibratory sensation is tested by touching a vibrating tuning<br />

fork to a bony structure (such as a finger joint, the lateral<br />

malleolus, or the great toe) and asking the patient<br />

whether he or she can tell if it is vibrating; if so, the tuning<br />

fork is held in place and the patient is asked to say when the<br />

vibration ceases, with the physician taking note, in a rough<br />

sort of way, of how much the tuning fork is still vibrating at<br />

that point. If there are any abnormalities in elementary<br />

sensation it is critical to determine whether or not they<br />

are bilateral. In general, it is sufficient to test sensation<br />

at both hands and both feet, reserving more detailed testing<br />

for cases in which the history suggests a more focal<br />

sensory loss.<br />

Graphesthesia and two-point discrimination tests also<br />

constitute part of the sensory examination but these<br />

should only be used if elementary sensation is intact.<br />

Agraphesthesia is said to be present when patients, with<br />

their eyes closed, are unable to identify letters or numerals<br />

traced on their palms by a pencil or dull pin. Two-point<br />

discrimination may be tested by ‘bending a paperclip to<br />

different distances between its two points . . . [starting]<br />

with the points relatively far apart . . . [then] approximated<br />

until the patient begins to make errors’ (Dejong 1979). As<br />

two-point discriminatory ability varies on different parts

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!