09.12.2012 Views

Second edition

Second edition

Second edition

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

02.qxd 3/10/08 9:32 AM Page 64<br />

64 ‘Cortical’ signs and symptoms<br />

Bedside testing for motor neglect may be accomplished<br />

by asking patients to clap their hands or fasten a button:<br />

when motor neglect is present, the arm on the neglected<br />

side will either not participate in the task or do so only<br />

minimally. Importantly, this is not the result of a lack of<br />

strength or coordination: with strong urging, patients are<br />

generally able to bring the affected arm into play such that<br />

there is more or less full bimanual cooperation in the task<br />

at hand.<br />

Etiology<br />

As indicated earlier, left neglect, due to a lesion in the right<br />

hemisphere, is clinically far more prominent than right<br />

neglect. Although right neglect certainly does occur, it is<br />

less common, less severe, and, in the case of stroke, less<br />

enduring (Beis et al. 2004; Stone et al. 1991).<br />

Although neglect may occur secondary to both cortical<br />

and to subcortical lesions, in the great majority of cases, it<br />

is a lesion of the cortex that is responsible (Vallar and<br />

Perani 1986; Vallar et al. 1994). Cortical lesions capable of<br />

causing neglect may be found in the frontal lobe (primarily<br />

in the posterior portion of the inferior frontal gyrus<br />

[Daffner et al. 1990; Husain and Kennad 1996; Heilman<br />

and Valenstein 1972; Maeshima et al. 1994; Stein and<br />

Volpe 1983]), the parietal lobe (Bender 1945; Cherington<br />

1974; Critchley 1949; Frantz 1950) (primarily in the inferior<br />

parietal lobule [Mort et al. 2003; Vallar and Perani<br />

1986]), and in the temporal lobe (primarily the medial<br />

aspect of the parahippocampal gyrus [Mort et al. 2003]).<br />

Subcortical structures implicated include the caudate<br />

nucleus (Caplan et al. 1990; Kumral et al. 1999), the putamen<br />

(Hier et al. 1977), and the thalamus (Kumral et al.<br />

1995; Motomura et al. 1986; Watson et al. 1981). Rarely,<br />

neglect has been noted with a lesion of the posterior limb<br />

of the internal capsule (Bogousslavsky et al. 1988c; Ferro<br />

and Kertesz 1984) and with a combined lesion of the<br />

medial aspect of the occipital lobe and the splenium<br />

(Chung Park et al. 2005).<br />

Although neglect is most commonly seen as part of a<br />

stroke syndrome, it may also occur with other focal lesions<br />

(such as tumors) and may also be seen as but one sign in a<br />

dementing disorder such as Alzheimer’s disease.<br />

Differential diagnosis<br />

Visual neglect must be distinguished from hemianopia and<br />

constructional apraxia. Hemianopic patients, of course,<br />

will fail to attend to objects in the hemianopic field, given<br />

that they simply do not see them. Confrontational testing<br />

for visual fields, done with sufficient vigor, however,<br />

reveals full fields in patients with neglect. In constructional<br />

apraxia, drawing a clock face or daisy may be very difficult;<br />

however, here one sees a random pattern of errors, rather<br />

than a ‘bunching up’ on the unneglected side.<br />

Motor neglect must be distinguished from hemiparesis,<br />

and this distinction is made, as indicated earlier, on the<br />

basis of the response to vigorous encouragement. Patients<br />

with neglect eventually respond to vigorous direction,<br />

whereas patients with hemiparesis simply cannot.<br />

Treatment<br />

Various rehabilitation techniques are helpful for visual<br />

neglect, including prism adaptation (Frassinetti et al. 2002;<br />

Keane et al. 2006), neck muscle vibration (Schindler et al.<br />

2002), and visual scanning training.<br />

REFERENCES<br />

Achiron A, Ziv I, Djaldetti R et al. Aphasia in multiple sclerosis:<br />

clinical and radiologic correlation. Neurology 1992;<br />

42:2195–7.<br />

Agnetti V, Carreras M, Pinna L et al. Ictal prosopagnosia and<br />

epileptogenic damage of the dominant hemisphere. Cortex<br />

1978; 14:50–7.<br />

Agostoni E, Coletti A, Orlando G et al. Apraxia in deep cerebral<br />

lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1983; 46:804–8.<br />

Aguirre GK, D’Esposito M. Topographical disorientation: a<br />

synthesis and txonomy. Brain 1999;122:1613–28.<br />

Ajax ET, Schenkenberg T, Kosteljanetz M. Alexia without agraphia<br />

and the inferior splenium. Neurology 1977; 27:685–8.<br />

Albert ML. A simple test of visual neglect. Neurology 1973;<br />

23:658–64.<br />

Albert ML, Soffer D, Silverberg R et al. The anatomic basis of<br />

visual agnosia. Neurology 1979; 29:876–9.<br />

Alexander MP, LoVerme SR. Aphasia after left hemispheric<br />

intracerebral hemorrhage. Neurology 1980; 30:1193–202.<br />

Alexander MP, Schmitt MA. The aphasia syndrome of stroke in the<br />

left anterior cerebral artery territory. Arch Neurol 1980;<br />

37:97–100.<br />

Alexander MP, Fischette MR, Fischer RS. Crossed aphasias can be<br />

mirror image anomalous: case reports, review and hypothesis.<br />

Brain 1989; 112:953–73.<br />

Alsaadi T, Binder JR, Lazar RM et al. Pure topographic<br />

disorientation: a distinctive syndrome with varied localization.<br />

Neurology 2000; 54:1864–6.<br />

Anderson B. Pieces of the true crossover effect in neglect.<br />

Neurology 1997; 49:809–12.<br />

Anderson SW, Damasio AR, Damasio H. Troubled letters but not<br />

numbers. Domain specific cognitive impairments following<br />

focal damage in the frontal cortex. Brain 1990; 113:<br />

749–66.<br />

Andre JM, Beis JM, Morin N et al. Buccal hemineglect. Arch<br />

Neurol 2000; 57:1734–41.<br />

Arsland D, Larsen JP, Hoien T. Alexia in dementia of the<br />

Alzheimer’s type. Acta Neurol Scand 1993; 88: 434–9.<br />

Ashtary F, Janghorbani M, Chitsaz A et al. A randomized, doubleblind<br />

trial of bromocriptine efficacy in nonfluent aphasia after<br />

stroke. Neurology 2006; 66: 914–16.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!