09.12.2012 Views

Second edition

Second edition

Second edition

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

02.qxd 3/10/08 9:32 AM Page 54<br />

54 ‘Cortical’ signs and symptoms<br />

left hemisphere, although reported (Darby 1993; Ross et al.<br />

1989), are rare.<br />

MOTOR APROSODIA<br />

In motor aprosodia, speech is monotonous, but comprehension<br />

is intact and there is no ‘mismatch’; repetition<br />

does not relieve the monotonous tone. The presence of<br />

motor aprosodia may be quite disconcerting to patients.<br />

One of Ross and Mesulam’s (1979) patients complained<br />

that she had great difficulty in disciplining her children<br />

because when she said to them that she was angry they<br />

didn’t respond, because she didn’t sound angry. She<br />

eventually came up with a way to circumvent her motor<br />

aprosodia by adding a parenthetical statement, such as<br />

‘God damnit, I mean it’ or ‘I am angry and I mean it,’ afterward,<br />

thereby getting across what she was feeling. Of note,<br />

however, in this case, ‘even the parenthetical statement was<br />

voiced in a complete monotone’.<br />

Motor aprosodia most commonly occurs secondary to<br />

lesions in the posterior frontal operculum (Gorelick and<br />

Ross 1987; Ross 1981; Ross and Mesulam 1979); cases have<br />

also been reported with lesions in the internal capsule<br />

(Ross et al. 1981) and basal ganglia (Speedie et al. 1993).<br />

TRANSCORTICAL MOTOR APROSODIA<br />

Transcortical motor aprosodia is essentially identical to<br />

motor aprosodia except that the monotonous tone of<br />

voice disappears with repetition. A case was reported secondary<br />

to a lesion in the anterior limb of the internal<br />

capsule (Ross 1981).<br />

SENSORY APROSODIA<br />

In sensory aprosodia there is a normal range of intonation<br />

in the patient’s spontaneous speech. Comprehension,<br />

however, is impaired, and one finds mismatches; repetition<br />

is also impaired in that when patients repeat the neutral<br />

phrase, the intonation of their speech will not be the<br />

same as that of the examiner.<br />

Sensory aprosodia has been noted with lesions affecting<br />

the temporoparietal cortex (Darby 1993; Gorelick and<br />

Ross 1987; Ross 1981); a case has also been reported secondary<br />

to a lesion of the thalamus and adjacent posterior<br />

limb of the internal capsule (Wolfe and Ross 1987).<br />

TRANSCORTICAL SENSORY APROSODIA<br />

This aprosodia is similar to sensory aprosodia with the<br />

exception that repetition is performed well, with the<br />

patient accurately mimicking the examiner’s tone of voice.<br />

Cases have been described secondary to a lesion of the<br />

anterior temporal cortex (Ross 1981), and the striatum and<br />

adjacent posterior limb of the internal capsule (Gorelick<br />

and Ross 1987).<br />

GLOBAL APROSODIA<br />

In global aprosodia, speech is monotonous and comprehension<br />

of other’s prosody is poor; repetition does not<br />

improve the patient’s prosodoic output. Global aprosodia<br />

has been noted with lesions affecting both the frontal operculum<br />

and the posterior temporal cortex (Darby 1993;<br />

Ross 1981).<br />

TRANSCORTICAL MIXED APROSODIA<br />

Transcortical mixed aprosodia, which might just as well<br />

be called transcortical global aprosodia, is essentially identical<br />

to global aprosodia with the exception that the patient’s<br />

prosody improves with repetition. A case has been reported<br />

with a lesion involving the frontoparietal cortex (Ross<br />

1981).<br />

CONDUCTION APROSODIA<br />

In conduction aprosodia spontaneous speech shows a normal<br />

range of intonation, and comprehension is intact.<br />

Remarkably, however, there is a ‘mismatch’ between the<br />

patient’s statement as to how he is feeling and the tone with<br />

which that statement is made. Furthermore, and remarkably<br />

so in light of the preserved comprehension of prosody,<br />

patients are unable to repeat sentences with the prosody<br />

spoken by the examiner. Conduction aprosody appears to<br />

be very rare; a case was reported with a lesion involving the<br />

temporoparietal cortex (Gorelick and Ross 1987).<br />

PURE AFFECTIVE DEAFNESS<br />

In this condition, spontaneous speech shows a normal range<br />

of intonation and there is no mismatch present. Despite<br />

these preserved abilities; however, patients are unable to<br />

comprehend the prosody with which others speak.<br />

Remarkably, however, patients are able to mimic the examiner’s<br />

intonation upon repetition testing.<br />

Cases of pure affective deafness have been reported with<br />

lesions affecting the posterior frontal and immediately<br />

subjacent temporal operculum (Gorelick and Ross 1987;<br />

Ross 1981) and also with a large lesion affecting the occipitoparietal<br />

cortex (Gorelick and Ross 1987).<br />

Etiology<br />

Almost all reported cases of aprosodia have occurred as<br />

part of a stroke syndrome, secondary to either ischemic<br />

infarction or, much less commonly, intracerebral hemorrhage.<br />

A case has also been reported of a gradually progressive<br />

motor aprosodia secondary to a progressive focal<br />

atrophy of the right frontal lobe (Ghacibeh and Heilman<br />

2003). Further, there is also a report of motor aprosodia<br />

occurring paroxysmally as a simple partial seizure<br />

(Bautista and Ciampetti 2003).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!