09.12.2012 Views

Second edition

Second edition

Second edition

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

09.qxd 3/10/08 9:39 AM Page 417<br />

with an IQ of 71, although unable to abstract on proverbs<br />

testing or do simple arithmetic, was nevertheless able to ‘correctly<br />

give the day of the week of any day this century’ (Hurst<br />

and Mulhall 1988). In the past, such patients have been<br />

referred to as ‘idiots savants’ (Treffert 1988), and although<br />

this terminology is unfortunate, it does forcefully convey the<br />

sometimes astounding contrast between the ‘islets’ of superior<br />

functioning and the overall intellectual decrement.<br />

Seizures occur in roughly one-third of patients and,<br />

although these are most common in those with mental retardation,<br />

they may also occur in those of normal intelligence.<br />

Infantile spasms may occur in younger children, whereas in<br />

older children and in adults, simple partial, complex<br />

partial, and grand mal seizures may be seen (Danielsson<br />

et al. 2005; Olsson et al. 1988).<br />

Before leaving this discussion of the clinical features of<br />

autism, it is appropriate to comment on the putative entity<br />

known as Asperger’s syndrome. As noted earlier, in some mild<br />

cases of autism, symptoms may not come to light until the<br />

child enters elementary school and, in very mild cases, recognition<br />

may be delayed until much later. Some authors believe<br />

that these cases represent a separate disorder, called Asperger’s<br />

syndrome; myself and others, however, conceive of such<br />

patients as merely constituting the very ‘high-functioning’<br />

end of the spectrum of clinical severity of autism.<br />

Course<br />

The overall outcome is strongly influenced by whether or not<br />

there is an associated mental retardation and, if so, its severity.<br />

With regard to autistic symptoms, there is typically some<br />

gradual improvement by adult years, and, from a prognostic<br />

point of view, the course during middle childhood is particularly<br />

important: those who attain some language and some<br />

social skills around this time have a much more favorable<br />

outcome than those who do not. No matter how great the<br />

spontaneous recovery, however, residual symptoms, such as<br />

aprosodia, social awkwardness, and a reduced awareness of<br />

social conventions, remain in adult years (Rumsey et al.<br />

1985).<br />

Etiology<br />

Etiologic theories regarding autism have changed radically<br />

over the past few decades. This disorder was once believed to<br />

result from faulty child rearing by cold and distant parents.<br />

This theory has now been soundly discredited, and current<br />

research focuses primarily on genetics and neuropathology.<br />

Family studies strongly support a genetic basis. As<br />

noted earlier, the lifetime prevalence is about 0.05 percent;<br />

however, the concordance among monozygotic twins is<br />

over 60 percent (Bailey et al. 1995; Folstein and Rutter<br />

1977). Genetic heterogeneity is almost assured, and linkage<br />

studies have identified loci on various chromosomes,<br />

including chromosomes 2, 3, 7, 11, 15, 19, and X.<br />

9.14 Autism 417<br />

Although MRI studies overall have not been conclusive,<br />

with numerous contradictory results and failures of<br />

replication, two findings appear fairly solid, namely<br />

vermal hypoplasia and a degree of macroencephaly.<br />

Neuropathologic studies (as recently reviewed by Palmen<br />

et al. in 2004), although likewise not conclusive, do suggest<br />

cerebral cortical dysgenesis, abnormalities of neuronal cell<br />

packing and size within the limbic cortex, and a loss of<br />

Purkinje cells within the cerebellum.<br />

It is unclear what role environmental factors play in the<br />

etiology of autism. Recent concerns regarding an etiologic<br />

role of the measles/mumps/rubella vaccine appear<br />

unfounded (Madsen et al. 2002).<br />

Although the etiology of the vast majority of cases of<br />

autism thus remains unclear, it does appear that in a small<br />

minority, perhaps 10 percent, autism occurs secondary to<br />

certain other well-described disorders (Kielinen et al. 2004;<br />

Ritvo et al. 1990), including tuberous sclerosis (Alsen et al.<br />

1994; Lawlor and Maurer 1987), the fragile X syndrome<br />

(Wisniewski et al. 1985c), Rett’s syndrome (Percy et al.<br />

1990), Down’s syndrome (Lund and Munk-Jorgensen<br />

1988), velocardiofacial syndrome (Fine et al. 2005), and<br />

the congenital rubella syndrome (Chess et al. 1978).<br />

Differential diagnosis<br />

As noted, mental retardation is seen in association with<br />

autism in about three-quarters of cases, and hence autism<br />

with mental retardation must be distinguished from<br />

mental retardation of other causes. At times this may be<br />

difficult, given that many patients with mental retardation<br />

of other causes will display repetitive, stereotyped behaviors,<br />

which at times may be similar to the ‘fascinations’ and<br />

stereotypies seen in autism. A cardinal differential feature,<br />

however, is the patient’s social relatedness: upon<br />

approaching a child with mental retardation of other<br />

causes, one may be greeted by a smile and an expectant<br />

posture; by contrast, the child with autism may show no<br />

more interest in the approaching physician than might be<br />

evinced for a machine.<br />

Developmental dysphasia, especially when both expressive<br />

and receptive components are present, may be confused<br />

with autism; however, here again the quality of the<br />

patient’s social relatedness enables a differentiation. The<br />

child with developmental dysphasia, although unable to<br />

communicate verbally, will still, by gesture, tone of voice,<br />

and facial expression, clearly desire social contact, in stark<br />

contrast to the child with autism.<br />

Schizophrenia may enter the differential, especially in<br />

cases characterized by significant inaccessibility and bizarre<br />

stereotypies. Here the age of onset is helpful. As noted<br />

earlier, most cases of autism become obvious by the age of<br />

3 years; by contrast, schizophrenia only very rarely has an<br />

onset before the age of 8 years. Furthermore, in schizophrenia<br />

one finds hallucinations and delusions, symptoms<br />

that are absent in autism.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!