09.12.2012 Views

Second edition

Second edition

Second edition

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

02.qxd 3/10/08 9:32 AM Page 55<br />

Various forms of aprosodia may also be seen in dementia,<br />

as for example Alzheimer’s disease (Perez-Trullen and<br />

Modrego Pardo 1996); however, here associated cognitive<br />

deficits, such as disorientation and short-term memory<br />

loss, will suggest the correct syndromal diagnosis.<br />

Differential diagnosis<br />

Motor aprosodia must be distinguished from flattened<br />

affect and hypophonia. Flattened affect is typically accompanied<br />

by a monotone voice; however, here there is also a<br />

‘flattening’ of the patient’s feelings: patients with flattened<br />

affect typically report having no feelings and this is in<br />

contrast with motor aprosodia wherein patients, although<br />

speaking in a monotone, still have feelings, sometimes<br />

quite strong ones. Hypophonia, as seen in parkinsonism, is<br />

a speech deficit characterized by whispering and low volume,<br />

which stands in contrast with the normal volume<br />

seen in motor aprosodia.<br />

Sensory aprosodia must be distinguished from emotional<br />

incontinence and from inappropriate affect. Emotional<br />

incontinence, as discussed in Section 4.7, is characterized<br />

by episodes of involuntary laughing or crying (accompanied<br />

by, respectively, a mirthful or sad tone of voice) during<br />

which the patient has no feelings, except, perhaps, a<br />

sense of consternation at being unable to control the emotional<br />

display. This, superficially, is similar to the ‘mismatch’<br />

seen in sensory aprosodia; however, on closer<br />

inspection there are differences. First, there is, in fact, no<br />

actual ‘mismatch’ in the sense of a patient feeling one thing<br />

and displaying a discordant tone of voice as he reports that<br />

feeling, for in emotional incontinence patients often are<br />

emotionally neutral during the episode. <strong>Second</strong>, in contrast<br />

with aprosodia, which in almost all cases is constant<br />

and more or less chronic, emotional incontinence occurs<br />

in discrete episodes, in between which there is a congruence<br />

between what the patient feels and the tone with<br />

which that feeling is reported.<br />

Inappropriate affect is very similar to sensory aprosodia,<br />

in that in both these signs there is a mismatch between<br />

what the patient feels and the tone of voice in which that<br />

feeling is expressed. Differentiating between the two<br />

requires attention to comprehension of prosody, which is<br />

present in patients with inappropriate affect, and absent in<br />

those with sensory aprosodia.<br />

Finally, aphasia must be clearly distinguished from<br />

aprosodia. Aphasia represents a disturbance in what is said,<br />

aprosodia a disturbance in how it is said. Consider, for<br />

example, two patients who are both grief-stricken over a<br />

recent loss. The first one, having a motor aphasia (Broca’s<br />

aphasia), although restricted to simply repeating the word<br />

‘sad . . . sad . . . sad’ over and over again, might yet say it so<br />

lugubriously that the listener has no doubt about the depth<br />

of the patient’s grief. By contrast, the second patient, with<br />

a motor aprosodia, although able to say the words ‘I’ve<br />

never felt so sad in my entire life’, would say them in such<br />

2.8 Apraxia 55<br />

a monotone that the listener might well doubt whether the<br />

patient was, in fact, really feeling any sadness.<br />

Treatment<br />

Speech therapy may be helpful in addition to treatment, if<br />

possible, of the underlying condition.<br />

2.8 APRAXIA<br />

Apraxia is said to be present when, despite preserved<br />

strength, sensation, and coordination, patients are unable<br />

to carry out purposeful activities. In the literature, there are<br />

a large number of different kinds of apraxia described;<br />

in this chapter, four of these are considered: ideomotor,<br />

ideational, constructional, and dressing.<br />

Clinical features<br />

Each of the four kinds of apraxia is described below.<br />

IDEOMOTOR AND IDEATIONAL APRAXIA<br />

Ideomotor and ideational apraxia both have to do with the<br />

use of tools, considered in a broad sense, such as combs,<br />

knives and forks, and scissors. Bedside testing is readily<br />

accomplished using either some plastic knives/forks from<br />

the hospital cafeteria or a plastic blunt-ended pair of scissors<br />

carried discreetly in the pocket of one’s white coat.<br />

Begin first by asking the patient to pantomime the use of a<br />

knife and fork, or perhaps a pair of scissors, and observe<br />

the performance. In some cases, the evidence for apraxia is<br />

obvious, as patients appear perplexed and are unable to<br />

make any appropriate movements. In other cases, there<br />

may be some doubt, as for example when patients use a<br />

body part as the tool itself. An example of this might be<br />

when, in attempting to pantomime using scissors to cut an<br />

imaginary piece of paper, the patient moves the index and<br />

middle fingers as if they were the blades of the scissors,<br />

rather than making a repetitive, squeezing kind of motion<br />

with the hand, as one does when the scissors are actually<br />

present. Some authorities consider this an example of<br />

apraxia; however, in this author’s experience normal individuals<br />

are as likely to use their fingers as scissors as not. If<br />

the patient does display significant difficulty in mimicking<br />

the use of a tool, the next step is to provide the tool and ask<br />

the patient to use it; in this case, one pulls out the scissors<br />

and offers them, along with a piece of paper, and observes<br />

the response. In some cases, patients are able to pick up the<br />

tool and use it with little or no difficulty. In others, however,<br />

the perplexity persists: patients may pick up the scissors<br />

by the wrong end, turn them upside down, or<br />

otherwise hold them in a useless position. Cases wherein<br />

patients are unable to pantomime use of a tool but then go

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!