09.12.2012 Views

Second edition

Second edition

Second edition

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

07.qxd 3/10/08 9:35 AM Page 256<br />

256 Other major syndromes<br />

2007; Smaje et al. 1987): in such cases, if seizures cannot be<br />

controlled, placement of a pacemaker may be required.<br />

PETIT MAL SEIZURES<br />

Petit mal seizures (Delgado-Escueta 1979; Penry et al. 1975;<br />

Sadleir et al. 2006), also known as absence seizures, are<br />

abrupt in onset and occur without an aura; they are very<br />

brief, lasting of the order of 10 seconds, and generally consist<br />

of an arrest of all activity accompanied by a blank stare:<br />

the seizure ends as abruptly as it began, and there is no postictal<br />

confusion or drowsiness. To the observer, it may<br />

appear that the patient had a ‘blank’ spell or was merely<br />

momentarily ‘out of it’ then ‘snapped to’. In some cases,<br />

there may be some myoclonic fluttering of the eyelids and<br />

occasionally some myoclonic jerks of the hands. Some<br />

patients will also experience a partial loss of muscle tone:<br />

the head may drop forwards, or the patient may slump<br />

somewhat, but falls are unusual. Many patients will also<br />

have some simple automatisms, such as lip-smacking,<br />

chewing or fumbling, during the absence (Fuster et al. 1954;<br />

Penry and Dreifuss 1969; So et al. 1984, Sadleir et al. 2006).<br />

There may very rarely be other features, such as auditory or<br />

visual hallucinations (Guinena and Taher 1955).<br />

A variant of petit mal, known as ‘atypical absence’ may<br />

be seen, most commonly in patients with mental retardation.<br />

These atypical absences are of more gradual onset and<br />

offset, tend to last longer overall, and may be associated<br />

with prominent increased muscle tone (Holmes et al.<br />

1987).<br />

GRAND MAL SEIZURES<br />

Grand mal seizures, often referred to as generalized<br />

tonic–clonic seizures, may be preceded by an aura composed<br />

of any of the symptoms or signs seen during simple<br />

partial seizures, or may evolve out of a complex partial partial<br />

seizure, a process known as secondary generalization.<br />

As discussed below, under Etiology, it is critical to determine<br />

whether or not grand mal seizures are preceded by an<br />

aura or a complex partial seizure: grand mal seizures preceded<br />

by such events may be assumed to have a ‘focal’<br />

onset, that is to say to be due to a more or less localized<br />

lesion, whereas those that lack such preceding events are<br />

more likely to be occurring as part of one of the idiopathic<br />

generalized epilepsies. Caution must be exercised here,<br />

however, before deciding that no ‘focal’ features are present,<br />

given that many patients who did, under video–EEG<br />

monitoring, clearly have an aura, will be unable, after<br />

recovering from the grand mal seizure, to recall the aura<br />

(Schulz et al. 1995).<br />

Typical grand mal seizures (Theodore et al. 1994) begin<br />

with an abrupt loss of consciousness, often accompanied<br />

by an inarticulate ‘cry’: immediately there is tonic activity<br />

in all four extremities. After perhaps 15–20 seconds, the<br />

tonic activity slowly fades to be gradually replaced by regular<br />

clonic activity, which, in turn, may last anywhere from<br />

30 seconds to 1.5 minutes. In some cases, there may be<br />

variations, the tonic phase being preceded by a few clonic<br />

jerks, or the tonic phase constituting the majority of the<br />

seizure, with only a few clonic jerks trailing behind. During<br />

the tonic activity, respirations cease and cyanosis may<br />

appear. There is often incontinence of urine, and, during<br />

the clonic phase, the tongue may be bitten. Upon cessation<br />

of the seizure proper, most patients remain in a coma or<br />

stupor for a matter of minutes. A delirium then supervenes,<br />

with prominent confusion, lasting perhaps 15–30<br />

minutes, after which most patients fall into a deep sleep.<br />

ATONIC SEIZURES<br />

Atonic seizures (Gambardella et al. 1994; Lipinski 1977;<br />

Pazzaglia et al. 1985), also known as astatic seizures or<br />

‘drop attacks’, occur without prodrome or aura and are<br />

characterized by a sudden loss of motor tone. In most<br />

cases, this atonus is generalized, and patients fall or slump<br />

to the ground; occasionally, however, the lack of tone may<br />

be focal, with, for example, only an abrupt drooping of the<br />

head. The atonus itself generally lasts on the order of a few<br />

seconds and may or may not be associated with a loss of<br />

consciousness. After the restoration of normal tone, most<br />

patients arise immediately, without any post-ictal confusion;<br />

however, others may, for a minute or two, experience<br />

a more or less profound degree of post-ictal confusion.<br />

Atonic seizures generally occur only in patients who<br />

have already suffered from complex partial or grand mal<br />

seizures for many years (Gambardella et al. 1994; Lipinski<br />

1977; Tinuper et al. 1998).<br />

AMNESTIC SEIZURES<br />

Amnestic seizures, also known as ‘pure epileptic amnesia’,<br />

are characterized solely by the appearance of amnesia. As<br />

such, they differ from complex partial seizures in that there<br />

is no impairment of consciousness, no motionless stare,<br />

and no automatic behavior. Typically, the amnesia itself is<br />

primarily of the anterograde type but, in some cases, the<br />

amnesia represents a combination of anterograde and retrograde<br />

types. Rarely, the seizure will be characterized by<br />

retrograde amnesia alone. Examples of each type follow.<br />

Amnestic seizures of the anterograde type<br />

These seizures are characterized by the abrupt onset of a<br />

loss of short-term memory: patients are able to recall<br />

events that occurred up to the onset of the seizure, and<br />

behave normally during the seizure itself, but subsequent<br />

to the termination of the seizure, they have no or only<br />

spotty recall of the events that transpired concurrent with<br />

the seizure itself; they generally last from minutes up to an<br />

hour (Butler et al. 2007; Palmini et al. 1992). Palmini et al.<br />

(1992) provide some interesting examples. In one case, a<br />

waitress, during her seizure, ‘was able to compute a customer’s<br />

bill accurately and to bring him a correct amount<br />

of change’; later, upon recovery, ‘she then realized she had

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!