30.04.2013 Views

Vol. 15 - Deutsches Primatenzentrum

Vol. 15 - Deutsches Primatenzentrum

Vol. 15 - Deutsches Primatenzentrum

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Lemur News <strong>Vol</strong>. <strong>15</strong>, 2010 Page <strong>15</strong><br />

Observations<br />

The event was observed during regular behavioural observations<br />

of red-fronted lemurs in Kirindy Forest,60 km north of<br />

Morondava. It was the only snake-lemur interaction observed<br />

during the entire study period from November 2007<br />

to April 2010, in which four lemur groups were followed<br />

daily by one or two observers,respectively (> 4,000 h of observation<br />

data).Red-fronted lemurs live in multi-male,multifemale<br />

groups of 5-12 individuals (Pereira and Kappeler,<br />

1997;Wimmer and Kappeler,2002).The study group (B) that<br />

encountered the boa included 9 individually marked animals<br />

at the time (2 adult females, 5 adult males, 1 juvenile male, 1<br />

male infant).<br />

On March 1,2010,at 7.19 h,sudden alarm calls of several redfronted<br />

lemurs were heard in the study area known as CS7.<br />

Six individuals (2 adult females,2 adult males,1 juvenile male,<br />

1 male infant) could be identified after approaching the group<br />

to within 10 m.Three of them (2 adult females,1 adult male)<br />

emitted "Woofs" and "Huvvs",vocalisations typically uttered<br />

during predator encounters (Fichtel and Kappeler, 2002).<br />

The 5 individuals surrounded an approximately 2 m long<br />

Madagascar ground boa (Fig. 1) that was lying motionless on<br />

the ground. The lemurs sat at a height of 1-2 m, each about<br />

3 m away from the snake, wagging their tails vigorously, ex-<br />

Fig. 1: Madagascar ground boa in Kirindy. (Photo: Lennart<br />

Pyritz)<br />

cept the infant that kept a distance of 5 m during the entire<br />

event and did not display any vocalisations or tail-wagging.<br />

During the next 4 min,one of the adult males approached the<br />

front end of the boa twice, getting as close as 1-2 m. After 5<br />

min, he left the scene. During this time, one of the adult females<br />

also approached the snake up to within 2 m.When the<br />

male left,the second female started to quickly circuit the boa<br />

for 4 min,maintaining a distance of 2-3 m.After 14 min of several<br />

approaches and continuous alarm calls by 3-5 individuals,<br />

the boa moved for the first time,heading slowly away.The remaining<br />

adult male approached the moving snake also within<br />

2 m; also at its front end. About 1 min later, the boa had<br />

moved <strong>15</strong> m away, and the lemurs left in the opposite direction,still<br />

uttering grunts continuously.Once the boa was out<br />

of sight, the mobbing stopped and the lemurs` behaviour returned<br />

to baseline levels.<br />

Discussion<br />

The mobbing reaction of the group was strong and prolonged<br />

and included most of the group members.This is similar<br />

to the behaviour of a black lemur (Eulemur macaco<br />

macaco) group encountering a Madagascar boa at Ambato<br />

Massif, where the group mobbed the snake for <strong>15</strong>-20 min,<br />

and some individuals approached it as close as 1 m before<br />

finally leaving the location (Colquhoun, 1993). It is also noteworthy<br />

that females and males mobbed and approached the<br />

snake in equal measure as observed in a number of other<br />

species (e.g., Tamura, 1989; Ferrari and Ferrari, 1990; Tello et<br />

al., 2002). The infant maintained a larger distance to the boa<br />

and did not engage in the mobbing displays, however. Similar<br />

infant behaviour has also been reported for other primates<br />

(e.g., Ferrari and Ferrari, 1990) and might be due to a higher<br />

susceptibility to an attack due to smaller body size or a lack<br />

of innate experience regarding predator encounters and<br />

mobbing strategies (Curio et al., 1978; Fichtel, in press).<br />

The strong mobbing reaction of the lemurs might be explained<br />

by the hunting strategy of the snake.Boas are ambush<br />

hunters that usually abandon an attack as soon as they have<br />

been detected (Montgomery and Rand, 1978; Slip and Shine,<br />

1988). Therefore, it seems beneficial for prey animals to signal<br />

the ambush hunter quickly and distinctly that it has been<br />

detected. As boas do not pursue their prey after an unsuccessful<br />

attack, it is also unsurprising that the lemurs’ behaviour<br />

returned to baseline levels of anxiety shortly after departing<br />

the site of the predator encounter. In contrast,<br />

groups of red-fronted lemurs showed increased vigilance behaviour<br />

for at least 30-60 min after encountering a fossa<br />

(Cryptoprocta ferox;pers.comm.Jean-Pierre Tolojanahary and<br />

pers.observation by LP),which is probably due to the higher<br />

agility and climbing abilities of the largest mammalian carnivore.Furthermore,fossas<br />

have been observed to hunt cooperatively<br />

and pursue prey up to 45 min (Lührs and Dammhahn,<br />

2009).<br />

There are no quantitative data on predation rates of lemurs<br />

by snakes in Kirindy, only opportunistic observations (e.g.,<br />

Schülke, 2001; Eberle and Kappeler, 2008) that are biased by<br />

several factors, however. The low observation rate of boalemur<br />

interactions could be due to the reptiles` nocturnal<br />

lifestyle (Raxworthy,2003),so that most of the attacks would<br />

occur at night when no or only few observers are working in<br />

the forest. Furthermore, boas at Kirindy are only active during<br />

the rainy season from January to April, when observations<br />

are often limited by dense foliage and frequent rainfalls.<br />

Five of the six individuals taking part in the mobbing were related<br />

(1 adult female and her 4 offspring from the last 4<br />

years), while it is currently unknown whether the second<br />

adult male sired one of the two youngest group members.<br />

However, due to the small number of detailed observations<br />

of predator encounters it remains impossible to identify the<br />

ultimate causes of collective mobbing (kin defence/parental<br />

care, self-/group defence or cultural transmission of enemy<br />

recognition; Curio et al., 1978) in this species for the time<br />

being.<br />

Acknowledgements<br />

LP was supported financially by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft<br />

(DFG; KA 1082/16-1, FuE). We thank field assistant<br />

Jean-Pierre Tolojanahary for sharing his long-term observation<br />

experiences with us. We are also grateful to Peter<br />

Kappeler, Claudia Fichtel and Moritz Rahlfs for constructive<br />

and helpful comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript.<br />

References<br />

Burney, D.A. 2002. Sifaka predation by a large boa. Folia Primatologica<br />

73: 144-145.<br />

Caro, T. 2005. Antipredator defenses in birds and mammals.<br />

The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, London.<br />

Colquhoun, I.C. 1993. The socioecology of Eulemur macaco:<br />

A preliminary report. Pp. 11-23. In: P.M. Kappeler; J.U.<br />

Ganzhorn (eds.). Lemur social systems and their ecological<br />

basis. Plenum Press, New York and London.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!