30.01.2015 Views

Contents - Konrad Lorenz Institute

Contents - Konrad Lorenz Institute

Contents - Konrad Lorenz Institute

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

A Test for Comprehension of False Belief in Chimpanzees<br />

“Smartie” test a True belief b False belief b<br />

correct (%) N correct (%) N correct (%) N<br />

Children: 3-yr-olds 40.0 10 65.0 9 c 27.5 9 c<br />

4-yr-olds 63.6 11 77.3 11 38.6 11<br />

5-yr-olds 60.0 10 60.0 10 22.5 10<br />

6-yr-olds 100.0 10 72.5 10 80.0 10<br />

Autistics 20.0 5 d 56.8 11 14.8 11<br />

Chimpanzees N/A 20.0 4 42.5 4<br />

Table 1. Mean percent correct scores on the three tasks for each category of subject. (a) one trial<br />

each per subject; expected number of correct scores if choosing randomly is 50%. (b) 4 trials of<br />

each per subject for the children, 8 for the autistics and 10 for the chimpanzees, with mean percent<br />

correct score for all subjects; expected with random choice is 25%. (c) one child failed to respond<br />

and was excluded; one child was given 9 trials. (d) 6 subjects failed to respond and were excluded.<br />

Benchmarking the Analogue False Belief Task<br />

Table 1 summarises the main results for the various<br />

tests and these are presented graphically in Figure 2.<br />

Note that chance response levels are 50% correct responses<br />

on the “Smartie” task (a two-choice task)<br />

and 25% correct responses on the analogue task (a<br />

four-choice task). The performance of the children<br />

and the autistic adults on the standard false belief<br />

task (the “Smartie” task) is broadly in line with conventional<br />

findings: three-year-old children and autistic<br />

adults perform at chance level on false belief<br />

tasks, but, as they increase in age, normal children<br />

eventually acquire theory-of-mind, such that by age<br />

six years they have little trouble with these tasks.<br />

However, in this sample only the 6-year-olds scored<br />

Correct Responses (%)<br />

100<br />

90<br />

80<br />

70<br />

60<br />

50<br />

40<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

0<br />

Smartie<br />

TB<br />

FB<br />

3-yr 4-yr 5-yr 6-yr Autistics Chimps<br />

Figure 2. Percentage of correct scores on the conventional false<br />

belief task (Smartie test) and mean percentage of correct scores<br />

on the true and false belief versions of the mechanical analogue<br />

task. The solid horizontal line marks the chance level of<br />

performance on the mechanical analogue task with four choices<br />

(25% correct). The Smartie task is a conventional two-choice<br />

false belief task (expected chance performance of 50% correct<br />

responses: dashed line). All subjects received one trial on the<br />

Smartie task, but four (children), 8 (autistics) or 10 (chimpanzees)<br />

trials on each version of the mechanical analogue task.<br />

at significantly better than chance levels on the<br />

“Smartie” task (binomial test with parameter<br />

p = 0.5: P = 0.001 1-tailed). The 5-year-olds typically<br />

performed at a much poorer level on this task than<br />

would normally be expected.<br />

Comparison of these results with those for the<br />

mechanical analogue false belief task confirms that<br />

the performance of the children and the autistic<br />

adults on this task parallels that for the standard<br />

false belief task (including the unusually poor performance<br />

of the 5-year-olds). The only important<br />

difference here is that the children clearly found the<br />

mechanical analogue a good deal harder to solve<br />

than the conventional task: their performance remains<br />

at chance level for much longer before showing<br />

the expected improvement in performance.<br />

This suggests that the mechanical analogue was<br />

more demanding: it had four choices, whereas conventional<br />

false belief tasks have only two. With this<br />

caveat, the results suggest that the mechanical false<br />

belief task is a reasonable analogue of the standard<br />

false belief task.<br />

Note that both the children of all ages and the autistic<br />

adults are broadly competent on the true belief<br />

version of the mechanical analogue task, as they<br />

should be when they can see that the experimenter<br />

knows about the current state of the apparatus.<br />

Once again, however, the four-choice structure to<br />

the task clearly taxes even the abilities of the sixyear-olds:<br />

they fail to choose correctly on about a<br />

quarter of trials, even though they can pass more<br />

conventional two-choice false belief tasks without<br />

error.<br />

One concern might be that the autistic subjects’<br />

inability to solve false belief tasks correctly could be<br />

attributed to their relatively low verbal ages. However,<br />

only four had verbal ages below 5 years (by<br />

which time normal children are competent at false<br />

Evolution and Cognition ❘ 135 ❘ 2003, Vol. 9, No. 2

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!