30.01.2015 Views

Contents - Konrad Lorenz Institute

Contents - Konrad Lorenz Institute

Contents - Konrad Lorenz Institute

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Fredric J. Heeren<br />

Notes<br />

1 Biological dictionaries now frequently replace the subphylum<br />

name Vertebrata by the newer, broader phylum<br />

name Craniata in order “to represent the distinguishing<br />

characteristics more accurately” (RUDIN 1997). Chief<br />

among craniate distinctions is a manifest head containing<br />

a brain and sensory organs. Modern craniates are also<br />

characterized, as vertebrates were, by a segmented vertebral<br />

column. The group continues to include fish, amphibians,<br />

reptiles, birds, and mammals. In modern<br />

cladograms, the Chordata clade includes the Craniata<br />

clade, the Craniata clade includes the Vertebrata and Myxinoidea<br />

clades, etc.<br />

2 The following distinction is made here between cognition<br />

and intelligence: Cognition is used to describe the application<br />

of mental processes involved in knowledge; while<br />

intelligence describes the ability to know, regardless of its<br />

use. Cognition is the act of using of one’s intelligence. Thus<br />

the human capacity for thought and reason called intelligence<br />

results in human-level cognition, an awareness involving<br />

reasoning and judgment apparently unlike the<br />

mental processes of any other animal on this planet. Human-level<br />

cognition should be detectable, since it tends<br />

to find expression in human-level communication, engineering<br />

feats, abstract and mathematical problem-solving,<br />

musical compositions, fine art, literature, science, etc.<br />

3 That is, the evolution of human-level cognition is dependent<br />

upon a long series of unpredictable, historical events,<br />

making its occurrence on Earth a rarity. If other planets<br />

harbor life, only a very tiny fraction, if any, would then<br />

be expected to host human-level-or-higher cognition.<br />

4 One might argue for a fourth option: that human-level cognition<br />

exists as both a rare fluke and a common or lawlike<br />

property of the universe; but the statements can both be<br />

true only by rendering them meaningless. While there is<br />

nothing logically contradictory about chance mutations<br />

(flukes) and natural selection (law) working together to produce<br />

novel forms of life, the question here is whether it is<br />

rare or common for any such combination of law and<br />

chance to produce forms that result in human-level cognition.<br />

When referring to the evolution of cognition, the first<br />

two hypotheses are contradictory and do not allow for both<br />

as a primary cause.<br />

5 The terms body plan and Bauplan are generally used interchangeably.<br />

James VALENTINE applies the term Bauplan to<br />

“the upper levels of the taxonomic hierarchy” where “phyla-<br />

or class-level clades are characterized by their possession<br />

of particular assemblages of homologous architectural and<br />

structural features” (VALENTINE 1986). Wallace ARTHUR<br />

identifies six morphological characters to distinguish animal<br />

body plans: skeleton, symmetry, pairs of appendages,<br />

body cavity, cleavage pattern, and segmentation (ARTHUR<br />

1997, p27). Like others, ARTHUR tends to identify animal<br />

body plans in the Cambrian period with the animal phyla<br />

(he speaks of the Cambrian “origin of the 35 or so animal<br />

body plans” (ARTHUR 1997, opening page), though in more<br />

general contexts (non-Cambrian) he speaks of “phylum/<br />

class level body plans” (ARTHUR 1997, p27).<br />

6 Developmentalism: emphasizes the importance of understanding<br />

ontogeny—the history of, and the genetic processes<br />

involved in, the development of the individual<br />

organism—for understanding evolution.<br />

Neo-DARWINISM: emphasizes natural selection and mutations<br />

as the overwhelming driving forces for understanding<br />

evolution. Also called the Modern Synthesis (since it synthesizes<br />

these two mechanisms).<br />

Formalism: emphasizes internal constraints toward the evolution<br />

of particular body forms.<br />

Functionalism: emphasizes external adaptations as the primary<br />

force behind the production of characters that function<br />

best in particular environments.<br />

Punctuationalism: emphasizes the geologically abrupt origin<br />

and subsequent stasis (“equilibrium”) of most species.<br />

Gradualism: emphasizes the slow and constant accretion of<br />

small changes that eventually add up to larger changes and<br />

separations between organisms.<br />

Top-down theory: emphasizes the evolution of the higher<br />

taxa first, so that the most widely separated groups appear<br />

early, and “the diversification of the phyla occurs before<br />

that of classes, classes before that of orders, orders before<br />

that of families” (ERWIN/VALENTIN/SEPKOWSKI 1987).<br />

Bottom-up theory: emphasizes the evolution of the higher<br />

taxa from the accumulation of lower taxa, creating a phylogenetic<br />

tree of increasing diversity and eventual disparity.<br />

7 Reductionism is a philosophical method of explaining a<br />

complex set of facts by reducing them to a set of smaller,<br />

simpler facts; the whole should be predictable from its<br />

smaller, constituent parts.<br />

8 Constraints may be negative or positive; negatively, they are<br />

restrictions on evolution’s direction; positively, they are<br />

preferred directionality of variation; either internal or external<br />

factors may constrain evolution toward particular<br />

forms. Channels are usually positive, internal, preferred<br />

evolutionary pathways.<br />

9 Convergence is the explanation for shared characters of independently<br />

evolved organisms. In GOULD’s lexicon, the<br />

convergence of characters is based upon common external<br />

adaptations. He carefully distinguishes convergence from<br />

parallelism, which is the independent origin of common<br />

features channeled by internal constraints of homologous<br />

genes or developmental pathways. Other scientists frequently<br />

employ the term convergence to include any case<br />

where the evolution of characters repeats itself, whether<br />

explained by external constraints or internal channeling.<br />

10 Disparity is the word usually used to describe differences<br />

between organisms that involve whole body plans; diversity<br />

is reserved for differences between lower-taxa organisms,<br />

especially at the species level (GOULD 1989, p49).<br />

11 WEINBERG distinguishes between “consciousness itself”, the<br />

self-awareness/feelings experienced by humans, and “correlatives<br />

to consciousness” that may be examined in terms<br />

of brain waves, electrical activity, hormones in the blood,<br />

etc. (WEINBERG 1992, p44).<br />

Evolution and Cognition ❘ 154 ❘ 2003, Vol. 9, No. 2

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!