30.01.2015 Views

Contents - Konrad Lorenz Institute

Contents - Konrad Lorenz Institute

Contents - Konrad Lorenz Institute

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Theresa S. S. Schilhab<br />

What supports this contention First, explicit<br />

learning involves a significant reduction of parameters<br />

in mental representation, which I refer to as<br />

“condensation of reality”. But research paradigms<br />

demonstrating implicit learning are typically complex<br />

in the sense of concealing a huge number of parameters<br />

essential to solving the task in a large web<br />

of information, which makes explicit strategies impossible<br />

to employ. For instance VINTER/PERRUCHET<br />

(2000) reported on implicit drawing behaviour in<br />

children aged 4–10 years. To confront criticism related<br />

to explicit knowledge criteria, the researchers<br />

developed a paradigm named a “neutral parameter<br />

procedure” and deliberately diverted participant’s<br />

attention to test clues to avoid the use of explicit<br />

knowledge obtained during the test procedure. Experimenters<br />

specifically attempted to blur conscious<br />

awareness of connections between any two variables.<br />

Still, by responding appropriately, the subjects<br />

seemed capable of catching contingencies between<br />

many variables, while being perfectly<br />

unaware of doing so.<br />

The same applies to the artificial grammar test.<br />

When subjects were asked to explain how they discriminated<br />

between grammatical and nongrammatical<br />

sentences, they either denied consciously following<br />

any rule or they gave reasons irrelevant to<br />

solving the task.<br />

Similar results are obtained with computer simulations<br />

of sugar fabrication (BERRY/DIENES 1993).<br />

Here people were asked to attain a specific amount<br />

of sugar (in tons) by varying different parameters<br />

interconnected by a pattern created by the experimenters,<br />

but opaque to the subjects. After a number<br />

of trials they succeeded. When subjects were<br />

subsequently asked to verbalise which rules they<br />

applied to obtain the outcome and these rules were<br />

followed by novices the end result was strikingly<br />

poor. Obviously, the articulated rules do not mirror<br />

the rules actually applied. Somehow knowledge residual<br />

to the volunteered information remains to<br />

be revealed. In support of this are studies within<br />

the sugar production paradigm that focus on how<br />

to make explicit what could originally be learned<br />

only implicitly. By pinpointing the connections<br />

between dependent variables or reducing the number<br />

of parameters responsible for a successful outcome,<br />

subjects suddenly gained insight they could<br />

subsequently verbalise. Evidently, to switch from<br />

implicit to explicit modes of learning involves<br />

both accentuating elements that determine the<br />

task and reduction of the number of elements<br />

(DIENES 1993).<br />

Implicit Learning and the<br />

Evolutionary Stance<br />

The premise that implicit learning is more congruent<br />

with reality than explicit learning as regards representation<br />

can be explained theoretically. From an<br />

evolutionary perspective, for social organism like humans<br />

the physical as well at the social environment<br />

is enmeshed in information of potential importance<br />

to survival (BYRNE 1995). The mere fact of balancing<br />

the surface can be realised only by taking in a large<br />

amount of information (COTTERILL 2002). Likewise,<br />

navigation in the social world, originally adapted to<br />

interpret tribal relations and coalitions in prehistoric<br />

society (WRANGHAM et al. 1994), now entails coping<br />

with a multitude of modern social conventions and<br />

regulations (COLLINS 2001) such as, for example, table<br />

manners and traffic rules. Information important<br />

to survival does not come about in isolated incidents,<br />

but relates to cues in the environment.<br />

Environmental complexity clearly explains why<br />

implicit learning is less detached from context<br />

with respect to condensating reality. But the question<br />

remains, does this hold for (a) detachments<br />

caused by simplification and (b) does not any interpretation<br />

involve detachments irrespective of<br />

whether it results in simplifcations or distortions<br />

of the original source of information Of course<br />

the answer is affirmative. Over time, natural selection<br />

favoured perception and learning of certain<br />

stimuli and environmental contingencies, which<br />

naturally entails perspective-taking followed by<br />

simplification or distortion of information.<br />

Though the difference in contextual detachment<br />

between implicit and explicit learning is a matter<br />

of degree, the consequences are significant. Explicit<br />

learning, however, emerged as an expansion<br />

to an already well functioning system (see also EN-<br />

NEN 2002). The contribution of the new system was<br />

primarily to control learning capabilities, including<br />

the deliberate selection of information to be<br />

learned and the intentional application of the<br />

knowledge obtained (GIBSON/INGOLD 1993).<br />

Though the explicit learning system evolved to<br />

profit from intentionel manipulation of information,<br />

it emerged to increase survival.<br />

To understand in what respect implicit learning<br />

is closer to context we must return to evolutionary<br />

considerations. Implicit learning registers a greater<br />

number of variables, as well as their intricate pattern,<br />

‘invisible’ to the explicit capacity, because it<br />

seems appropriate to survival. But why This appears<br />

due to that implicit learning was developed<br />

Evolution and Cognition ❘ 174 ❘ 2003, Vol. 9, No. 2

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!