21.06.2013 Views

Ganong's Review of Medical Physiology, 23rd Edition

Ganong's Review of Medical Physiology, 23rd Edition

Ganong's Review of Medical Physiology, 23rd Edition

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

sylvian fissure (lateral cerebral sulcus) <strong>of</strong> the categorical hemisphere.<br />

A region at the posterior end <strong>of</strong> the superior temporal<br />

gyrus called Wernicke’s area (Figure 19–7) is concerned with<br />

comprehension <strong>of</strong> auditory and visual information. It projects<br />

via the arcuate fasciculus to Broca’s area (area 44) in the<br />

frontal lobe immediately in front <strong>of</strong> the inferior end <strong>of</strong> the motor<br />

cortex. Broca’s area processes the information received<br />

from Wernicke’s area into a detailed and coordinated pattern<br />

for vocalization and then projects the pattern via a speech articulation<br />

area in the insula to the motor cortex, which initiates<br />

the appropriate movements <strong>of</strong> the lips, tongue, and<br />

larynx to produce speech. The probable sequence <strong>of</strong> events<br />

that occurs when a subject names a visual object is shown in<br />

Figure 19–8. The angular gyrus behind Wernicke’s area appears<br />

to process information from words that are read in such<br />

a way that they can be converted into the auditory forms <strong>of</strong> the<br />

words in Wernicke’s area.<br />

It is interesting that in individuals who learn a second language<br />

in adulthood, fMRI reveals that the portion <strong>of</strong> Broca’s<br />

area concerned with it is adjacent to but separate from the<br />

area concerned with the native language. However, in children<br />

who learn two languages early in life, only a single area<br />

is involved with both. It is well known, <strong>of</strong> course, that children<br />

acquire fluency in a second language more easily than<br />

adults.<br />

LANGUAGE DISORDERS<br />

Aphasias are abnormalities <strong>of</strong> language functions that are not<br />

due to defects <strong>of</strong> vision or hearing or to motor paralysis. They<br />

are caused by lesions in the categorical hemisphere (see Clinical<br />

Box 19–2). The most common cause is embolism or thrombosis<br />

<strong>of</strong> a cerebral blood vessel. Many different classifications <strong>of</strong> the<br />

aphasias have been published, but a convenient classification<br />

divides them into fluent, nonfluent, and anomic aphasias. In<br />

nonfluent aphasia, the lesion is in Broca’s area (Table 19–1).<br />

Speech is slow, and words are hard to come by. Patients with severe<br />

damage to this area are limited to two or three words with<br />

which to express the whole range <strong>of</strong> meaning and emotion.<br />

Broca’s<br />

area<br />

Arcuate<br />

fasciculus<br />

Angular gyrus<br />

Wernicke’s area<br />

FIGURE 19–7 Location <strong>of</strong> some <strong>of</strong> the areas in the categorical<br />

hemisphere that are concerned with language functions.<br />

CHAPTER 19 Learning, Memory, Language, & Speech 297<br />

Broca’s area<br />

Facial area <strong>of</strong><br />

motor cortex<br />

(area 4)<br />

Wernicke’s<br />

area<br />

(area 22)<br />

Angular gyrus<br />

(area 39)<br />

Higher order visual<br />

cortical areas<br />

(area 18)<br />

FIGURE 19–8 Path taken by impulses when a subject names a<br />

visual object projected on a horizontal section <strong>of</strong> the human brain.<br />

Sometimes the words retained are those that were being spoken<br />

at the time <strong>of</strong> the injury or vascular accident that caused the<br />

aphasia.<br />

In one form <strong>of</strong> fluent aphasia, the lesion is in Wernicke’s<br />

area. In this condition, speech itself is normal and sometimes<br />

the patients talk excessively. However, what they say is full <strong>of</strong><br />

jargon and neologisms that make little sense. The patient also<br />

fails to comprehend the meaning <strong>of</strong> spoken or written words,<br />

so other aspects <strong>of</strong> the use <strong>of</strong> language are compromised.<br />

Another form <strong>of</strong> fluent aphasia is a condition in which<br />

patients can speak relatively well and have good auditory<br />

comprehension but cannot put parts <strong>of</strong> words together or<br />

conjure up words. This is called conduction aphasia because<br />

it was thought to be due to lesions <strong>of</strong> the arcuate fasciculus<br />

connecting Wernicke’s and Broca’s areas. However, it now<br />

TABLE 19–1 Aphasias. Characteristic<br />

responses <strong>of</strong> patients with lesions in various<br />

areas when shown a picture <strong>of</strong> a chair.<br />

Type <strong>of</strong> Aphasia and Site <strong>of</strong> Lesion<br />

Nonfluent (Broca’s area) “Tssair”<br />

Characteristic<br />

Naming Errors<br />

Fluent (Wernicke’s area) “Stool” or “choss” (neologism)<br />

Fluent (areas 40, 41, and 42; conduction<br />

aphasia)<br />

6<br />

4<br />

Arcuate<br />

fasciculus<br />

5<br />

From lateral<br />

geniculate nucleus<br />

1<br />

3 2<br />

Left Right<br />

Primary visual cortex<br />

(area 17)<br />

“Flair . . . no, swair . . . tair.”<br />

Anomic (angular gyrus) “I know what it is . . . I have a<br />

lot <strong>of</strong> them.”<br />

Modified from Goodglass H: Disorders <strong>of</strong> naming following brain injury. Am Sci<br />

1980;68:647.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!