21.06.2013 Views

Ganong's Review of Medical Physiology, 23rd Edition

Ganong's Review of Medical Physiology, 23rd Edition

Ganong's Review of Medical Physiology, 23rd Edition

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

298 SECTION III Central & Peripheral Neurophysiology<br />

appears that it is due to lesions in and around the auditory<br />

cortex (areas 40, 41, and 42).<br />

When a lesion damages the angular gyrus in the categorical<br />

hemisphere without affecting Wernicke’s or Broca’s areas,<br />

there is no difficulty with speech or the understanding <strong>of</strong><br />

auditory information; instead there is trouble understanding<br />

written language or pictures, because visual information is not<br />

processed and transmitted to Wernicke’s area. The result is a<br />

condition called anomic aphasia.<br />

The isolated lesions that cause the selective defects<br />

described above occur in some patients, but brain destruction<br />

is <strong>of</strong>ten more general. Consequently, more than one form <strong>of</strong><br />

aphasia is <strong>of</strong>ten present. Frequently, the aphasia is general<br />

(global), involving both receptive and expressive functions.<br />

In this situation, speech is scant as well as nonfluent. Writing<br />

is abnormal in all aphasias in which speech is abnormal, but<br />

the neural circuits involved are unknown. In addition, deaf<br />

subjects who develop a lesion in the categorical hemisphere<br />

lose their ability to communicate in sign language.<br />

Stuttering has been found to be associated with right cerebral<br />

dominance and widespread overactivity in the cerebral<br />

cortex and cerebellum. This includes increased activity <strong>of</strong> the<br />

supplementary motor area. Stimulation <strong>of</strong> part <strong>of</strong> this area has<br />

been reported to produce laughter, with the duration and<br />

intensity <strong>of</strong> the laughter proportionate to the intensity <strong>of</strong> the<br />

stimulus.<br />

RECOGNITION OF FACES<br />

An important part <strong>of</strong> the visual input goes to the inferior temporal<br />

lobe, where representations <strong>of</strong> objects, particularly faces,<br />

are stored (Figure 19–9). Faces are particularly important in<br />

distinguishing friends from foes and the emotional state <strong>of</strong><br />

those seen. In humans, storage and recognition <strong>of</strong> faces is<br />

more strongly represented in the right inferior temporal lobe<br />

in right-handed individuals, though the left lobe is also active.<br />

Lesions in this area cause prosopagnosia, the inability to recognize<br />

faces. Patients with this abnormality can recognize<br />

forms and reproduce them. They can recognize people by<br />

Stores<br />

biographical<br />

information<br />

Connects facial features<br />

to biographical information<br />

Extracts<br />

facial<br />

features<br />

FIGURE 19–9 Areas in the right cerebral hemisphere, in<br />

right-handed individuals, that are concerned with recognition <strong>of</strong><br />

faces. (Modified from Szpir M: Accustomed to your face. Am Sci 1992;80:539.)<br />

their voices, and many <strong>of</strong> them show autonomic responses<br />

when they see familiar as opposed to unfamiliar faces. However,<br />

they cannot identify the familiar faces they see. The left<br />

hemisphere is also involved, but the role <strong>of</strong> the right hemisphere<br />

is primary. The presence <strong>of</strong> an autonomic response to<br />

a familiar face in the absence <strong>of</strong> recognition has been explained<br />

by postulating the existence <strong>of</strong> a separate dorsal pathway<br />

for processing information about faces that leads to<br />

recognition at only a subconscious level.<br />

LOCALIZATION OF OTHER FUNCTIONS<br />

Use <strong>of</strong> fMRI and PET scanning combined with study <strong>of</strong> patients<br />

with strokes and head injuries has provided further insights—or<br />

at least glimpses—into the ways serial processing <strong>of</strong><br />

sensory information produce cognition, reasoning, comprehension,<br />

and language. Analysis <strong>of</strong> the brain regions involved<br />

in arithmetic calculations has highlighted two areas. In the inferior<br />

portion <strong>of</strong> the left frontal lobe is an area concerned with<br />

number facts and exact calculations. Frontal lobe lesions can<br />

cause acalculia, a selective impairment <strong>of</strong> mathematical ability.<br />

There are areas around the intraparietal sulci <strong>of</strong> both parietal<br />

lobes that are concerned with visuospatial representations<br />

<strong>of</strong> numbers and, presumably, finger counting.<br />

Two right-sided subcortical structures play a role in accurate<br />

navigation in humans. One is the right hippocampus, which is<br />

concerned with learning where places are located, and the other<br />

is the right caudate nucleus, which facilitates movement to the<br />

places. Men have larger brains than women and are said to have<br />

superior spatial skills and ability to navigate.<br />

Other defects seen in patients with localized cortical lesions<br />

include, for example, the inability to name animals, though the<br />

ability to name other living things and objects is intact. One<br />

patient with a left parietal lesion had difficulty with the second<br />

half but not the first half <strong>of</strong> words. Some patients with parietooccipital<br />

lesions write only with consonants and omit vowels.<br />

The pattern that emerges from studies <strong>of</strong> this type is one <strong>of</strong><br />

precise sequential processing <strong>of</strong> information in localized brain<br />

areas. Additional research <strong>of</strong> this type should greatly expand<br />

our understanding <strong>of</strong> the functions <strong>of</strong> the neocortex.<br />

CHAPTER SUMMARY<br />

■ Long-term memory is divided into explicit (declarative) and<br />

implicit (nondeclarative). Explicit is further subdivided into<br />

semantic and episodic. Implicit is further subdivided into<br />

priming, procedural, associative learning, and nonassociative<br />

learning.<br />

■ Synaptic plasticity is the ability <strong>of</strong> neural tissue to change as reflected<br />

by LTP (an increased effectiveness <strong>of</strong> synaptic activity) or<br />

LTD (a reduced effectiveness <strong>of</strong> synaptic activity) after continued<br />

use.<br />

■ Hippocampal and other temporal lobe structures and association<br />

cortex are involved in declarative memory.<br />

■ Alzheimer disease is characterized by progressive loss <strong>of</strong> shortterm<br />

memory followed by general loss <strong>of</strong> cognitive function.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!