20.10.2015 Views

A COMPENDIUM OF SCALES for use in the SCHOLARSHIP OF TEACHING AND LEARNING

compscalesstl

compscalesstl

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

ends with some unanswered questions regard<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> assessment of professor-student<br />

relationships and call <strong>for</strong> future research <strong>in</strong> this area.<br />

Immediacy Scale<br />

Immediacy is <strong>the</strong> nonverbal and verbal behaviors that communicate lik<strong>in</strong>g, car<strong>in</strong>g, and<br />

approachability (Mehrabian, 1967; 1969). With regard to <strong>the</strong> classroom, immediacy refers to<br />

students’ impressions of <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>in</strong>structors’ availability or psychological distance. Early<br />

discussions of measur<strong>in</strong>g immediacy came about <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> late 1960s, with a focus on captur<strong>in</strong>g<br />

verbal and nonverbal communications of closeness (Mehrabian, 1969). Mehrabian identified<br />

five primary immediacy behaviors: touch, physical distance between communicator and<br />

audience, lean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>for</strong>ward, mak<strong>in</strong>g and ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g eye contact, and physical orientation<br />

toward audience. While immediacy does not <strong>in</strong> and of itself represent rapport (immediacy is<br />

behavior-based whereas rapport relates more to cognitive perceptions), it is considered a<br />

catalyst and predictor of rapport and relationship-build<strong>in</strong>g (see above discussion of Figure 1).<br />

Immediacy behaviors <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> classroom received fur<strong>the</strong>r exploration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> late 1980s (Gorham,<br />

1988). Expand<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>the</strong> five nonverbal behaviors outl<strong>in</strong>ed by Mehrabian (1969), Gorham<br />

created one of <strong>the</strong> first validated immediacy scales that <strong>in</strong>cluded both nonverbal and verbal<br />

constructs. The 34-item scale conta<strong>in</strong>s 20 verbal items (e.g., “addresses students by name,”<br />

“<strong>use</strong>s humor <strong>in</strong> class,” and “asks questions that solicit viewpo<strong>in</strong>ts or op<strong>in</strong>ions) and 14 nonverbal<br />

items (e.g., “gestures while talk<strong>in</strong>g to class,” and “smiles at <strong>the</strong> class as a whole, not just<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividual students”). Students respond by rat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> frequency with which each behavior<br />

occurs on a scale of 0 (never occurs) to 4 (occurs very often). This validation study demonstrated<br />

split-half reliabilities of .94 <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> verbal items and .84 <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> nonverbal items.<br />

The orig<strong>in</strong>al scale underwent some criticism when scholars argued that verbal behaviors<br />

represented teacher effectiveness, not immediacy (Rob<strong>in</strong>son & Richmond, 1995). Inspired by<br />

this criticism, Wilson and Locker (2008) set out to empirically address this argument. Their<br />

validation provided evidence <strong>for</strong> discrim<strong>in</strong>ant validity between immediacy and teacher<br />

effectiveness. Fur<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong>ir analysis of Gorham’s (1988) orig<strong>in</strong>al scale produced four dist<strong>in</strong>ct<br />

factors: 1) <strong>in</strong>dividual friendl<strong>in</strong>ess (8 items, α = .88), 2) flexibility dur<strong>in</strong>g lecture (5 items, α = .80),<br />

3) nonverbal immediacy (7 items, α = .76), and a s<strong>in</strong>gle item assess<strong>in</strong>g whe<strong>the</strong>r professors <strong>in</strong>vite<br />

students to address <strong>the</strong>m by <strong>the</strong>ir first name. Fur<strong>the</strong>r, based on <strong>the</strong>ir analysis, Wilson and<br />

Locker (2008) recommended exclud<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g three items: 1) “asks questions or<br />

encourages students to talk”, 2) “refers to class as ‘our’ class or what ‘we’ are do<strong>in</strong>g”, and 3)<br />

“<strong>in</strong>vites students to telephone or meet outside of class if <strong>the</strong>y have a question or want to<br />

discuss someth<strong>in</strong>g” from Gorham’s (1988) orig<strong>in</strong>al scale as <strong>the</strong>se items did not add any<br />

predictive value to <strong>the</strong> scale. The most f<strong>in</strong>al version of this scale is Wilson and Locker (2008).<br />

Nonverbal Immediacy Scale<br />

Whereas <strong>the</strong> updated immediacy scale (Gorham, 1980; Wilson & Locker, 2008) <strong>in</strong>cludes<br />

measures of both verbal and nonverbal psychological availability, <strong>the</strong> Nonverbal Immediacy<br />

Scale (NIS) foc<strong>use</strong>s on nonverbal cues specifically (V. Richmond et al., 2003). This scale, which is<br />

151

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!