20.10.2015 Views

A COMPENDIUM OF SCALES for use in the SCHOLARSHIP OF TEACHING AND LEARNING

compscalesstl

compscalesstl

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

ated on a 5-po<strong>in</strong>t Likert scale from 1 (never) to 5 (frequent) on how often <strong>the</strong> professor<br />

exhibits <strong>the</strong> given teach<strong>in</strong>g behavior. For <strong>in</strong>stance, <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> behavior of 'provides constructive<br />

feedback,' <strong>the</strong> question would state, “Writes comments on returned work, answers students’<br />

questions, and gives advice on test tak<strong>in</strong>g” (Keeley et al., 2006, p. 85). In <strong>the</strong> case of SoTL scales<br />

such as <strong>the</strong> TBC (or check out The Teacher Immediacy Scale by Gorham, 1988) <strong>the</strong>se measures<br />

are, albeit <strong>in</strong>direct records of specific behaviors, but allow researchers to approximate specific<br />

behavioral outcomes.<br />

Third, to mitigate some of <strong>the</strong> pitfalls with self-report scales is to <strong>use</strong> SoTL scales that are<br />

scenario based. For <strong>in</strong>stance, Berry, West, and Denehey (1989) developed a self-report SoTL<br />

scale to measure self-efficacy <strong>for</strong> memory tasks called <strong>the</strong> Memory <strong>for</strong> Self-Efficacy<br />

Questionnaire (MSEQ). The MSEQ has been widely <strong>use</strong>d and reta<strong>in</strong>s acceptable validity and<br />

reliability (Berry et al., 1989; West, Thorn & Bagwell, 2003). The measure is comprised of 40<br />

confidence rat<strong>in</strong>gs on 10 memory scenarios. With<strong>in</strong> each scenario <strong>the</strong> first question describes<br />

<strong>the</strong> simplest memory task <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> scenario <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> 4 th question describes <strong>the</strong> most difficult<br />

memory task. Individuals are asked to give <strong>the</strong>ir confidence rat<strong>in</strong>g (0-100%) on <strong>the</strong>ir ability to<br />

successfully accomplish <strong>the</strong> memory task. For example, <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> MSEQ <strong>in</strong>dividuals are given a<br />

scenario about <strong>the</strong>ir ability to remember a grocery list. The first question (which is <strong>the</strong> easiest<br />

memory task) would state, “If I heard it twice, I could remember 2 items from a friend’s grocery<br />

list of <strong>the</strong> 12 items, without tak<strong>in</strong>g any list with me to <strong>the</strong> store” (Berry et al., 1989, p. 713).<br />

Whereas <strong>the</strong> most difficult memory task would state, “If I heard it twice, I could remember 12<br />

items from a friend’s grocery list of 12 items, without tak<strong>in</strong>g any list with me to <strong>the</strong> store” (p.<br />

713). Confidence rat<strong>in</strong>gs are <strong>the</strong>n totaled <strong>for</strong> each memory scenario (e.g., grocery list, phone<br />

numbers, pictures, location, words, etc.). As you can see, SoTL scales such as <strong>the</strong> MSEQ are selfreport,<br />

however <strong>the</strong>y are rooted <strong>in</strong> past per<strong>for</strong>mance and actual real-life examples of what<br />

students or teachers may experience. The benefit of <strong>the</strong>se types of measures is that <strong>the</strong>y<br />

attempt to obta<strong>in</strong> external validity by putt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> respondents <strong>in</strong> real-life scenarios.<br />

In SoTL research, self-reports are <strong>in</strong>evitable and do serve a purpose. However, we are not<br />

cryptozoologists, we are SoTL scholars. So <strong>in</strong> order to debunk <strong>the</strong> mythical creatures such as<br />

Bigfoot, it is important that we complement self-report measures with behavioral measures<br />

and/or select and create SoTL scales that are rooted <strong>in</strong> actual behavior.<br />

Measur<strong>in</strong>g Yeti’s Metacognition and Use of Learn<strong>in</strong>g Strategies<br />

Do you th<strong>in</strong>k Yeti, aka <strong>the</strong> abdom<strong>in</strong>al snowman, th<strong>in</strong>ks about th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g or employs effective<br />

learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies? Likely—not! But if he did, how would we know? For that matter, how do<br />

we know our students’ metacognition and <strong>use</strong> of learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies? We could <strong>use</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

previously described MAI by Schraw and Dennison (1994), but it is 52-items long and a little<br />

<strong>the</strong>oretically dated (e.g., metacognitive awareness is more than knowledge and regulation of<br />

cognition). If you are comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g this with o<strong>the</strong>r measures <strong>for</strong> a study, you run <strong>the</strong> risk of gett<strong>in</strong>g<br />

participant fatigue and consequential response sets (e.g., answer<strong>in</strong>g 5 to all questions or<br />

play<strong>in</strong>g connect <strong>the</strong> dots). If you want someth<strong>in</strong>g shorter you could <strong>use</strong> <strong>the</strong> Need <strong>for</strong> Cognition<br />

Scale (NCS) by Cacioppo, Petty, Fe<strong>in</strong>ste<strong>in</strong>, and Jarvis (1996). The NCS is a personality measure of<br />

metacognition <strong>in</strong>tended to measure <strong>the</strong> extent to which an <strong>in</strong>dividual might need to engage <strong>in</strong><br />

178

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!