20.10.2015 Views

A COMPENDIUM OF SCALES for use in the SCHOLARSHIP OF TEACHING AND LEARNING

compscalesstl

compscalesstl

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

not limited to professor-student relationships, conta<strong>in</strong>s a total of 26 items drawn from <strong>the</strong><br />

previous literature (13 positively worded items and 13 negatively words items). An advantage<br />

to <strong>the</strong> NIS is <strong>the</strong> ability to distribute an o<strong>the</strong>r-report<strong>in</strong>g version (NIS-O) as well as a selfreport<strong>in</strong>g<br />

(NIS-S) version. Although <strong>the</strong> items on <strong>the</strong> NIS-O and NIS-S measure <strong>the</strong> same<br />

constructs, <strong>the</strong>ir word<strong>in</strong>g varies slightly <strong>in</strong> that <strong>the</strong> NIS-O frames items <strong>in</strong> terms of what "I" do<br />

and <strong>the</strong> NIS-S frames items <strong>in</strong> terms of what s/he (<strong>the</strong> professor) does. Participants rate <strong>the</strong><br />

frequency of <strong>the</strong> behavior us<strong>in</strong>g a 5-po<strong>in</strong>t Likert-type scale from 0 (never) to 5 (very often).<br />

Sample items from <strong>the</strong> scale <strong>in</strong>clude, "I smile when I talk to people," "I <strong>use</strong> a monotone or dull<br />

voice while talk<strong>in</strong>g to people," and "I avoid eye contact while talk<strong>in</strong>g to people." Negative<br />

behaviors such as us<strong>in</strong>g a monotone voice and avoid<strong>in</strong>g eye contact are reverse scored. This<br />

scale yielded high <strong>in</strong>ternal reliability estimates <strong>for</strong> <strong>the</strong> NIS-S and NIS-O of .90-.93 across several<br />

targets (self, teacher, supervisor, and a romantic date).<br />

LeFebvre and Allen (2014) provided evidence <strong>for</strong> criterion validity of <strong>the</strong> Nonverbal Immediacy<br />

Scale. In <strong>the</strong>ir study, students enrolled <strong>in</strong> lecture/laboratory or self-conta<strong>in</strong>ed courses<br />

completed <strong>the</strong> NIS, a measure of affective learn<strong>in</strong>g, an <strong>in</strong>struction evaluation measure, and<br />

allowed <strong>the</strong>ir grades to be tracked. Students who perceived greater nonverbal immediacy from<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir teach<strong>in</strong>g assistants also received higher grades <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> courses, reported more affective<br />

learn<strong>in</strong>g (e.g., lik<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>the</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g assistant, want<strong>in</strong>g to enroll <strong>in</strong> ano<strong>the</strong>r course with that<br />

teach<strong>in</strong>g assistant), and provided more positive <strong>in</strong>struction evaluations.<br />

Aside from assess<strong>in</strong>g students’ perceptions of professors’ nonverbal immediacy behaviors,<br />

students may also benefit from us<strong>in</strong>g this scale when discuss<strong>in</strong>g nonverbal communication,<br />

person-perception accuracy, and universal versus cultural understand<strong>in</strong>gs of emotional<br />

expression. Beca<strong>use</strong> <strong>the</strong> NIS (V. Richmond et al., 2003) has a self-assessment version and an<br />

o<strong>the</strong>r-assessment version, students can rate <strong>the</strong>ir own nonverbal immediacy behaviors and ask<br />

a close friend and a new acqua<strong>in</strong>tance to also rate <strong>the</strong>m. Through this activity, students can<br />

learn about measurement error, self- vs. o<strong>the</strong>r-rat<strong>in</strong>g methods, and convergent validity.<br />

Teacher Humor Scale<br />

Humor is one component of verbal immediacy that may lead to <strong>in</strong>creases <strong>in</strong> rapport when <strong>use</strong>d<br />

<strong>in</strong> a context-appropriate and respectful manner. Research shows that <strong>the</strong> <strong>use</strong> of humor by <strong>the</strong><br />

professor corresponds to teachers’ immediacy behaviors <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> classroom, and has an impact<br />

on learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes (Gorham & Christophel, 1990). High rat<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> professor humor<br />

orientation, or <strong>the</strong> predisposition <strong>for</strong> a professor to engage <strong>in</strong> humorous communication, are<br />

associated with greater positive affect toward <strong>the</strong> professor as well as <strong>in</strong>creased perceived<br />

learn<strong>in</strong>g. There is also a positive correlation between perceived professor humor and nonverbal<br />

immediacy behaviors and responsiveness to students (Wanzer & Frymier, 1999).<br />

An important part of successfully us<strong>in</strong>g humor, especially with<strong>in</strong> a classroom sett<strong>in</strong>g, is <strong>the</strong> need<br />

to understand how <strong>the</strong> audience (e.g., <strong>the</strong> students) <strong>in</strong>terprets humor. Torok, McMorris, and<br />

L<strong>in</strong> (2004) conducted a study explor<strong>in</strong>g students’ perceptions of professor humor, specifically<br />

hop<strong>in</strong>g to f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong> reason<strong>in</strong>g beh<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong> positive relationship between professor humor and<br />

student engagement. Seventy three percent of students questioned stated that <strong>the</strong>y felt very<br />

152

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!