11.04.2013 Views

The Literary Mind.pdf

The Literary Mind.pdf

The Literary Mind.pdf

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

MANY SPACES 87<br />

manipulating the input space in which it resides? George Lakoff and I have given<br />

one argument for the existence of generic spaces, as follows.<br />

When we read a proverb in a book of proverbs, and so have no reason to<br />

connect the meaning of the proverb to any specific target, we arrive at a generic<br />

reading. For example, "Look before you leap," found in a book of proverbs (or in<br />

a fortune cookie), will be interpreted generically. Similarly, "When the cat's away,<br />

the mice will play" presents a source story of mice who behave in a restricted<br />

fashion when the cat is around but who behave with fewer restrictions when the<br />

cat is gone. <strong>The</strong> generic-level information in this story can be projected to a<br />

generic space with an abstract story: One agent or group of agents constrains<br />

another agent or group of agents, and when the governing agent is inattentive,<br />

the otherwise constrained agent or agents behave more freely. We can reach this<br />

generic interpretation even if we have no specific target onto which we wish to<br />

project it. So the generic space has a conceptual existence.<br />

Lakoff and I have called this kind of projection from a source story to a<br />

generic story GENERIC IS SPECIFIC: generic information, often image-schematic,<br />

is projected from a specific space to give structure to a generic space. Of course,<br />

this generic space "applies" to the specific space from which it came. Once the<br />

generic space is established, we may project it onto a range of specific target spaces.<br />

<strong>The</strong> generic space constructed out of "When the cat's away, the mice will play"<br />

can be projected onto stories of the office, the classroom, infidelity, congressional<br />

oversight committees, computer antivirus utilities, and so on, over an unlimited<br />

range.<br />

Blends can be constructed if two stories can be construed as sharing abstract<br />

structure. <strong>The</strong> abstract structure they share is contained in the generic space that<br />

connects them. Consider, for example, the riddle of the Buddhist monk. Its<br />

generic space has a single journeyer taking a single journey from dawn to sunset,<br />

over a single distance along a single path. <strong>The</strong> generic space does not specify the<br />

direction of the journey (up or down), the date of the journey, or the internal<br />

form of the journey (starting and stopping, moving slower or faster). This degree<br />

of inspecificity allows the generic structure to be projected equally well onto the<br />

space of the ascent on the first day, the space of the descent on the second day,<br />

and the blended space where both journeys occur on an unspecified day. <strong>The</strong><br />

riddle of the Buddhist monk involves two input spaces, a generic space, and a<br />

blended space.<br />

It often takes work to find a generic space that fits two input spaces; there<br />

are often alternative generic spaces that might connect two input spaces. But in<br />

some cases, a generic space has been constructed repeatedly between two spaces.<br />

It has come to structure the two input spaces and to establish what seem to be<br />

fixed counterparts. <strong>The</strong> generic space has become fully available from each of<br />

the input spaces. In that case, the generic space becomes invisible to us. If the

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!