24.07.2013 Views

Onto.PT: Towards the Automatic Construction of a Lexical Ontology ...

Onto.PT: Towards the Automatic Construction of a Lexical Ontology ...

Onto.PT: Towards the Automatic Construction of a Lexical Ontology ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

42 Chapter 3. Related Work<br />

• part, n: a piece <strong>of</strong> something;<br />

• piece, n: a portion <strong>of</strong> some material;<br />

Amsler (1981) believes that <strong>the</strong>se loops are usually <strong>the</strong> evidence <strong>of</strong> a truly primitive<br />

concept, such as <strong>the</strong> set containing <strong>the</strong> words class, group, type, kind, set,<br />

division, category, species, individual, grouping, part and section. These primitives<br />

are <strong>of</strong>ten related with “covert categories” (Ide and Véronis, 1995), which are concepts<br />

that do not correspond to any particular word and are introduced to represent<br />

a specific category or group <strong>of</strong> concepts. For instance, <strong>the</strong>re is no word to describe<br />

<strong>the</strong> hypernym <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> concepts described by tool, utensil, implement and instrument,<br />

so a new “covert” hypernym, instrumental-object, is artificially created.<br />

Chodorow et al. (1985) introduced <strong>the</strong> notion <strong>of</strong> “empty heads”. Words belonging<br />

to this small class (e.g. one, any, kind, class, manner, family, race, group,<br />

complex) might occur in <strong>the</strong> beginning <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> definition followed by <strong>the</strong> preposition<br />

<strong>of</strong>, but do not represent <strong>the</strong> superordinate concept. Guthrie et al. (1990) explored<br />

<strong>the</strong> class <strong>of</strong> “empty heads” to extract o<strong>the</strong>r semantic relations, besides hyponymy.<br />

For instance, <strong>the</strong> word member is related with <strong>the</strong> member-set relation (Markowitz<br />

et al., 1986) and <strong>the</strong> word part is related with <strong>the</strong> is-part relation (included by Amsler<br />

(1981) in his tangled hierarchies). Concerning this problem, Nakamura and Nagao<br />

(1988) provide a list <strong>of</strong> function nouns that appear in <strong>the</strong> beginning <strong>of</strong> dictionary<br />

definitions, and <strong>the</strong> relations <strong>the</strong>y are usually associated with:<br />

• kind, type → is-a<br />

• part, side, top → part-<strong>of</strong><br />

• set, member, group, class, family → membership<br />

• act, way, action → action<br />

• state, condition → state<br />

• amount, sum, measure → amount<br />

• degree, quality → degree<br />

• form, shape → form<br />

Ano<strong>the</strong>r typical issue is <strong>the</strong> disambiguation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> genus, which consists<br />

on matching words that appear in <strong>the</strong> definition with <strong>the</strong>ir correct sense in <strong>the</strong><br />

dictionary. In Amsler (1981) and Chodorow et al. (1985), this task requires human<br />

intervention. Some years later, Bruce and Guthrie (1992) worked on an automatic<br />

procedure to accomplish genus disambiguation. First, <strong>the</strong>y identify <strong>the</strong> genus <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

definition. Then, <strong>the</strong>y exploit category markups (e.g. plant, solid) and frequency<br />

information to disambiguate <strong>the</strong> genus with 80% accuracy.<br />

More recently, Navigli (2009a) presented an algorithm to disambiguate words in<br />

dictionary definitions. Their approach is based on <strong>the</strong> exploitation <strong>of</strong> circularity in<br />

dictionaries.<br />

Electronic dictionaries are certainly an important source <strong>of</strong> lexical-semantic<br />

knowledge, but <strong>the</strong>ir organisation does not favour <strong>the</strong>ir direct use as NLP tools,<br />

since <strong>the</strong>y were made to be read by humans. Wilks et al. (1988) mention several

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!