15.01.2015 Views

Journal of Research in Innovative Teaching - National University

Journal of Research in Innovative Teaching - National University

Journal of Research in Innovative Teaching - National University

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Memory retention is related to the spac<strong>in</strong>g effect discussed above because, if the study<br />

sessions are spaced too far apart, memory retention will be poor and the spac<strong>in</strong>g effect will lose<br />

its potency. Rohrer and Pashler (2007) argue that the spac<strong>in</strong>g effect depends jo<strong>in</strong>tly on two<br />

<strong>in</strong>tervals: the <strong>in</strong>terval between study sessions (the <strong>in</strong>tersession <strong>in</strong>terval, ISI) and the <strong>in</strong>terval<br />

between study sessions and the test (the retention <strong>in</strong>terval, RI). These researchers suggest that the<br />

optimal ISI for the spac<strong>in</strong>g effect is roughly 10–30 percent <strong>of</strong> the RI. In other words, if the<br />

<strong>in</strong>terval between study and test (the RI) is ten days (not uncommon for accelerated classes), the<br />

ISIs should be spaced somewhere between one and three days apart. If the retention <strong>in</strong>terval is<br />

one month (common for semester classes), the optimal ISI would be between three and n<strong>in</strong>e<br />

days. Rohrer and Pashler (2007) reported that mathematics students <strong>in</strong> traditional classes who<br />

spaced practice problems across a one-week period performed better on a subsequent test than<br />

math students who massed the practice problems <strong>in</strong>to one session. If their formulation is correct,<br />

we should see the same improvement with math students tak<strong>in</strong>g accelerated classes and spac<strong>in</strong>g<br />

their practice over one or two days rather than a week.<br />

Proactive and Retroactive Interference<br />

Classical verbal learn<strong>in</strong>g theory proposed two types <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>terference—proactive and retroactive<br />

(Underwood, 1957). Proactive <strong>in</strong>terference occurs when prior learned material <strong>in</strong>terferes with<br />

and creates forgett<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> the current material under study. Retroactive <strong>in</strong>terference refers to<br />

subsequent learn<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>in</strong>terferes with and causes forgett<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> the to-be-remembered material.<br />

Just how <strong>in</strong>terference theory relates to the accelerated versus traditional education debate is<br />

difficult to p<strong>in</strong> down. On the one hand, <strong>in</strong>terference theory might suggest that it is not a good<br />

idea to take shorter, accelerated courses <strong>in</strong> a sequential fashion (as done, for example, <strong>in</strong> some<br />

nontraditional colleges and universities) because earlier and later classes might <strong>in</strong>terfere with<br />

learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> the current class. On the other hand, tak<strong>in</strong>g courses concurrently over an extended<br />

time period (as is done <strong>in</strong> more traditional semester sessions) might create proactive and<br />

retroactive effects when students try to study one subject but experience <strong>in</strong>terference from<br />

material learned <strong>in</strong> the other classes. What little evidence exists seems to <strong>in</strong>dicate that whether<br />

classes are taken <strong>in</strong> sequence or concurrently does not effect learn<strong>in</strong>g performance (Boddy, 1985;<br />

Tatum & Parker, 2007).<br />

Multiple Test<strong>in</strong>g Effects<br />

If students are tested regularly (e.g., once per week), they tend to study more and space their<br />

study<strong>in</strong>g more evenly dur<strong>in</strong>g the class (Bangert-Drowns, Kulik, & Kulik, 1991; Leem<strong>in</strong>g, 2002).<br />

Moreover, Roediger and Karpicke (2006a, 2006b) have shown that test<strong>in</strong>g alone, <strong>in</strong>dependent <strong>of</strong><br />

the effect <strong>of</strong> study<strong>in</strong>g, can have a positive impact on long-term retention <strong>of</strong> the study material.<br />

These facts suggest that the longer, more traditional forms <strong>of</strong> course delivery should produce<br />

better learn<strong>in</strong>g and retention than accelerated classes because more time is available to test<br />

students. As discussed below, the research evidence is equivocal on this po<strong>in</strong>t, with most studies<br />

show<strong>in</strong>g no difference between accelerated and extended learn<strong>in</strong>g on long-term retention.<br />

Although no difference is typically the cases, a handful <strong>of</strong> studies show better retention for the<br />

extended classes, while a few other studies show slightly better retention for accelerated classes.<br />

Focused Attention<br />

Most <strong>of</strong> us have experienced situations <strong>in</strong> which we have become deeply immersed <strong>in</strong> an<br />

activity, los<strong>in</strong>g self-consciousness and feel<strong>in</strong>g highly productive and satisfied afterward.<br />

36

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!