15.01.2015 Views

Journal of Research in Innovative Teaching - National University

Journal of Research in Innovative Teaching - National University

Journal of Research in Innovative Teaching - National University

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

and curricula. Most <strong>of</strong> the studies <strong>in</strong> the literature fall with<strong>in</strong> the category <strong>of</strong> research that<br />

Campbell and Stanley (1963) refer to as “quasi-experimental.” These studies lack the k<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>of</strong><br />

controls, random assignment, and measurement precision that characterize true experiments.<br />

Consequently, the validity <strong>of</strong> the <strong>in</strong>ferences and conclusions drawn from this research is limited.<br />

This is not to say that the research has no value; it just means that we must be very cautious<br />

about what we can say with confidence about the results. Many <strong>of</strong> the threats to validity noted by<br />

Campbell and Stanley (e.g., repeated test<strong>in</strong>g, selection bias, differential loss <strong>of</strong> respondents) are<br />

found <strong>in</strong> the studies review above, and it is beyond the scope <strong>of</strong> this paper to discuss them all.<br />

However, there are some artifacts that are common to many <strong>of</strong> the studies and bear special<br />

attention. In addition, certa<strong>in</strong> measurement issues are relevant and will be addressed.<br />

Different Populations<br />

A frequent problem with the studies <strong>of</strong> accelerated education is that the <strong>in</strong>dividuals engaged <strong>in</strong><br />

the accelerated courses or programs come from different populations than the comparison group.<br />

For example, Wlodkowski, Mauld<strong>in</strong>, and Gahn (2001) compared grades <strong>in</strong> accelerated programs<br />

at Regis <strong>University</strong> with grades for similar programs at the <strong>University</strong> <strong>of</strong> Missouri at Kansas City<br />

run under a semester system. Obviously, the population <strong>of</strong> students was quite different, and the<br />

superior grades for students <strong>in</strong> the accelerated classes may have been due to differences <strong>in</strong> the<br />

type <strong>of</strong> student rather than the pace <strong>of</strong> the class.<br />

Selection Bias<br />

Somewhat related to population differences is selection bias. Even with<strong>in</strong> the same population,<br />

different types <strong>of</strong> students, when given the choice, may gravitate to either accelerated classes or<br />

traditional classes. Wlodkowski, Iturralde-Albert, and Mauld<strong>in</strong> (2000) compared four<br />

undergraduate courses (economics, history, human relations, and labor relations) taught <strong>in</strong> their<br />

accelerated program (5 weeks) with the same courses taught <strong>in</strong> their traditional program (16<br />

weeks). Despite the fact that the courses rema<strong>in</strong>ed the same, the differences <strong>in</strong> performance may<br />

be due to differences <strong>in</strong> the type <strong>of</strong> student who chooses one type <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>struction over another.<br />

Likewise, when Jonas and Weimer (1999) reported that the bus<strong>in</strong>ess students <strong>in</strong> their accelerated<br />

program scored higher on a standardized test than the bus<strong>in</strong>ess students <strong>in</strong> their traditional<br />

program, the results could have been due to a selection bias rather than program differences.<br />

Extraneous Variables<br />

The primary goal <strong>of</strong> much <strong>of</strong> the research <strong>in</strong> this area is to allow causal <strong>in</strong>ferences from the<br />

results (e.g., accelerated classes lead directly to improved learn<strong>in</strong>g). To make an unambiguous<br />

cause-and-effect connection between two variables (e.g., type <strong>of</strong> class causes different learn<strong>in</strong>g<br />

outcomes), all or most <strong>of</strong> the other compet<strong>in</strong>g variables must be controlled with<strong>in</strong> the study<br />

design. Most <strong>of</strong> the studies on accelerated education do not adequately deal with these<br />

“extraneous variables.” Some <strong>of</strong> these variables are an <strong>in</strong>herent part <strong>of</strong> the accelerated approach<br />

(e.g., differences <strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g style), but many others are simply nuisance variables that compete<br />

with the primary causal variables <strong>in</strong> the study (e.g., differences <strong>in</strong> time <strong>of</strong> day, <strong>in</strong>structors,<br />

textbooks, type <strong>of</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g, class size). Geltner and Logan (2001) is an example <strong>of</strong> a study that<br />

<strong>in</strong>volved many extraneous variables. These researchers demonstrated several advantages <strong>of</strong> 6-<br />

week classes over 8-week and 16-week classes, but there were many differences between these<br />

classes besides their length (e.g., time <strong>of</strong> day, academic discipl<strong>in</strong>e, student age, cumulative GPA<br />

<strong>of</strong> students, faculty status). To elim<strong>in</strong>ate these extraneous variables, researchers need to control<br />

44

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!