28.11.2012 Views

Beer : Health and Nutrition

Beer : Health and Nutrition

Beer : Health and Nutrition

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

22 Chapter One<br />

well return to a style of moderate consumption (Clark & Cahalan 1976). The reader<br />

is referred to the autobiographical confessions of Jack London in John Barleycorn<br />

(1913) for a literary example of this. As Schuckit (1984) observes, in any given month<br />

half of all alcoholics will be abstinent, with an average of four months being ‘dry’ in a<br />

1- to 2-year period. Keller (1972) points out that virtually all of the alcoholics that he<br />

had encountered said that they could frequently take just 1 to 3 drinks for a period of<br />

weeks without any episodes of being unable to stop. Keller observed that if there had<br />

been an unavoidable slide towards uncontrolled drinking as a result of simply taking<br />

one drink, then that would not explain why an alcoholic would lack the self-control<br />

simply to avoid taking that rst drink. In other words, the lack of self-control exists<br />

before the drink is taken.<br />

Several studies have presented powerful evidence that heavy drinkers do possess selfcontrol.<br />

Mello <strong>and</strong> Mendelson (1972) (see also Heather & Robertson 1981) performed<br />

an experiment whereby heavy consumers of bourbon were allowed to earn ounces of<br />

bourbon in periods of between 5 <strong>and</strong> 15 minutes in response to their ability <strong>and</strong> preparedness<br />

to partake of simple tasks involving pushing a button according to instructions.<br />

Under conditions where they could certainly earn enough bourbon to become intoxicated,<br />

none of the subjects attempted to drink to gross excess. In fact they drank to maintain<br />

high but approximately constant blood alcohol levels, in spontaneously initiated <strong>and</strong><br />

terminated sessions over a prolonged period as opposed to continuously. It was also<br />

concluded that the amount of alcohol consumed was related to the effort that needed to<br />

be exerted to get it – there was a bene t versus cost balance, which ies in the face of<br />

the lack of control supposition associated with alcoholism.<br />

In another study it was shown that, when given the choice of more liquor or the ability<br />

to remain in a pleasant social environment, alcoholics mostly retrained themselves to<br />

moderate drinking (Cohen et al. 1971). Pattison et al. (1977), in a review of more than<br />

50 clinical studies, drew the conclusion:<br />

Within a hospital or laboratory environment the drinking of chronic alcoholics is<br />

explicitly a function of environmental contingencies.<br />

This must mean either that there is something about non-controlled environments that<br />

impacts on drinking behaviours or that properly controlled experiments <strong>and</strong> observations<br />

made out of a clinical or laboratory setting have not been made. If the former is<br />

the case, coupled with the observations made on individuals’ drinking habits in relation<br />

to reward, then this argues for the importance of a range of other motivations for heavy<br />

drinking that are not chemical based.<br />

Indeed, a compelling study by Marlatt et al. (1973) showed that alcoholics consume<br />

beverages in response to what they are directed to believe that those drinks comprise.<br />

Thus, if given tonic water alone but told that it contained vodka, the subject consumes

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!