06.09.2021 Views

Torts - Cases, Principles, and Institutions Fifth Edition, 2016a

Torts - Cases, Principles, and Institutions Fifth Edition, 2016a

Torts - Cases, Principles, and Institutions Fifth Edition, 2016a

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Witt & Tani, TCPI 1. Introduction<br />

973 F. Supp. at 548. The Restatement authors agree with Judge Black. The substantial certainty<br />

doctrine, the Restatement asserts, should be limited to cases in which “the defendant has<br />

knowledge to a substantial certainty that the conduct will bring about harm to a particular victim<br />

or to someone within a small class of potential victims within a localized area.” RESTATEMENT<br />

(THIRD) OF TORTS: PHYS. & EMOT. HARM § 1 cmt. e (2010) (emphasis added). Why limit the tort<br />

of battery in this way?<br />

3. Is intentionality intuitive? The famed Supreme Court justice Oliver Wendell Holmes, Jr.,<br />

once claimed that “even a dog knows the difference between being kicked <strong>and</strong> being stumbled<br />

over.” OLIVER WENDELL HOLMES, JR., THE COMMON LAW 3 (1881). Yet research from<br />

developmental psychology paints a more complicated picture.<br />

It is true that the ability to distinguish intentional from unintentional action is a<br />

foundational skill that emerges early in development. Chimpanzees, orangutans, <strong>and</strong> eightmonth-old<br />

human infants can reliably discriminate between purposeful <strong>and</strong> accidental action.<br />

Josep Call & Michael Tomasello, Distinguishing Intentional from Accidental Actions in<br />

Orangutans (Pongo Pygmaeus) <strong>and</strong> Chimpanzees (Pan Troglodytes) <strong>and</strong> Human Children, 122 J.<br />

COMP. PSYCHOL. 192 (1998). This faculty plays an essential role in helping children acquire<br />

language <strong>and</strong> develop interpersonal skills. In fact, preschoolers whose ability to infer others’<br />

intentions is disrupted, often due to maltreatment or physical abuse, are at heightened risk for later<br />

social maladjustment <strong>and</strong> behavioral problems. See Kenneth A. Dodge, John E. Bates & Gregory<br />

S. Petit, Mechanisms in the Cycle of Violence, 250 SCIENCE 1678 (1990). Indeed, some have<br />

argued that our ability to identify unintentional harms as accidents <strong>and</strong> to respond with<br />

forgiveness is what allows for the evolution of cooperative societies in which individuals<br />

coordinate their behavior to maximize collective welfare. E.g., Drew Fudenberg, David G. R<strong>and</strong><br />

& Anna Dreber, Slow to Anger <strong>and</strong> Fast to Forgive: Cooperation in an Uncertain World, 102<br />

AM. ECON. REV. 720 (2012).<br />

Yet while the distinction between intentional <strong>and</strong> accidental behavior is intuitive for apes<br />

<strong>and</strong> babies alike, we do not always use this distinction when forming moral judgments. For<br />

instance, the celebrated developmental psychologist Jean Piaget observed in 1932 that young<br />

children consider it morally worse (“naughtier”) for a child to make a large ink stain accidentally<br />

than to make a small stain intentionally. Older children, he noticed, felt just the opposite. Since<br />

Piaget’s discovery, this developmental trajectory has been extensively documented. Preschoolers<br />

tend to judge the moral valence of an act by focusing on its outcome <strong>and</strong> largely ignoring the<br />

agent’s beliefs <strong>and</strong> intentions. By around age 6, children begin to condemn actions on the basis of<br />

the agent’s mental states such as intent to harm. In other words, children shift from outcomebased<br />

moral judgment to intent-based moral judgment as they age. Fiery Cushman, Rachel<br />

Sheketoff, Sophie Wharton & Susan Carey, The Development of Intent-Based Moral Judgment,<br />

127 COGNITION 6 (2013).<br />

Under some circumstances, adults will also exhibit a tendency to focus on outcomes rather<br />

than intentions when judging actions that result in harm. Research from cognitive psychology<br />

demonstrates that adults who are placed under time pressure <strong>and</strong> asked to make hurried moral<br />

judgments show a systematic bias toward judging actions as intentional. In their haste, these<br />

decision-makers will say that harms were intentional. Given more time to make their decisions,<br />

they revert to saying that harms were unintentional. Evelyn Rosset, It’s No Accident: Our Bias<br />

23

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!